Motor Planning, Articulation, Phonology Flashcards

1
Q

What is phonology?

A
  • Phonology is the study of how sounds are put together to form words and other linguistic units
  • Determining the language specific phonemes and the rules that describe the changes that take place when these phoneme occur in words
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

It is the smallest linguistic unit that is able to distinguish meaning between words

A

Phonemes

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What is phonetics?

A

Study of speech emphasizing the description and classification of speech sounds according to their production, transmission, and perceptual features

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

What are the three branches of phonetics?

A
  • Articulatory phonetics (speech production)
  • Acoustic phonetics (speech transmission)
  • Auditory phonetics (speech perception)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

These are produced with a relatively open vocal tract. There is no significant constriction of the oral (and pharyngeal) cavities is required, and airstream from vocal folds is relatively unimpeded. These are always voiced

A

Vowels

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

There is significant constriction. Airstream encounters some type of articulatory obstacle. For some consonants, the constriction occurs along the sagittal midline of the vocal tract. It can be voiced or voiceless.

A

Consonants

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What is sonority?

A

Loudness relative to that of other sounds with the same length, stress, and pitch. Vowels have greater sonority (sonorants) than consonants.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What is the sonority scale?

A

Vowels → glides [w, j] → [r] → [l] → nasals [m, n, ŋ] → voiced fricatives → [z, v, ð] → voiceless fricatives [s, f, θ] → voiced stop-plosives [b, d, g] → voiceless stop-plosives [p, t, k]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What has greater sonority? Vowels or consonants?

A

Vowels–also referred to as sonorants.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What are obstruents?

A

Characterized by a complete or narrow constriction between articulators hindering the expiratory airstream.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

The obstruents include the following:

A

Plosives [p, b, t, d, k, g]
Fricatives [f, v, s, z, θ, ð, ʃ, ʒ, h]
Affricates [ʤ, ʧ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

The sonorant consonants include the following:

A

Nasals [m, n, ŋ]
Approximants [l, ɹ, w, j]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

The sonorant consonants are produced with a relatively ___________.

A

Open expiratory passageway

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

A consonant that functions as a syllable nucleus is referred to as

A

Syllabic

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

These serve as the center syllables or as syllable nuclei. Consonant or vowels

A

Vowels

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Vowels are commonly described according to certain parameters (Abercrombie, 1967; Crystal, 2000; Heffner, 1975; Kantner & West, 1960; Shriberg, Kent, McAllister, & Preston, 2019). What are these parameters?

A
  • The height of the tongue relative to the palate (e.g., high vs low vowels)
  • The portion of the tongue involved in the articulation (e.g., front vs back vowels)
  • The degree of roundness of the lips (e.g., rounded and unrounded)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

There are two types of vowels. What are they?

A

Monophthongs
Diphthongs

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Degree of muscular activity

A

Tense (e.g., /i/ you put more effort)
Lax (e.g., /ɪ/ less effort)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Relative closeness of the tongue to the roof of the mouth

A

Close (e.g., [i] is more close]
Open (e.g., [ɪ] is more open]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

What are the front vowels?

A

[i, ɪ, e, ɛ, æ, a]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

This is a high-front vowel, unrounded

A

[i] (e.g., eat)
[ɪ] (e.g., in)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

[ɪ] (e.g., in)

A

A high-front vowel, unrounded

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

A mid-front vowel, unrounded. In General American English, this vowel is typically produced as a diphthong, especially in stressed syllables or when articulated slowly.

A

[e] (e.g., bait)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

A mid-front vowel, unrounded

A

[ɛ] (e.g., elephant]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

A low-front vowel, unrounded

A

[æ] (e.g., bat)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

A low-front vowel, unrounded. In General American English, the use of this vowel depends on the particular regional dialect of the speaker. In the New England dialect of the Northeast, one might often hear it.

A

[a] (e.g., aso)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

These are the back vowels

A

[u, ʊ, o, ɔ, ɑ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

A high-back vowel, rounded

A

[u, ʊ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

A mid-back vowel. This vowel is typically produced as a diphthong, especially in stressed syllables or when articulated slowly.

A

[o] (e.g., boat)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

A low mid-back vowel, rounded. The use of this vowel depends on regional pronunciation.

A

[ɔ] (e.g., bought)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

A low-back vowel, unrounded. There seems to be some confusion in transcribing [ɔ] and [a], although acoustic differences certainly exist. One distinguishing feature is that [ɔ] shows some degree of lip rounding, whereas [a] does not.

A

[ɑ] (e.g., bot]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

What are the central vowels

A

[ɝ, ɚ, ɜ, ʌ, ə]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

A central vowel, rounded, with r-coloring.

A

[ɝ, ɚ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
34
Q

A central vowel, unrounded. It is a stressed vowel.

A

[ʌ] (e.g., but)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
35
Q

A central vowel, unrounded. It is an unstressed vowel.

A

[ə] (e.g., buh)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
36
Q

These are vowels in which there is a change in quality during their production–a vowel sound that demonstrates articulatory movement resulting in a qualitative change during its production.

A

Diphthong

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
37
Q

The initial segment, the beginning portion of such a diphthong, is phonetically referred to as ___________. More prominent or longer one.

A

Onglide

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
38
Q

End portion of diphthong is called

A

Offglide

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
39
Q

What is a rising diphthong?

A

Characterized by a smooth transition from the initial vowel to the final vowel, creating a rising pitch or tone. This is very common in General American English. This means that when producing these diphthongs, essential portions of the tongue move from a lower onglide to a higher offglide position; thus, relative to the palate, the tongue moves in a rising motion. Example: [aɪ (e.g., bite), aʊ, (e.g., house), eɪ (e.g., bait), oʊ (e.g., boat), ɔɪ (e.g., boy)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
40
Q

What are centering diphthongs?

A

These diphthongs have the same characteristics as the ones previously noted: a louder and typically longer onglide and a less intense and shorter offglide. However, in these diphthongs, the offglide, or less prominent element of the diphthong, is a central vowel, typically [ə] or [ɚ].

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
41
Q

Refers to diphthongs that are paired with [ɚ]

A

Rhotic diphthongs

Rhotic vowels are [ɝ] as in “bird” and [ɚ] as in “father” whereas rhotic consonants are [ɹ] as in “rabbit.” The word “farm,” [faɚm], contains a rhotic diphthong with the rhotic vowel [ɚ] as offglide.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
42
Q

Centering diphthongs may contain either [ə] or [ɚ] as offglides, where as rhotic diphthongs are limited to the r-vowel [ɚ] as an offglide. True or False.

A

t

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
43
Q

What is a nonphonemic diphthong?

A

Nonphonemic diphthongs are those that the meaning of the word would not change if the vowel were to be pronounced as a monophthong [e] vs a diphthong [ei]. Therefore, no change in meaning would result if just the onglide were realized. Words pronounced [beɪ]k] or [bek], for example, would be recognized as the same word.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
44
Q

What are the nonphonemic diphthongs?

A

[eɪ] [oʊ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
45
Q

What are phonemic diphthongs?

A

Phonemic diphthongs are those that the meaning of the word would change if only the vowel onglide were produced–that is, if the vowel was realized as a monophthong. A realization of [a] instead of [aɪ] will change the meaning in General American English, as the words sod [sad] versus sighed [saɪd] demonstrate.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
46
Q

Three phonetic categories are used to characterize consonants. What are these?

A

Voicing features
Place of articulation
Manner of articulation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
47
Q

What is the place of articulation?

A

Describes where the constriction or narrowing occurs for the various consonant productions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
48
Q

What are the main stationary places of articulation?

A

The upper lip, upper teeth, palate, and velum are the main stationary places of articulation when describing the consonants of General American English

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
49
Q

Movable portions involved in articulating consonants

A

Bottom lip and tongue

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
50
Q

Refers to the type of narrowing that the articulators produce for the realization of a particular consonant. (Example: Plosives, Affricates, Nasals, etc.,)

A

Manner of articulation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
51
Q

What are stop-plosives?

A
  • Complete occlusion
  • Velum is raised to prevent air leakage to the nasal cavity
  • Expiratory air pressure builds up naturally behind this closure (stop); compression results, which is then suddenly released (plosive)
  • [p] and [b]
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
52
Q

What are fricatives? What are sibilants?

A
  • Result when active and passive articulators approximate each other so closely that the escaping expiratory airstream causes an audible friction
  • Velum is raised
  • [f] and [v]
  • Sibilants: have a sharper sound than others because of the presence of high-frequency components: [s] [z]
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
53
Q

These are pairs of similar sounds (e.g., [p] [b], that differ only in their voicing feature.

A

Cognates

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
54
Q

Are produced with the velum lowered so that air can pass freely through the nasal cavity. However, there is a complete blockage within the oral cavity between the articulators. These sounds have been called nasal stops because of the occlusion of the active and passive articulators and ensuring free air passage through the nasal cavity.

A

Nasal

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
55
Q

First, a complete closure is formed between the articulators, and the velum is raised. Because of these articulatory conditions, expiratory air pressure builds up behind the blockage formed by the articulators-the stop phase, which is considered the first portion of the affricate. Second, the stop is then slowly (in comparison to the plosives) released orally, resulting in the friction portion of the speech sound.

A

Affricates

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
56
Q

Constriction is not as narrow. Gliding movement from a relatively constricted into a more open position. These are also considered as approximants.

A

Glides

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
57
Q

What are laterals? Liquids are composed of?

A
  • Midline closure but lateral openings within the oral cavity
  • Expiratory airstream can pass around one or both sides of the tongue
  • [l] (lateral approximant)
  • Liquids: laterals + rhotics
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
58
Q

At least two types of production: retroflexed and bunched. Positioning of the tongue for individual speakers is highly variable.

A

Rhotic

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
59
Q

This is the presence or absence of simultaneous vibration of the vocal cords, resulting in voiced or voiceless consonants

A

Voicing

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
60
Q

What are allophone?

A

includes varieties used in various contexts and by different speakers. Variations in phoneme realizations that do not change the meaning of a word.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
61
Q

Voiceless bilabial plosive

A

[p]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
62
Q

Voiced bilabial plosive

A

[b]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
63
Q

Voiceless alveolar plosive

A

[t]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
64
Q

Voiced alveolar plosive

A

[d]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
65
Q

Voiceless velar plosive

A

[k]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
66
Q

Voiced velar plosive

A

[g]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
67
Q

Voiceless labiodental fricative

A

[f]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
68
Q

Voiced labiodental fricative

A

[v]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
69
Q

Voiceless alveolar fricative

A

[s]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
70
Q

Voiced alveolar fricative

A

[z]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
71
Q

Voiceless postalveolar fricative usually with lip rounding

A

[ʃ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
72
Q

Voiced postalveolar fricative typically with lip rounding

A

[ʒ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
73
Q

Voiceless dental fricative

A

[θ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
74
Q

Voiced dental fricative

A

[ð]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
75
Q

Voiced bilabial nasal

A

[m]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
76
Q

Voiced alveolar nasal

A

[n]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
77
Q

Voiced velar nasal

A

[ŋ]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
78
Q

Voiced labio-velar approximant

A

[w]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
79
Q

Voiced palatal approximant

A

[j]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
80
Q

Voiced alveolar lateral approximant

A

[l]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
81
Q

Voiced alveolar approximant

A

[r]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
82
Q

Voiceless glottal fricative

A

[h]

83
Q

Voiceless alveolar plosive portion followed by a voiceless postalveolar fricative portion

A

[ʧ]

84
Q

Voiced alveolar plosive portion followed by a voiced postalveolar fricative portion

A

[ʤ]

85
Q

This is the presence or absence of simultaneous vibration of the vocal cords, resulting in voiced or voiceless consonants

A

Voicing

86
Q

What are allophones?

A

Includes varieties used in various contexts and by different speakers. Variations in phoneme realizations that do not change the meaning of a word. In other words, they are different sounds that are recognized as the same phoneme by speakers of a language.

87
Q

True or False. Open syllables are harder to produce in comparison to close syllable

A

False. Open syllables are easier to produce in comparison to closed syllables.

88
Q

Stressed syllables are easier to produce in comparison to unstressed syllables. Example: Target [t]: taco → today → tomorrow

A

True

89
Q

Singletons are easier to produce compared to consonant clusters

A

True

90
Q

Fewer syllables are easier to produce compared to more syllables

A

True

91
Q

Give an example of an allophone

A

For example, in English, the phoneme /t/ can be pronounced differently depending on its position in a word:
Aspirated [tʰ]: In the word “top,” the /t/ sound is pronounced with a puff of air, known as aspiration.
Unaspirated [t]: In the word “stop,” the /t/ sound is not aspirated.
Flapped [ɾ]: In the word “butter” in American English, the /t/ sound is pronounced as a quick tap or flap of the tongue, which sounds almost like a soft “d.
These different sounds ([tʰ], [t], and [ɾ]) are all allophones of the same phoneme /t/ in English. Despite the differences in pronunciation, they do not create different words or meanings, so they are considered allophones rather than distinct phonemes.

92
Q

Speech sounds, which can also be labeled as _______

A

Phones

93
Q

These represent physical sound realities, they are end products of articulatory motor processes. When talking about a child’s s-production in the context of an articulation test, for example, we refer to the speech sound or ______ production of [s].

A

Phones

94
Q

What is a phoneme?

A

Phoneme is the smallest linguistic unit that is able, when combined with other such units, to distinguish meaning between words (Bauman-Waengler & Garcia, 2020)

95
Q

Give an example of a phoneme and phones

A

For example, [s] indicates that it was a sound someone actually pronounced in a specific manner. On the other hand, /s/ signifies the phoneme “s.” which makes up the word “sick”
Phonemes are about sound and how changing a sound can change the meaning of a word.

96
Q

What is coarticulation?

A

This describes the concept that the articulators are continually moving into position for other segments over a stretch of speech.
Coarticulation is when the pronunciation of a sound is influenced by the sounds that come before or after it

97
Q

What is assimilation? Also known as?

A
  • The result of coarticulation is referred to as assimilation.
  • Refers to adaptive articulatory changes by which one speech sound becomes similar, sometimes identical, to a neighboring sound segment.
  • Perfectly natural consequences of normal speech production, aka harmony process
98
Q

What are the types of assimilatory processes?

A
  • Contact/contiguous vs remote/noncontiguous
  • Progressive/perseverative vs regressive
  • Phonemic assimilation vs phonemic similitude
  • Total vs partial
  • Coalescence
99
Q

What is contact/contiguous vs remote/noncontiguous?

A
  • Contact assimilation happens when two sounds are right next to each other, and one sound changes to become more like the other.
    - (Example: “Input”, the /n/ sound changes to [m] because it’s right next to the /p/ sound, making it easier to say.)
  • Remote assimilation happens when two sounds are not right next to each other (there’s at least one sound in between them), but one still changes to be more like the other.
    - (Example: In “yellow,” some people might say “lellow” because the “l” sound at the end of the word influences the “y” sound at the beginning, even though they aren’t directly next to each other.)
100
Q

What is progressive vs regressive assimilation?

A
  • Progressive assimilation, here a sound influences the sound that comes after it. The first sound makes the next one change

(Example: In “cats,” the “t” sound makes the “s” sound like a “ts” because the “t” influences the following sound.)

  • Regressive assimilation in this case is when a sound influences the sound that came before it. The upcoming sound makes the previous one change

(Example: In “have to,” the “t” sound can make the “v” turn into an “f,” so it sounds like “hafta.” The “t” sound affects the “v” that came before it.)

101
Q

What is total vs partial assimilation?

A
  • Total assimilation is when a sound changes completely to become exactly like the sound it’s influenced by.

(Example: If someone says “cupboard” as “cubboard,” the /p/ changes completely to /b/, making both sounds identical.)

  • Partial assimilation happens when a sound changes a little bit to become more like another sound, but doesn’t become exactly the same.

(Example: In “input,” the /n/ might change to [m] because of the nearby /p/ sound, but it doesn’t become identical to /p/—it just becomes closer in sound.)

102
Q

What is phonemic assimilation vs phonemic similitude?

A
  • Phonemic Assimilation: A sound actually changes to become more like a neighboring sound, potentially altering its phonemic identity (e.g., /n/ changing to [m] in “input”).
  • Phonemic Similitude: Sounds influence each other to become more alike, but without completely changing into another phoneme (e.g., the /d/ in “handbag” subtly becoming more like a /b/ but not fully).
103
Q

What is coalescence?

A

Coalescence is a phonological process where two adjacent sounds (phonemes) merge to form a new, single sound that has characteristics of both original sounds. This new sound often combines elements from both of the original sounds, resulting in a blend. (Example: “don’t you” → “don’tcha”)

104
Q

What are the parts of the syllable?

A

Peak, Onset, and coda

105
Q

What is the peak?

A
  • It is the most prominent, acoustically most intense part of the syllable.
  • The nucleus is the central part of the syllable and is almost always a vowel sound. It is the core of the syllable around which the other parts are arranged. Sometimes, a syllabic consonant (like /l/ or /n/) can serve as the nucleus, especially in unstressed syllables.
106
Q

Consonants that serve as the syllable peak are referred to as

A

Syllabic

107
Q

What is the onset?

A
  • Consists of all the segments prior to the peak
    This is the initial consonant sound or group of consonants that come before the vowel in a syllable

(Example: In the word “cat”, the onset is /k/. In the word “star”, the onset is /st/. However, not all syllables have an onset; for example, the word “eat” starts directly with a vowel, so it has no onset.

108
Q

What is the coda?

A
  • The code is made up of all the sound segments of a syllable following its peak.

The coda is the consonant sound or group of consonants that follow the nucleus in a syllable. Example: In “cat,” the coda is /t/. In “go,” there is no coda, so it’s an open syllable.

109
Q

The segments that comprise the onset are also termed

A

Syllable releasing sounds

110
Q

The segments that comprise the code are termed

A

syllable arresting sounds

111
Q

The peak and coda together are referred to as the ______

A

Rime. Example: In the word “sun,” the onset is “s” and the rime is “un”

112
Q

Syllables that do not contain a coda are called

A
  • Open or unchecked syllable.

Examples of open, unchecked syllables are do [du], glee [gli], or the first syllable of rebound [ri baund].

113
Q

Syllables that do have codas are called

A

Closed or checked syllable

114
Q

Nucleus + coda

A

Rime

115
Q

What is the rime of the word “sheep”?

A

/iːp/. So, in “sheep,” the rime is the combination of the vowel sound /iː/ and the ending consonant /p/.

116
Q

What are phonotactic restraints?

A

In other words, the phoneme use is restricted, and the phonemes are not used in all possible word positions.

  • Constraints are any patterns noted that seem to limit or restrict the productional possibilities of our clients (Blache, 2000).
117
Q

True or False. Phonotactic rules vary in every language. As such, the phonotactics of an individual’s native language may influence their pronunciation patterns in English.

A

t

118
Q

True or False. Open syllables are harder to produce in comparison to close syllable

A
119
Q

What is a distinctive feature theory? Can be marked as?

A

By Roman Jakobson.

Distinctive features are the basic attributes or characteristics of sounds that make them different from each other.

  • These features are binary, meaning they can either be present (marked as +) or absent (marked as -).

Examples: Voicing: Whether the vocal cords vibrate (e.g., /b/ is +voiced, /p/ is -voiced).

120
Q

What is generative phonology?

A

By Noam Chomsky.

  • Generative: Speech sounds are generated by transforming the underlying representation into a surface form using a language-specific rule
  • 2 major concepts:
    - Phonological rules map underlying representations (phoneme) onto surface pronunciations–sound is dependent on the phoneme.
    - Phonological descriptions depend on information from other linguistic levels–apart from phonology, it is also affected by semantics, syntax, and pragmatics.
  • Enabled description of the relationship of children’s productions to adult pronunciation in terms of phonological rules.
121
Q

According to generative phonology, there are 2 levels of sound representations. What are they?

A

Surface form
Deep form

122
Q

What is surface form?

A
  • The phonetic level with sounds (phones, allophones) as central units.
  • This is the “real” way the word sounds when you actually say it. It’s how you hear the word spoken
123
Q

What is the deep form?

A
  • The phonemic level represented by phonemes.
  • The rough draft of a word. Think of it as the “idea” or “blueprint” of how a word should sound, before any changes are made.
124
Q

What are the five features in generative distinctive features?

A
  • Major class
  • Cavity features
  • Manner of articulation features
  • Source features
  • Naturalness and markedness
125
Q

What are the major class features?

A
  • Sonorant - open vocal tract configuration promoting voicing. General american english vowels, glides, nasal, and liquids
  • Consonantal - Sounds produced with a high degree of oral obstruction. Stops, fricatives, affricates, liquids, and nasals.
  • Vocalic - sounds produced with a low degree of oral obstruction (not higher than required for the high vowels [i] and [u]). Vowels and liquids
126
Q

What are cavity features? What are the 9 types?

A

Cavity features help distinguish sounds based on where in the mouth they are produced

● Coronal: Sounds produced with the tip or blade of the tongue raised from its neutral position. Examples: [t], [d], [s], [z], [n], and [l].
● Anterior: Sounds produced in the front of the oral cavity, with the alveolar ridge as the posterior boundary. This includes labial, dental, and alveolar consonants. Examples: [m], [n], [b], [p], [f], [v], [d], and [t].
● Distributed: Sounds characterized by a relatively long constriction along the midline of the vocal tract. Examples: [ʃ], [s], and [z].
● Nasal: Sounds produced with an open nasal passageway, allowing air to flow through the nose. Examples: [m], [n], and [ŋ].
● Lateral: Sounds produced by lowering the sides of the tongue, allowing air to escape around the sides. Example: [l].
● High: Sounds produced with a high tongue position, including both vowels and consonants. Examples: [i], [u], [k], and [ŋ].
● Low: Sounds produced with a low tongue position. This includes vowels like [ɑ] and consonants like [h], [ʔ], and pharyngeal sounds, which are produced with the root of the tongue as the active articulator.
● Back: Sounds produced with a retracted tongue body. This encompasses back vowels and consonants like velar and pharyngeal sounds.
● Round: Sounds articulated with rounded lips. Examples: [u], [w]

127
Q

What is the manner of articulation features?

A

The way that active and passive articulators work together

128
Q

What are the features in the manner of articulation features?

A
  • Continuant - “incessant” sounds produced without hindering the airstream by any blockages within the oral cavity
  • Delayed release - sounds produced with a slow release of total obstruction within the oral cavity
  • Tense - produced with a relatively greater articulatory effort
129
Q

What are source features?

A

Refers to subglottal air pressure, voicing, and stridency

130
Q

What consonants are sonorant sounds?

A

Nasals, Glides, Liquids

[m] [n] [ŋ] [r] [l] [w] [j] [h]

131
Q

What’s naturalness?

A

Naturalness designates two features:
(1) the relative simplicity of a sound production
(2) its high frequency of occurrences. More natural sounds are those that are considered easier to produce and occur in many languages of the world.

132
Q

What’s markedness?

A

Sounds that are relatively more difficulty to produce and are found less frequently in languages [p] vs [ʧ]

133
Q

What is natural phonology?

A

Stampe.

  • Presents language as a natural reflection of the needs, capacities, and world of its users, rather than as a merely conventional institution. Incorporates features of naturalness theories and was specifically designed to explain the normal development of children’s phonological system.
    Disordered phonology is seen as an inability to realize this natural process of goal oriented adaptive change.

It suggests that children have an innate, universal set of phonological processes that simplify adult speech.

134
Q

What are phonological processes?

A

Phonological processes are innate and universal; therefore, all children are born with the capacity to use the same system of processes.

Phonological processes:

● They make speech easier to produce when a child’s motor capacities are limited, allowing them to approximate adult speech sounds.
● They help children organize their phonological systems, paving the way for the development of the specific sound system of their native language.
● They act as a tool for children to continuously refine their speech by reducing the differences between their innate speech patterns and adult pronunciation.

135
Q

________ occurs when substitutions that appeared unordered and random become more organized

A
  • Ordering

A child’s first revisions may appear unordered. To stay with the plosive for fricative example, a child might at first also devoice the voiced plosives of the substitution; thus, ([s] ~ [t] and [z] ~ [t]). Thus, Sue is pronounced [tu], but zoo is also articulated as [tu] . Later, the child might begin to “order” the revisions by voicing initial voiced plosives but still retaining the plosive substitution.

136
Q

_______ occurs when differences between a child’s and an adult’s systems become limited to only specific sounds, sound classes, or sound sequences.

A

Limitation

137
Q

________ refers to the abolishment of one or more phonological processes as children move from the innate speech patterns to the adult patterns.

A

Suppression

138
Q

The articulatory simplification of consonant clusters into a single consonant. Example: [pun] for spoon.

A

Cluster reduction

139
Q

This process is considered a syllable structure process because the syllable structure is “simplified”; that is, the second syllable becomes merely a repetition of the first.

A

Reduplication

140
Q

An unstressed syllable is omitted.

A

Weak syllable deletion

141
Q

A syllable-arresting consonant, a coda, is omitted.

A

Final consonant deletion

142
Q

Sound substitutions in which the place of articulation is more anteri- orly located than the intended sound.

A

Fronting

143
Q

The replacement of a nonlabial sound by a labial one.

A

Labialization

144
Q

The change of non alveolar sounds, mostly interdental and labiodental sounds, into alveolar ones. Example: [sʌm] for thumb.

A

Alveolarization

145
Q

The substitution of stops for fricatives or the omission of the fricative portion of affricates. Example: [tʌn] for sun; [dus] for juice; [bʌn] for sun

A

Stopping

146
Q

The replacement of fricatives by homorganic affricates. Example: [ʧu] for shoe

A

Affrication

147
Q

The production of affricates as homorganic fricatives. Example: [ʃiz] for cheese

A

Deaffrication

148
Q

The replacement of nasals by homorganic plosives. Example: [dud] for noon.

A

Denasalization

149
Q

The replacement of liquids or fricatives by glides. Example: [wɛd] for red; [ju] for shoe

A

Gliding

150
Q

The replacement of syllabic liquids and nasals, foremost [1] [ɚ], and [n], by vowels. Example: [teɪbo] for table; [lædə] for ladder

A

Vowelization

151
Q

The loss of r-coloring in central vowels with r-coloring, [ɝ] and [ɚ]. Examples: [bɛd] for bird; [læda] for ladder

A

Derhotacization

152
Q

The replacement of a voiceless sound by a voiced sound. Example: [du] for two

A

Voicing

153
Q

The replacement of a voiced sound by a voiceless sound. Example: [pit] for beet

A

Devoicing

154
Q

The change of a nonlabial sound into a labial sound under the influence of a neighboring labial sound. Example: [fwɪŋ] for swing

A

Labial assimilation

155
Q

The change of a non velar sound into a velar sound under the influence of a neighboring velar sound. Example: [gag] for dog

A

Velar assimilation

156
Q

The influence of a nasal on a non-nasal sound. Example: [mʌni] for bunny Nasal assimilation

A

Nasal assimilation

157
Q

The influence of a liquid on a non liquid sound. Example: [lɛloʊ] for yellow

A

Liquid

158
Q

What is linear phonology?

A
  • Phonological theories were based on the understanding that all speech segments are arranged in a sequential order. For example: Wow, what a test. [waʊ wʌt ə tɛst]
  • All segments between follow each other in a specific order to convey a particular message.
159
Q

The phonological rules generated apply only to the segmental level (as opposed to the suprasegmental level) and to those changes that occur in the distinctive feature (Dinnsen, 1997). It focuses on?

A

Linear phonology

  • focuses on the sequential arrangement of sound segments in an utterance.
160
Q

This emphasizes the linear, sequential arrangement of sound segments. Each discrete segment of this string of sound elements consists of a bundle of distinctive features. All segments have equal value, and distinctive features are equal, thus no one sound segment has control over other units. A common set of distinctive features is attributable to all sound segments according to a binary + and - system.

A

Linear phonology

161
Q

What is nonlinear phonology?

A
  • Segments are governed by more complex linguistic dimensions.
    Example: The suprasegmental features (stress, intonation, metrical and rhythmic factors).
  • These theories explore the relationships among units of various sizes, specifically the influence of larger linguistic entities on sound segments.
162
Q

Hierarchy of factors is hypothesized to affect segmental units. Rather than a static sequence of segments of equal value (as in linear phonology), a dynamic system of features, ranked one above the other, is proposed. Children have a tendency to delete segments in unstressed syllables.

A

Non-linear phonology

163
Q

What are the syllable stress patterns?

A

Trochee, Iambic, Spondee

164
Q

Two syllable foot with a stressed syllable followed by an unstressed syllable. Pattern is STRESSED-unstressed

A

Trochee

165
Q

Two syllable foot with an unstressed syllable followed by a stressed syllable. Patter is unstressed-STRESSED.

A

Iambic

166
Q

Two syllable foot with equally stressed syllables

A

Spondee

167
Q

What is autosegmental phonology?

A

Proponent: John Goldsmith.
- To account for tone phenomena in languages in which segmental features interact with varying tones. Application: affricates

168
Q
  • Extended a hierarchical based analysis to stress.
  • It is a branch of nonlinear phonology that focuses on the hierarchical structure of stress and intonation patterns in spoken language.
A

Metrical phonology

169
Q

What is feature geometry?

A

Represents a group of theories that have adopted the tiered representation of features used in autosegmental phonology + a number of other hierarchically ordered feature tiers.

Attempts to explain why some features (but not others) are affected by assimilation. Different nodes and features: laryngeal, manner, and place features

170
Q

Underlying representations contain only “unpredictable features”. A predictable feature is one that would be commonly associated with that particular segment or class of sounds.

It suggests that the underlying representations of sounds in our minds do not include all their phonetic features but only those that are unpredictable or distinctive

A

Radical underspecification

171
Q

Constraint based approach, not a rule-governed one. Constraints characterize patterns that are and are not possible within or across languages. Application in evaluation/therapy. Constraints are based on the principles of markedness. It presupposed a universal grammar and states that constraints characterize universal, however, constraints can be violated.

A

Optimality theory

172
Q

Faithfulness constraint and markedness constraints are opposite constraints.

A
173
Q

_________ or sounds are less common or harder to articulate. For instance, voiced stops (like /b/, /d/, /g/) or clusters of consonants (like /str-/ in “street”) are more marked.

A

Marked features

174
Q

_________ sounds are more natural, common, and easier to pronounce. For example, voiceless stops (like /p/, /t/, /k/) are considered unmarked because they are easier to produce and more common across languages.

A

Unmarked features

175
Q

Explain markedness constraint with faithfulness constraint

A

Basic Form: The word is “cat.”
Faithfulness Constraint: Tries to keep the word “cat” sounding like “cat” when you say it.
Markedness Constraint: Might prefer simpler sounds, so it could make the word change (like making it “bat” to make it easier to say).

176
Q

Under prelinguistic stages, we have 5 stages. What are these?

A
  • Reflexive crying and vegetative sounds
  • Cooing and laughter
  • Vocal play
  • Canonical babbling
  • Jargon
177
Q

Birth to 2 months.

A

Reflexive crying and vegetative sounds

178
Q

___________ includes cries, coughs, grunts, and burps that seem to be automatic responses reflecting the physical state of an infant

A

Reflexive vocalizations

179
Q

___________ may be divided into grunts and sighs associated with activity and clicks and other noises associated with feeding

A

Vegetative sounds

180
Q

2-4 months

A

Cooing and laughter

181
Q

Cooing or gooing sounds are produced during _________

A

Comfortable states

182
Q

Produced as Vowel-like, but also contain brief periods of consonantal elements that are produced at the back of the mouth

A

Cooing and laughter

183
Q

Early comfort sounds have _____________, in other words, they are produced as a syllabic nasal consonant or as a nasalized vowel

A

Quasi-resonant nuclei

184
Q

At this stage, most infants’ primitive vegetative sounds start to disappear and there is also decrease in the frequency of crying

A

12 weeks – Cooing and laughter

185
Q

There is sustained laughter

A

16 weeks

186
Q

4-6 months

A

Vocal play

187
Q

There are extreme variations in loudness and pitch. When compared to those to older children, the transition between segments in this stage is much slower and incomplete. Vowels demonstrate more variation in tongue height and position

A

Vocal play

188
Q

6 months and older

A

Canonical babbling

189
Q

Collective term for reduplicated and non reduplicated babbling stages. Usually begins around 6 months of age.

A

Canonical babbling

190
Q

These are similar strings of consonant-vowel productions. Slight quality variations in the vowel sounds of these strings of babbles, but the consonant will stay the same from syllable to syllable

A

Reduplicated babbling

191
Q

10 months and older

A

Jargon

192
Q

Strings of babbled utterances that are modulated primarily by: intonation, rhythm, pausing. Overlaps with the first meaningful words

A

Jargon

193
Q

Not yet true vowels

A

Vocoids

194
Q

13 to 14 months: predominance of

A

ɛ], [ɪ], [ʌ].

195
Q

These were found to be favored over high and back vocoids

A

Front and central vocoids

196
Q

Not yet true consonants. Predominate in the late babbling stage.

A

Contoids

197
Q

During the later babbling periods, _______ are still the most frequent type

A

Open syllables

198
Q

This is the most common intonation contour for the first year of life

A

Falling pitch

199
Q

1st word

A

12 months

200
Q

Children produce approximately __ meaningful words before the two-word stage

A

50 meaningful words

201
Q

Put two-words together at what age?

A

18 - 24 months

202
Q

Also known as proto-words, phonetically consistent forms. Used in a consistent manner, thereby demonstrating that they seem to have meaning for the children but without a recognizable adult model

A

Invented words

203
Q

When a child can produce approximately 50 words, the child is typically capable of understanding how many words?

A

200 words