Chapter 13 Study Questions Flashcards
Define logical positivism and describe its historical origins, including the positions of Comte, Mach, and the Vienna Circle
Logical positivism was a philosophical movement that emerged in the early 20th century. It aimed to reconcile empiricism with a sophisticated understanding of scientific theory.
Auguste Comte, the founder of positivism, emphasized the importance of empirical observation and the scientific method. He believed that knowledge should be based on empirical evidence and that metaphysical speculation should be avoided.
Ernst Mach, building on Comte’s ideas, argued that all we can be certain of is our sensations. He asserted that sensations are the ultimate subject matter for any science and that understanding the correlations between sensations enables better adaptation to the environment.
The Vienna Circle, a group of philosophers and scientists in Vienna in the 1920s, further developed logical positivism. They combined the earlier positivist ideas of Comte and Mach with the rigorous methods of formal logic.
What are observational terms and theoretical terms?
Observational terms are those that refer to directly observable events or objects. These terms are grounded in empirical evidence and can be measured or observed objectively. Examples include terms like “force,” “energy,” or “mass.” Theoretical terms, on the other hand, are concepts that attempt to explain what is observed. These terms often go beyond direct observation and refer to entities or processes that cannot be directly perceived. Examples include theoretical concepts like “gravity,” “magnetism,” or “atom.”
Define an operational definition and operationism
An operational definition is a clear, precise, and detailed definition of a variable that allows it to be measured and observed objectively. It defines a concept in terms of the specific procedures or operations used to measure or manipulate it. This approach makes theoretical terms concrete and testable, anchoring them in observable phenomena. Operationism, in the context of logical positivism, refers to the insistence that all scientific concepts should be defined in terms of the operations or procedures used to measure them. By operationalizing theoretical terms, they become linked to observable phenomena, thereby reducing ambiguity and metaphysical speculation.
Contrast positivism with logical positivism
Positivism, as introduced by Auguste Comte, emphasized empirical observation and the scientific method. It discouraged metaphysical speculation and focused on concrete, observable phenomena. Logical positivism, while building on these ideas, allowed for the incorporation of theoretical terms in science, provided they could be logically tied to empirical observations. It accepted the use of theory in science without sacrificing objectivity, as long as such theories were rooted in empirical verification.
Define neobehaviourism and describe its historical origins
Neobehaviorism was a psychological approach that emerged from the combination of behaviorism and logical positivism. It emphasized the importance of empirical observation and the operational definition of theoretical terms. Neobehaviorists insisted on the use of nonhuman animals in research, believing that their behavior could be generalized to humans. They prioritized the learning process as the primary mechanism through which organisms adapt to their environment.
The historical origins of neobehaviorism lie in the period from approximately 1930 to 1950, during which logical positivism had a significant influence on the field of psychology. Despite the dominance of neobehaviorism during this period, some psychologists were drawn to psychoanalysis and Gestalt psychology, which stood in contrast to the principles of logical positivism and neobehaviorism.
Briefly describe the life and work of Edwin Ray Guthrie (1886–1959)
Edwin Ray Guthrie (1886–1959) was an influential American psychologist known for his work in the field of learning theory. His life was marked by an academic inclination from an early age. After obtaining his MA in philosophy from the University of Nebraska and later a PhD from the University of Pennsylvania, he taught at various educational institutions before finally settling at the University of Washington, where he made significant contributions to the field of psychology
Describe Guthrie’s single law of learning
Guthrie’s single law of learning, the law of contiguity, posited that a combination of stimuli that accompanied a movement would tend to be followed by that same movement when the situation recurred. This principle emphasized the significance of associations between stimuli and responses, disregarding the notions of confirmatory waves or reinforcement
Describe Guthrie’s concept of one-trial learning
Guthrie departed from traditional views on learning by introducing the idea of one-trial learning. Unlike his predecessors who believed in the law of frequency, Guthrie postulated that a stimulus pattern gains its full associative strength upon its first pairing with a response. This concept challenged the notion that repeated exposure was necessary for learning to occur
Describe Guthrie’s distinctions between movements, acts, and skills, and his explanation of why practice improves performance
Guthrie made distinctions between movements, acts, and skills. He described movements as specific responses to specific stimulus configurations, while acts were responses to varying stimulus configurations. Skills were then defined as a combination of multiple acts. According to Guthrie, practice improves performance because learning a movement occurs in a single trial, but mastering an act or a skill necessitates the learning of numerous stimulus-response associations
Describe Guthrie’s concept of the nature of reinforcement
Guthrie rejected the traditional view of reinforcement put forth by Thorndike, arguing that reinforcement merely changes the stimulating conditions, preserving the association that preceded it. According to his recency principle, the last response made in a situation will tend to be repeated in similar situations
Describe Guthrie’s view of the forgetting process
Guthrie viewed forgetting as an active process, wherein old stimulus-response associations are displaced by new ones. He emphasized that forgetting occurs only if an existing association is interfered with, requiring active unlearning rather than passive fading over time
Describe Guthrie’s notion of habit and how to break habits
Guthrie defined habits as acts associated with a multitude of stimuli. To break undesirable habits, he recommended replacing the undesirable act with a different, desirable act in the presence of the same stimuli. By doing so, the new act would become associated with the stimuli, eventually superseding the old habit
Describe Guthrie’s concept of punishment
Guthrie believed that the effectiveness of punishment lay in its ability to elicit behavior incompatible with the undesirable behavior in the presence of specific stimuli. If punishment could provoke a response incongruent with the undesirable behavior in specific circumstances, it would likely be effective
Describe Guthrie’s attempts of Voeks and Estes to formalize Guthrie’s theory
Virginia W. Voeks and William Kaye Estes attempted to formalize Guthrie’s theory. Voeks, a student of Guthrie, developed a more structured version of Guthrie’s ideas, creating postulates, definitions, and theorems. Meanwhile, Estes built on Guthrie’s one-trial learning concept, developing stimulus sampling theory, which provided a more comprehensive understanding of the complexity inherent in Guthrie’s seemingly simple model. Though Estes later modified his theory to align with cognitive psychology, the core concept of Guthrie’s law of contiguity remained central to his revised model
Briefly describe the life and work of Clark Leonard Hull (1884–1952)
Clark Leonard Hull, born in 1884 near Akron, New York, overcame several physical challenges and financial constraints to become one of the most influential psychologists of the 20th century. Initially aspiring to become a Unitarian minister, he eventually found his passion in psychology, driven by a desire to work with apparatus and to engage in theoretical work. His strong interest in developing machines that could simulate human thinking aligned with his fascination with Pavlov’s work on conditioned responses, and he sought to apply this knowledge to the study of learning processes in both humans and animals.