21 - Word Meaning 1 Flashcards

1
Q

How do infants become more perceptive and making distinctions in their own language?

A

Distributional information (statistical learning)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Why can’t adults find distinctions in other languages?

A

Because they have developed a categorical perception of phonemes through statistical learning

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Does word comprehension or word production come first in infants aged 1-2 years?

A

Comprehension

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Do infants aged 1-2 years have a sharper increase with their comprehension or word production?

A

Word production occurs rapidly towards the end of first year, comprehension is more steady

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

How many words do adults know on average?

A

More than 60,000

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

How many words do children learn on average each day at 12-16 months?

How many at 8-10 years?

A

12-16 months = .3 words per day

8-10 years =- 12.1 words per day

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Adults tend to stop learning words at a certain age, what are the two arguments put forth for why?

A

The critical period argument, we are no longer able

The environment: we know most words in our immediate environment

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What was the Werker et al. (1998) infant word learning experiment (what was the method)?

A

Infant are able to associate words with objects in the lab

8, 12, and 14-months olds trained with novel word-object pairings

Repeated presentations

Tested with habituation

  • Correct word/object pairings given until habituation
  • Incorrect word/ object pairings then presented
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What were the results of the Werker et al. (1998) infant word learning experiment?

A

All the infants looked at the incorrect word/object pairings

When there were 2 word/object pairings only 14-month olds could do this for both

Younger groups could only do this for one or the other

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Difficulty to map onto a label with what is referring to in the world when there are multiple objects present in the field of view is called…

A

Referential Uncertainty

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

It is hard to determine whether infants understand what we think they understand because of

A

Referential Uncertainty

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Infants will often hear unique words in a sentence rather than in isolation, why is this a challenge for learning?

A

Word segmentation and referential uncertainty makes it hard for infants to understand what the word is referring to

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

How do we learn word meanings?

A

Through a combination of statistical learning and a bias towards communication

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

What are the 5 suggested mechanisms for how we learn word meanings in the real world?

A

Fast mapping

Cross-situational word learning

Zipfian distributions

Mutual Exclusivity

Communicative intent

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What is fast mapping?

A

The fact that biologically prone to quickly refer words to objects and remember them well

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Describe the Markson and Bloom (1997) experimental method which they used to show ‘fast mapping’?

A

Experiment with 2 phases

Training phase:

Playing measuring games with 10 objects

3 conditions for 3 objects. Casually mention ONCE ONLY:

“This is a Koba. Let’s put the Koba here.”

“This is from my uncle. Let’s put it here.”

“Let’s put this sticker here [on object].”

Test phase:

ONE MONTH LATER:

10 items, pick out the referenced item

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

What were the results of the Markson and Bloom (1997) fast mapping experiment?

A

Children could learn the item based on its name, but they were even stronger at identifying the social aspect item (that it was given by an uncle). This is not language-specific but social connections is another valuable learning tool.

Sticker was not helpful in remembering, only language and social connection

18
Q

What is ‘cross-situational word learning’?

A

Learning a word by seeing it across different situations

Aggregating information after hearing it be used over multiple scenarios

- e.g. hear “ball and bat” infant is unsure what is ball and what is bat. However, if they later hear “ball and dog” they will be able to aggregate the info the determine which one is the ball

19
Q

What is this an example of?

A bottle of tesguïno is on the table.

Everybody likes tesguïno.

Tesguïno makes you drunk.

We make tesguïno out of corn

A

Cross-situational word learning.

We have learned that Tesguino is an alcoholic drink without it ever being described to us.

20
Q

12-14 month infants have been shown to be able to learn new words through cross-situational word learning.

How was this shown?

A

By pairing artificial words with different shapes

21
Q

What are ‘Zipfian distributions’?

A

Words are not distributed evenly but are skewed.

Some words are very common whereas other words are more rare.

Low-frequency words are hard to learn because they are not heard often

High-frequency words might help

22
Q

How has it been suggested that we use zipfian distributions to learn words?

How was it shown?

A

We use ‘high-frequency words’ to learn ‘low-frequency words’

Adults learned new words at a high or low frequency

They had to match 4 objects to 4 certain words

They were better at matching objects when high-frequency words were included rather than just low-frequency words

I.e. we know two words so we only need to figure out the last two word-object pairings

23
Q

What is ‘mutual exclusivity’?

A

We have a bias that states that one word is equal to one object

(one to one mapping bias)

24
Q

How does ‘mutual exclusivity’ lead to statistical learning?

A

If there are two objects and we are asked (which one is Dax) and we know one is a carrot, we know that the other one must be Dax

25
Q

What is the ‘one-to-one mapping bias’?

A

That we believe that each name refers to only one object

26
Q

Are object-word relationships mutually exclusive?

A

No, there are multiple different words we can use to portray an object

27
Q

What have experiments shown in regards to our performance when 1-to-1 mapping is available?

A

We are better at doing 1-to-1 mapping

We can do it without 1-to-1 mapping, but it is harder

This means that it is important to our learning but it is not a hard and fast rule

28
Q

What is ‘communicative intent’?

A

Infants and adults are sensitive to communication and pick up on social cues. They primarily learn things through watching others or in reference to others.

29
Q

What are three experimental results that showed ‘communicative intent’?

A

Only learn labels if the speaker is confident

(Sabbagh & Baldwin, 2001)

Only learn labels in person, not through a voice recording

(Fulkerson and Haaf, 2003)

Only learn labels when the speaker is looking at the object

(Baldwin et al., 1996)

30
Q

What explanation do theorists give for ‘communicative intent’?

A

Evolutionary bias. Children learn through communicative context and form bonds with adults.

They learn what to learn and what to avoid.

31
Q

What was the Frank, Goodman and Tenenbaum (2009) experiment regarding communicative intent?

What were the results?

A

Tracked how often the words a parent said co-occurred with that object being in view

This was done with and without the parents communicative intent (intentionally labelling object)

The best model for each was:

Co-occurence – 11% accuracy

Co-occurrence with intent – 55% accuracy

32
Q

What is generalising word meanings?

A

When words refer to more than one thing (e.g. mammal, fish - larger categories)

33
Q

How did Landau, Smith and Jones (1992) test (methodology) how we generalise word meanings?

A

Tested on both adults and children

The stimuli used were physical objects

They presented the ‘dax’ then other objects when the texture, size, or the shape was different (all other properties were the same).

Tested on whether they believed it ‘is a dax’

34
Q

What were the results of the Landau, Smith and Jones (1992) generalising word meanings experiment?

A

Most adults and children believe that it is still the same word if the size or texture changes

However, if the shape changes less children and very few adults believe it is the same word

This is called shape bias and it becomes more strong over time

35
Q

What is shape bias?

A

The tendency to view things of a different shape as outside of a category

36
Q

What are the three degrees of which we generalise word meanings?

A

Superordinate

Basic

Subordinate

37
Q

What does this experiment on generalising word meanings show us?

A

How we generalise words is sensitive to the context we have learnt them.

  • That if we use one-word, we are likely to generalise to the ‘basic level’ (i.e. might select a different dog if shown a dalmation)*
  • 3 sub examples we wont generalise to basic level*
  • 3 basic example we are not likely to generalise to the superordinate level*
  • If we are given 3 items from the super level they will generalise to the other category hierarchies.*
38
Q

People generalise words more tightly when there are _____ ______ available.

A

More examples

39
Q

Children are more likely to generalise to the superordinate level than adults - true or false

A

False, both aren’t overly likely to generalise to the superordinate level, kids less so

40
Q

Why are people less likely to want to generalise to the superordinate level?

A

Because of ‘communicative intent’

  • The superordinate level is too wide to be useful typically - subordinate level is most specific and is, therefore, most used*
  • The fact that someone is communicating a label to us gives us context of where to apply it*
  • People are very sensitive to communicative intent and not just tracking co-occurrences*
41
Q

If experiments pick training items that are similar in some way and label them something, participants are more likely to apply to label only to the subordinate level. If participants pick the training items they are likely to generalise to the basic level.

Why is this?

A

Communicative intent and distribution matters

If experimenter chooses the item, it suggests that there is meaning in those items and that they should be strict with their definition as these cover the full extent of the variation in items that get the label.

If participants choose items randomly from a selection they believe that these examples are less likely to cover the full extent of the label and they can be more broad.

42
Q
A