Short Answer Chp 10-11 Flashcards
Define prosocial behaviour and give an example of a prosocial behaviour
Actions intended to benefit others
Ex. donating money, giving rides to a friend, babysitting for a friend, volunteering, picking up mail for someone who is out of town
How does evolutionary theory explain helping behaviours among genetically-related relatvies, non-kin, and members of an in-group?
Evolutionary perspectives emphasize the survival of the individual’s genes.
The selfish Gene: survival of the individual’s genes. If a specific social behaviour enhances reproductive success, the genetic underpinnings of that behaviour are more likely to be passed on to subsequent generations.
Kin selection: preferential helping of genetic relatives so that genes held in common will survive
- The purpose of helping is to promote the survival of those who share your genetic makeup.
- kinship selection promotes genetic survival
- preferential helping of genetic relatives should be strongest when the biological stakes are par- ticularly high.
Non-kin helping behaviours
a) Reciprocal altruism: helping someone else can be in your best interests because it increases the likelihood that you will be helped in return.
In-group helping behaviours
a) Altruistic groups: groups with altruistic members may be more likely to thrive and avoid extinction than groups with only selfish individuals. Cooperation and helpfulness for the good of the group (especially when the group faces an external threat) could be an innate, universal tendency.
Define reciprocal altruism
helping someone else can be in your best interests because it increases the likelihood that you will be helped in return.
Explain the roles that costs and rewards play in helping others
Arousal: Cost-reward model: The proposition that people react to emergency situations by acting in the most cost-effective way to reduce the arousal of shock and alarm. Both the emotional and cognitive factors determine whether bystanders to an emergency will intervene.
Feeling Good: helping often simply feels good. Giving help increases feeling better. Helping improves psychological well-being. Children learn that helping others can be rewarding
Negative State Relief model: the proposition that people help others in order to counteract their own feelings of sadness. People who are feeling bad may be inclined to help others in order to improve their mood.
Being good: many of use are motivated to BE good. We want to help because it’s the right thing to do.
The costs of helping: courageous resistance. When help involves constant and exhausting demands, the effects on the helper’s physical and mental health can be negative (ex. caring long term for an elderly individual).
Describe Arousal: Cost-reward model
The proposition that people react to emergency situations by acting in the most cost-effective way to reduce the arousal of shock and alarm. Both the emotional and cognitive factors determine whether bystanders to an emergency will intervene.
Describe Negative State Relief model
the proposition that people help others in order to counteract their own feelings of sadness. People who are feeling bad may be inclined to help others in order to improve their mood.
Egotistical
Define altruistic motive
motivation by the desire to improve another’s welfare
Define egoistic motive
motivation by the desire to improve one’s own welfare. Selfish concerns or simple conformity to socialized norms.
Define empathy
understanding or vicariously experiencing another individual’s perspective and feeling sympathy and compassion for that individual
Describe the empathy-altruism hypothesis
the proposition that empathic concern for a person in need produces an altruistic motive for helping. Taking the perspective of a person in need creates feelings of empathic concern, which produces the altruistic motive to reduce the other person’s distress.
Describe an alternative explanation for the empathy-altruism hypothesis that has been proposed. Outline the research evidence that supports this hypothesis and the research evidence that does not.
There is increasing evidence that the brain is hardwired for social connection. A fascinating study suggests that human infants are capable of at least a rudimentary degree of perspective taking leading to helping behaviour. Study suggests infants are capable of a rudimentary degree of perspective taking leading to helping behaviour. 18 month old infants helping experimenter with a task to retrieve a marker. The experimenter did not ask for help, nor praise or reward the infant.
Limits: Not all helping is altruistically motivated, there are multiple motives to helping, many result in a mix of self-oriented and other-oriented motivators.
Another limit is that motives do not guarantee behaviour. Empathy leads to altruistic motivations, but not necessarily to helpful behaviour. Someone with an empathetic concern for another may not help if they judge that the potential cost of offering help is too high under the circumstances.Also, some people may tend to be more or less altruistic than others.
Another limit: Omoto and Snyder determined that volunteer will stay longer with egoistic goals than through altruistic goals. Self-interest is not necessarily a bad motivator. One’s feelings of empathic concern for others are usually limited to a few other people at a time and in relatively brief periods.
Define the bystander effect
the tendency for any given bystander to be less likely to give aid if other bystanders are present.
the effect whereby the presence of others inhibits helping.
List the steps that bystanders in an emergency go through, as proposed by Latane and Darley. In order for the victim to receive help, identify the obstacles that bystanders in a group have to overcome in each step of the process.
Noticing: Noticing that something is happening. Bystanders in this step need to overcome distractions, stimulus overload, and self concerns. For example, being late to a meeting or being distracted by your children.
Interpreting: Bystanders need to interpret the event as an emergency. In this step, bystanders need to overcome ambiguity and pluralistic ignorance. For example, bystanders may misinterpret cries of pain or fear as laughter. Individuals may also believe that other bystanders aren’t reacting because they know somehow that there is not an actual emergency
Taking Responsibility: Taking responsibility for providing help. Bystanders must overcome the diffusion of responsibility. For example, bystanders must not assume that someone else in the vicinity called 911
Decide how to help: Make a decision about how to help. In this step, bystanders need to overcome a lack of competence. For example, individuals may feel that they are not qualified to provide help, but there is usually an alternative option such as calling on others in the area for help or simply calling 911
Provide Help: Actually provide help. In this step, bystanders need to overcome audience inhibition, which is the thought that they will make a bad impression or look silly in front of observers. In addition, bystanders also need to overcome the thought that costs will exceed rewards, for example thinking that someone will sue you because of a mistake. We have things in place like the Good Samaritan Act to
cover situations like this.
Define pluralistic ignorance
the state in which people mistakenly believe that their own thoughts and feelings are different from those of others, even when everyone’s behaviour is the same. Each bystander thinks that other people aren’t acting because they somehow know there isn’t an emergency. Actually, everyone is confused and hesitant, but taking cues from each other’s inaction, each observer concludes that help is not required.
Define diffusion of responsibility
the belief that others will or should take the responsibility for providing assistance to a person in need (someone else will call 911). Usually takes place under conditions of anonymity
Define audience inhibition
reluctance to help for fear of making a bad impression on observers.
How may a person’s good mood affect the likelihood that the person will help others?
Good mood effect: good mood increases helping behaviour. When we’re happy we’re helpful.
■ Desire to maintain one’s good mood. When we are in a good mood, we are motivated to maintain that mood. Helping others makes us feel good, and so it can help maintain a positive mood.
■ Positive expectations about helping. If we have more positive expectations about the rewards of helping, we are more likely to help.
■ Positive thoughts. Positive moods trigger positive thoughts, and if we have posi- tive thoughts about others, we should like them more, which makes us more likely to help them.
■ Positive thoughts and expectations about social activities. Positive moods trigger positive thoughts and expectations about interacting with others and engaging in social activities. These positive thoughts and expectations can pro- mote interacting with others in prosocial ways, including helping them
WHY NOT
■ Costs of helping are high
■ Positive thoughts about other social activities that conflict with helping. Ex. If our good mood makes us want to go out and party with our friends, our motivation to participate in this activity may prevent us from taking the time to notice or take responsibility for helping someone in need
Define the good mood effect
the effect whereby a good mood increases helping behaviour. When we’re happy, we’re helpful.
How may a person’s bad mood affect the likelihood that the person will help others?
When bad moods make us more likely to help:
■ If we take responsibility for what caused our bad mood (“I feel guilty for what I did.”). Greatest determiner for whether or not bad moods lead to helping behaviour.
■ If we focus on other people (“Wow, those people have suffered so much.”)
■ If we are made to think about our personal values that promote helping (“I really shouldn’t act like such a jerk next time; I have to be nicer.”)
When bad moods make us less likely to help:
■ If we blame others for our bad mood (“I feel so angry at that jerk who put me in this situation.”)
■ If we become very self-focused (“I am so depressed.”)
■ If we are made to think about our personal values that do not promote helping (“I have to wise up and start thinking about my own needs more
How may personality, moral reasoning, and family background affect the likelihood that a person will help others?
a) Personality - empathy and advanced moral reasoning contribute to an altruistic personality, meaning those with altruism and empathy are more likely to help.
b) Moral reasoning - people who exhibit internalized and advances levels of moral reasoning are more altruistic than others. Ex. Adhering to moral standards independent of external social controls, and taking into account the needs of others when making decisions about courses of action. On the other hand people whose reasoning is focused on their own needs or on the personal consequences that their actions are likely to have are less engaged in helping behaviour.
c) Family background - positive role models = positive helping behaviour
Define social norms
a general rule of conduct reflecting standards of social approval and disapproval. Rule of conduct established by society.
Define norm of social responsibility
a moral standard emphasizing that people should help those who need assistance. People should help those who need assistance. Creates a sense of duty and obligation to which people respond by giving more help to those in greater need of it.