Module 9 - Emotional Development Flashcards
Discrete Emotions (Dr. Caroll Izzard)
Focused on the idea that emotions are innate and can be differentiated from one another very early on in life
There are a small number of basic emotions:
Interest, Joy, Surprise, Sadness, Anger, Disgust, Contempt, Self-Hostility, Fear, Shame, Shyness, and Guilt.
Continuous Emotions
emotions differ depending on arousal (mild-intense) and valence (pleasant-unpleasant). Emotions can be distinguished by plotting scores on these two dimensions.
Circumplex model (Russell, 1980)
functionalist approach to emotional development
Focused on the evolutionary/functional significance of emotions, not on differentiating them.
Emotions are evolutionarily adaptive because they motivate us to attend to environmental stimuli – motivate us to get away from something that’s bad for us, or motivate us to approach something that we like.
Circumplex model
emotions may be innate, the boundaries between emotions are fuzzy and are separated by differences in valence and arousal
Emotions are characterized by what components?
1) Neural Responses
2) Physiological responses
3) Subjective Feelings
4) Cognitions
5) Desire to take action
6) Expressive behaviour
Neural responses
The firing of brain regions like the amygdala, etc.
Physiological responses
Changes in heart rate, stress hormones, etc.
Subjective feelings
How we traditionally describe emotions - happiness, sadness, etc.
Cognitions
Our thoughts or inner dialogue related to the experience
Desire to take action
Typically either approach or avoidance based
Expressive Behaviour
Facial expressions, gestures, etc (although these may not always be expressed)
Theorists who take a functionalist approach to understanding emotional development propose that emotions:
Promote action toward a goal
Emotional development consists of much more than simply experiencing emotions and consists of:
Emotional expression: the ability to express different emotions.
Emotional recognition: the ability to recognize or become aware of different emotions.
Emotional understanding: the ability to verbally label and comprehend the use of emotion in themselves and others.
Emotion development also consists of emotion regulation
How we can organize basic emotions?
1) Valence (pleasant or unpleasant)
2) Discrete categories
Pleasant emotions
- Early smiles within the first two months of an infant’s life are reflexive (e.g., after they’ve been fed or passed gas).
- Social smiles start at around 2 months old.
-7 months, infants are more likely to smile at their caregivers than they are to smile at strangers.
-24 months, young children enjoy making other people smile and laugh.
Unpleasant emotions
-unpleasant emotions are not well-differentiated during the first few months.
-begin to differentiate by 6 months and are clearly different by age 2. By this point, parents and other people can tell the difference between a tired cry and a hungry cry.
-infants do not always demonstrate an emotion that is congruent to the situation.
Fear
adaptive as it motivates us to move out of a situation that could cause danger.
Stranger wariness
The degree to which a child may experience stranger anxiety is altered by several factors, including the size of the stranger, facial expressions, and the predictability of the situation.
Separation anxiety
Separation anxiety tends to peak at ~13 months old; however, this depends on culture and family norms.
Ex: mothers who live in the United States often only have a very short maternity leave, and so these babies will show less separation anxiety than babies who rarely leave their mothers
What does the Visual Cliff study demonstrate?
That infants use social-referencing to determine how to approach and react to ambiguous stimuli.
Social referencing is a major mechanism through which infants learn about the world around them (including emotions!).
Self-conscious emotions
-Sometimes called complex emotions or social emotions.
-They require one to be able to reflect upon actions in order to consider what others might think about them.
-develop later than basic emotions because children must first understand that they are different from other people.
-Self-conscious emotions start to differentiate around ~2 years old, and include emotions like embarrassment, pride, guilt, and shame.
What are some examples of self conscious emotions
-Pride
-Guilt
-Shame
-Embarrassment
Collectivist cultures
emphasize interpersonal relationships and connections with others
individualist cultures
emphasize the individual and their autonomy
Role of shame in misbehaviour - Collectivist
Parents emphasize shame in child’s misbehaviour. This is because misbehaviour is a reflection not only on the individual, but also on their family and social connections.
Thus, in collectivist cultures, shame is more normative (and is thus less strongly associated with difficulties).
Role of shame in misbehaviour - Individualistic
Parents emphasize mischievousness in child’s misbehaviour.
Shame is less commonly emphasized, as it is related to feeling bad about oneself.
Shame is less normative and is associated with adverse outcomes for children.
When do infants express anger?
Infants clearly express anger by 12 months
anger is not clearly differentiated from other basic emotions like sadness until ~12 months of age.
Emotion regulation involves initiating, inhibiting, or modulating the following:
1) Internal feeling states
2) Emotion-related cognitions
3) Emotion-related physiological processes
4) Emotion-related behaviours
Types of emotion regulation
1) Distraction
2) Rumination
3) Reappraisal
4) Relaxation/arousal control
5) Expressive engagement
6) Expressive suppression
Distraction
a cognitive emotion regulation strategy that develops very early. By ~6 months, young children begin to regulate their emotions using distraction, and can consistently do this by 12 months of age. This is related to their motor development.
Rumination
Rumination is a cognitive emotion regulation strategy that involves focusing attention on your distress, as well as the causes and consequences of your distress.
Reappraisal
a cognitive emotional regulation strategy that involves changing the way you think about an issue, and is generally considered adaptive.
Relaxation/Arousal control
Relaxation is a behavioural emotional regulation strategy that involves focusing on our body’s response to powerful emotions. Getting enough exercise, sleep, or deep breathing can all help with regulating emotions.
Expressive engagement
Expressive engagement is behavioural emotion regulation that involves actively engaging with the emotion. Expressive engagement is generally considered an adaptive emotion regulation strategy as it helps alleviate distress, facilitates understanding of emotions, and promotes social support.
Expressive suppression
Expressive suppression is a behavioural emotion regulation strategy that involves inhibiting or suppressing one’s emotions, which is related to cultural display rules.
relying too much on expressive suppression can be maladaptive.
Emotional regulation
The ability to initiate, inhibit, and modulate emotional experiences. It consists of a variety of behavioural and cognitive strategies.
It is both one thing and many.
How does emotion regulation develop?
Through help from parents, and practice, children learn to select the best emotion regulation strategy to help them in the moment. (caregiver’s, cognitive strategies, strategy selection)
It is a long, slow process that continues into adulthood.
Parents play an important role in helping their child develop their own emotion regulation strategies through effective ________________.
co-regulation
Young children tend to manage their distress through ______________, whereas older children are able to use _________________.
Behavioural strategies, cognitive strategies
What is temperament?
individual differences in emotion, activity level, and attention that are exhibited across contexts and that are present from infancy and thus thought to be genetically based
temperament reflects an infant’s biological tendencies in emotion regulation, activity, and attention. While temperament is thought to be largely genetic, the stability and outcomes associated with temperament depend on the environment.
closely linked to behavioural style and general arousal level
Thomas and Chess postulated that infants could be placed into 3 temperament categories (Historical perspective on operationalizing temperament)
Easy (40%) – these infants are cheerful, easy to distract or soothe when upset, and are quick to establish routines
Difficult (10%) – these infants get upset easily, are hard to settle, and are slow to adjust to new experiences
Slow-to-Warm-Up (15%) – these infants are somewhat difficult at first, but become easier over time
Dr. Mary Rothbart argued that temperament is best captured by 5 key factors (modern perspective on operationalizing temperament)
1) Fearful distress/inhibition – how the infant responds to new situations.
2) Irritable distress – when they get distressed, how distressed do they actually get?
3) Attention span – how long are they able to pay attention to something?
4) Activity level – how active they are.
5) Positive affect – how much positive affect they show.
The stability and outcomes associated with temperament depend on what?
the environment.
What factors influence stability?
-Different Time Frame
-Baseline levels
-parental environments
Time frame (temperament)
Early in childhood, temperament is relatively stable. However, as children gain autonomy and start spending more time with their peers, temperament can change.
Ex: research that examines the stability of temperament from infancy to late childhood finds that only ~30% of children remain very stable in their temperament.
Baseline levels (temperament)
children at the extremes are more likely to remain stable in their temperament.
That is, children who are very easily distressed (or those who are very mellow) are likely to retain that temperament over time.
We also see that the emotional qualities of temperament are more stable than activity levels.
Environment (temperament)
goodness-of-fit describes how well a baby’s temperament fits with the expectations and demands of environment.
parents have to fit their parenting style to the child to a certain extent.
Importantly, the quality of the parent-child relationship is more important for child development than temperament.