Attribution II: recent developments Flashcards
the Gilbert model
see notes
tweaks and revisions to the Gilbert model
Krull (1993); Krull & Erickson (1995) showed this model can also apply to situation perception as well as person perception
flipping the model around
Are we always looking for dispositional (person) explanations as a default?
Maybe studies are set up so that is what perceivers do, but maybe in other settings they might focus on situation first instead
Krull proposed that perceivers’ inferential goals might make the model also applicable to situation perception
examples of when we might weight situ more than person
You are thinking of taking a degree course and many people on the course say they did really badly on it (You might ask: “Is the coursework really impossible?” i.e., situation inference)
You are thinking of joining the army (“Why does everyone get up at five in the morning?”)
You are in your first tutorial and no one is answering the tutor’s questions (“Is it the norm to just keep silent in these sessions?)
a mixed model of social inference
see notes
So with this more flexible model, the process CAN go:
from dispositional first => situational correction OR
from situational first => dispositional correction
empirical demo
Replication of the Gilbert “anxious woman” study but comparing two different inferential goals, one dispositional, one situational
results
Replicated Gilbert’s findings for those in the dispositional goal conditions
Opposite pattern found for situational goal conditions
what do we conclude?
A more flexible model can aid our understanding of cultural differences (e.g., collectivist cultures; Markus & Kitayama, 1991) – influences cognitive and emotional factors
It can also help our understanding of individual differences (entity versus incremental theorists; Molden & Dweck, 2006) – whether you think personality fixed or can change over time – fixed more likely to focus on person and incremental focus on situation factors
Social cognitive neuroscience suggest three stages may not always be sequential (Lieberman et al., 2002)
But in spite of tweaks and additional research, three-stage model well-established in “adult” sphere
developmental issues (Haga et al., 2014)
The three stage model developed to explain behaviour of adults
But how well does it map onto developmental issues of person perception?
Why does it matter whether it does or not?
how children develop social inference strategies - the trait stage
Kids as young as four have a pretty good trait vocabulary (Rholes et al., 1990) – make inference
And they know how to apply it when faced with trait-implying behaviour (Liu et al., 2007)
But researchers don’t conclude kids are drawing dispositional inferences unless evidence these tendencies generalise over time – may not mean that they are always going to be like that
what about correcting for the situ?
Evidence that kids of five may not discount certain information even when alternative explanations are possible – (Baldwin & Baldwin, 1970)
They don’t differentiate between different explanations (may see effort and ability as equally important; Folmer et al., 2008)
This ability appears to happen later
This may fit the three stage model…
why?
“…children should be able to categorize and characterise actors/situations (more automatic processes) before they can use additional information to correct their inferences about the actor’s dispositions or the situation (more deliberate processes).” (Haga et al., 2014)
Haga et al. (2014)
Tested hypothesis that only older children would show ability to correct for situational info…
Younger children lack full mastery of more effortful correction process so should be more susceptible to CB
study 1 - the sad child
Kids in kindergarten (M = 5 years 4 months) or second graders (M = 7 years 11 months) or sixth graders (M = 11 years 5 months) or ninth graders (M = 14 years 6 months) or undergrads (M = 21 years)
Watched silent video clip of child (Anna/John)
Told child talking about:
- A time his/her parents got mad and punished him/her (punishment condition)
- A time when they were pleased with him/her and gave him/her a gift (gift condition) – must just be a sad child
Child in video wore sad facial expression
DVs
Dispositional ratings: “What do you think [Anna] is usually like, in her day to day life, when she is at home or at school?” (scales anchored very sad-very happy, or always crying-never cries or never laughs-is always laughing)
Understanding situational constraints (“How would you feel if you were talking about a time when your parents got mad and punished you/were pleased with you and gave you a gift?” – smiley/sad face scale)
results of Haga et al. (2014)
Participants of all age groups (except 5-year-olds) characterised sad-looking child as dispositionally sadder when situation could not account for sad behaviour (gift -0-) – greater correction for the situation
Correction appears to be happening in older but not younger kids
Last 3 ages function quite equivalent
take home points of Haga
Across several studies using different paradigms, goals and situational constraints, similar results obtained…
“…with age children show more and more adherence to the three stage model of social inference.” (Haga et al., 2014)
unanswered questions
But… maybe the tasks were just harder for younger kids (remember, cognitive busyness hampers correction)
Why do these differences emerge? Do older kids have more cognitive resources? Do they extract a rule (discounting) at a certain point?
Does socialization teach them that behaviour is multiply determined?