Attachment Flashcards
What is developmental psychology
Branch of psychology concerned with progressive behavioural changes that happen across someone’s life span
Reciprocity
When there’s an interaction between an adult and infant that flows both ways (taking it in turns)
Interactional synchrony
When adults and babies respond in time to sustain communication, where the adults and babies mirror each other. Doing the same interactions at the same time
Positive evaluation of caregiver and infant interactions
- Interactional synchrony has been demonstrated in several studies. Meltzoff and Moore (1983) found infants as young as three days old were displaying this behaviour, which seems to suggest that the imitation behaviours are not learned and are innate.
- Murray and Trevarthen (1985) got mothers to interact with their babies over a video monitor. In the next part of the study the babies were played a tape of their mother so she was not responding to them. The babies tried to attract their mother’s attention but when this failed they gave up responding. This shows that babies want their mothers to reciprocate.
- Abravanal and De Yong (1991)observed infant behaviour when interacting with puppet that looked like a human mouth opening and closing. Infants made small responses to this, showing they aren’t just imitating what they see; interactional synchrony is a special social response
Negative evaluation of caregiver and infant interactions
- Babies cannot communicate so psychologists rely on inferences. They can’t be 100% sure wether infants are actually trying to communicate
- Expressions test (tongue sticking out, yawing and smiling) are ones infants frequently make so may not be deliberately imitating what they saw
Pre- attachment ( 0-3 months)
From 6 weeks of age infants become attached to their humans, preferring them to objects and events.
Indiscriminate attachment ( 3-7 months)
Infants begin to discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar people, smiling more at people they know, but still let strangers hold them
Discriminate attachment ( 7 months onwards)
Infants develop a specific attachment to primary attachment figure. Show separation protest (distress shown when primary attachment figures leaves them) and can also display stranger anxiety (distress caused when stranger approaches). But, P.A.F was seen to b associated with person who built the mos quality relationship, and thus not quantity of the attachment.
Multiple attachments (7 months onward)
Soon after development of initial P.A.F, they form ties with people such as father and grandparents. These are secondary attachments. Fear of stranger weakens, but bond with P.A.F remains strongest
Evaluation of stages of attachment (5-)
- The data collected by Shaffer and Emerson (1964) may be unreliable because it was based on mothers’ reports of their infants. Some mothers might have been less sensitive to their infant’s protests and therefore been less likely to report them.
- The sample was biased because it only included infants from a working-class population and thus the findings might not apply to other social groups.
- The sample was also biased because it only included infants from individualist cultures, infants from collectivist cultures could form attachments in a different way.
- The study does not have temporal validity, it was conducted in the 1960s and parental care of children has changed considerably since then. More women go out to work and more men stay at home.
- Stage theories such as this one are inflexible and do not take account of individual differences, some infants might form multiple attachment first, rather than starting with a single attachment.
Strange situation
Methodology used by Ainsworth et al. (1970) to investigate differences in attachments between infants and their caregivers. It was a controlled observation which took place in a room that had been furnished with some toys.
The investigators observed the infants in a series of three-minute episodes;
- mother and baby,
- stranger enters,
- mother leaves,
- mother returns, etc.
They recorded an infant’s proximity seeking, stranger anxiety, separation protest and reunion joy.
Type A - insecure avoidant
20% had attachments that were classified as insecure-avoidant. Babies with this attachment style largely ignore caregiver and play independently. They show no signs of distress when the caregiver is absent (no separation protest) and continue to ignore them when they return (no reunion joy). The baby is distressed when left completely alone but is comforted by the stranger as easily as their caregiver (no stranger anxiety).
Type B - secure attachment
70% of babies were described as this.
These babies happily play with their caregiver and use them as safe base when exploring room. Babies show clear distress and separation protest when caregiver leaves, even when not left alone. They seek immediate reunion (reunion joy).
Type C - insecure resistant
10% of babies were placed here.
Babies who have an insecure-resistant attachment to their caregivers are fussy and cry more than other babies. They will not explore the room or play with the toys very much, instead they are clingy. The baby is distressed when the caregiver leaves (extreme separation protest), however they resist comfort from the caregiver on reunion (no reunion joy). They strongly resist the stranger’s attempts to make contact (extreme stranger anxiety).
Positive evaluation of strange situation
- The method has been replicated many times over the years, as its easy to replicate due to the high controls levels originally exerted with standardised procedures. Carried out successfully in many cultures
Negative evaluation of strange situation (4)
- Methodology was developed in the USA, so may be culturally biased. Eg. Healthy attachments in America may not be the same elsewhere, like Germany. In Germany at this time very few mothers worked (less than 1 in 5) but children were encouraged to be independent and self-reliant. Thus, behaviour exhibited by securely attached infants, such as crying when their mothers leave the room, was seen as being spoilt and so didn’t reward this behaviour.
- Validity may be questioned, as may be argued proximity seeking could be a measure of insecurity rather than security
- The SS is gender biased as only carried out on mother as the caregiver. Children might be insecurely attached to their mothers but securely attached to their fathers. They strange situation is therefore not measuring a child’s overall attachment style but their attachment to one individual. In fact, Main and Weston (1981) found that children behave differently depending on which parent they are with.
- As its artificial, may not reflect their real world behaviour (lacks eco validity). Studies found that babies attachment behaviours are stronger in lab settings than they are in home environment.
Cultural variation study: Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg (1988).
Procedure, Findings and similarities
Procedure:
- meta analysis of 32 studies into attachment across cultures. They all included strange situation to measure attachment. Looked at relationship between mothers-babies under 2 years of age.
- conducted in 8 countries: eg some individualistic (USA, UK, Germany) and collectivist (Japan, Chinese, Isreal)
Findings:
- most common attachment was secure, then insure-avoidant (expect japan and Isreal, resistant moe common)
- china had lowest % of secure attachment and UK highest %
- variations within culture 1,5 times greater than variation between cultures
Similarities: suggests caregiver/infant interactions have universal characteristics. But variations also show cultural differences in child rearing and that plays important role in attachment style. Variations within culture indicate sub culture differences like social class, also play important role in attachment.
Evaluation of cultural variation in attachment (1+ 4-)
- This study is a meta-analysis, which includes a very large sample. This increases the validity of the findings.
- Strange situation method was developed in the USA, and may not be valid in other cultures. Eg, Ainsworth assumed that willingness to explore means a child is securely attached but this may not be the case in other cultures. This means the methodology is culturally biased.
- Infants tested in Israel were from a closed community, and thus hadn’t come into contact with strangers. Could be reason why they showed severe distress when confronted with strangers and thus classes resistant.
- Wasn’t actually comparing cultures, but countries. Eg. The comparison with the USA and Japan. Both countries have many different sub-cultures that have different child rearing practices. One study of attachment in Tokyo found similar attachment style distributions to the USA, whereas studies in more rural areas of Japan found many more insecure-resistant infants.
- All studies in the meta-analysis looked at mother-infant attachment, and thus not a father one. strange situation is therefore not measuring a child’s attachment style but their attachment to one individual.
What does operant conditioning do to attachments and why
They strengthen attachment. The baby could receive + reinforcement for crying when they hungry. Caregiver receives - reinforcement for feeding the baby when they cry as feeding makes crying stop
Positive evaluation of Learning Theory
- The LT is plausible and scientific as its founded in established theory. Likely that association between the person of needs and person providing these needs can lead to strong attachment
Negative evaluation of Learning Theory (4)
- Harlow (1959) separated infant monkeys and put them in cages. Milk was given either by a wires mesh ‘surrogate mother’ or one made from a soft cloth. Monkeys clung to soft cloth ‘mother’, especially when scared by an adverse stimulus, even when not provided milk. Suggests comfort is more important that food when determining who babies attach to.
- Schaffer and Emerson (1964) also found food isn’t necessary for attachments forming. Discovered babies often attach to people who play with them, rather than feed. 39% of cases baby was more attachment to someone else, even though mother had fed them
- Theory explains how attachments form, but not why. According to Bowlby’s theory of attachment infants form attachments to caregivers to ensure protection
- LT is environmentally reductionist as it explains complex human behaviour in simple ways. Infant-caregiver relationship is varied, sophisticated and complicated. Thus, its unlikely attachment is merely the result of caregivers providing infants with food. LT is environmentally deterministic because it states that early learning determines later attachment behaviours
Why do attachments form
The principle of Bowlby’s theory is that attachment between infants and their caregivers is an instinct that has evolved because it increases the chances of both the babies’ survival and the parents’ passing on their genes. It is therefore adaptive.
Why may infants face difficulties in forming attachments in later life
The critical period for attachment is before a child is two years of age. Infant’s who do not have an opportunity to form an attachment during this time will have difficulty forming attachments later on