Top Hat module 5 Flashcards
Attention according to William James
“Everyone knows what attention is. It is taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seems several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration of consciousness are of its essence. It implies a withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others.” (quote from 1890)
Inattentional blindness
The failure to perceive an object or event that occurs in plain sight. The failure is not due to visual impairments, but to lack of attention (we are unable to perceive information outside of the attentional spotlight). Where you focus your attention determines what properties of a scene you are and aren’t able to report on.
Change-blindess
A form of inattentional blindness in which people have difficulty detecting the difference between two versions of a picture that are alternately presented. In the case of change blindness, researchers can measure the effects of different variables on the probability that someone will notice the change at a given point in the sequence.
Measuring change-blindness
One interesting question that researchers have looked at using this paradigm is the relationship between where people are directly fixating and the likelihood of noticing a change. Most of the time, people don’t notice the change until they look directly at the location where the change is taking place. However, several studies have suggested that people can miss changes even when they are looking directly at a changing object. This can explain why people fall for magic tricks, even though they focus their entire attention on figuring it out.
Why are we prone to change-blindness?
It takes a great deal of processing to go from a flat, two-dimensional image to recognizable objects situated in a three-dimensional world, and we simply don’t have sufficient processing resources to compute everything all at once, especially when you include all the other senses we are constantly using (touch, audition…)
Inattentional deafness
A phenomenon in which auditory information is not perceived when a different high-load task is being performed.
Selective attention
A form of attentional control in which a single data stream (e.g., an object or voice) is processed while others are ignored. Because of the selective nature of our attention, many researchers liken it to a filter, in which the desired information makes it through, while other information is discarded.
Cocktail party effect
The ability to attend to a specific voice in an environment in which other competing voices are present as well.
Dichotic listening task (Cherry, 1953)
An experimental task designed to assess selective attention. Participants are presented, via headphones, with two different audio streams to each of the two ears and tasked with repeating only one of the streams while ignoring the other. Cherry found that his participants could identify whether the unattended channel’s message was spoken by a male or female but could remember little else. In general, Cherry concluded that unless something changed that made the new message physically distinct, the attentional filter would block its contents.
Further research by Moray (1959) on the dichotic listening task
Moray found that a word could be repeated upwards of 30 times and still not be recognized by participants on a later test. This research on selective attention helped inform theories of when our attentional filter starts to block information and prevent it from further processing.
Early-selection models
A model of attention that posits that unattended information is filtered based on basic physical characteristics without processing meaning. For example, at a cocktail party, because understanding the meaning of words requires additional processing, the unattended conversations would only be so much meaningless noise to your brain.
Broadbent’s filter model (Broadbent, 1958)
A high amount of sensory information enters the nervous system. This high informational load is held briefly in sensory memory . Then, selective attention determines which (smaller) portion of the information makes its way through to the “detector,” which is the mechanism that processes the meaning of the information (this is what makes it an early-selection model: meaning is processed only after the filter has been applied).
Evidence against early-selection models
In Moray’s (1959) study mentioned above, participants were often able to process unattended information if their name was spoken in the ignored channel. In addition, research by Grey and Weddeburn (1960) found that if a meaningful narrative was played in such a manner that each successive word alternated between the ears, people would follow the narrative. For example, one stimulus presented a letter, alternating between ears. At the same time, a sequence of numbers was presented to the opposite ear. Participants usually reported the content of the letter rather than the numbers. Both of these results demonstrate that, under some circumstances, the meaning of an unattended stimulus is processed.
Late-selection models
A model of attention that posits that unattended information is first processed in terms of its meaning, and then filtered based on irrelevance to the current task. What attentional filtering is based on, according to these models, is not simple physical characteristics, but whether it fits semantically with attended information.
Attenuator model of attention
A theory of attention (proposed by Anne Treisman, 1964), in which unattended stimuli are processed but at a reduced level relative to attended stimuli. It’s a compromise between the early- and late-selection models. This theory states that there is some filtering of the incoming stimulus based on its physical properties. However, some of the information makes its way through the filter.
Attenuator model of attention - what makes info important enough to be processed?
The idea is that some portion of the signal makes its way through for further processing, although it is reduced (or “attenuated”). The meaning of this reduced signal may be identified if it sufficiently matches some high-priority word (such as your name) or an expected item (e.g., a word that may be more predicted by the previous words). Even reduced signals may be identified if they meet these criteria. We may think of a filtered stimulus as having reduced resolution compared with those that are attended.
Mackay’s twist on the dichotic listening task (1973)
Mackay (1973) altered the dichotic listening task by having participants focus on sentences with a potentially ambiguous meaning, while in the unattended channel a word was repeatedly played that would provide context to the attended sentence. Mackay was interested in how participants’ memory of the attended sentence might change based on the word repeated in the unattended channel. He found that the “unconscious” information was somehow affecting participants’ conscious perception. These results indicate that our attentional filter is much more flexible than once believed
Attentional load
A measure of how much processing resources are needed in order to perform a task. The idea of attentional load suggests that sometimes unattended stimuli may be processed even if we are trying to filter them out (ex. hearing a bag of chips at the theater: even if you are trying to just watch the movie, there may be some processing resources left over that end up processing the unwanted sound of the snack bag. ).
Eriksen flanker task (1974)
A technique used to study attention in which an irrelevant distractor is included alongside experimental stimuli in order to see whether the distractor is processed, increasing reaction time. This research demonstrated that when trying to search for a target letter among distractors, the difficulty varies by what is flanking, or distracting, for the target item. Participants can’t help but process the flankers. When they were consistent with the same response as the target, it caused no decrease in performance. But when the distractors indicated a different response, they created conflict, leading to longer reaction times.
Lavie’s version of the flanker task (1995)
In this case, participants have to search for a letter “O” in the two rings of letters. Next to the rings is a flanker letter that is not relevant to the task, but that may serve to distract the participant if it is incompatible. There is a “low-load” (the letters in the ring are uniform, so it’s easy to spot the target) task, and a “high-load task” (every letter is different).
Results form Lavie’s study
Lavie found that the incompatible flanker only caused an increased reaction time in the low-load condition but not in the high-load conditions. She concluded that in the low-load condition, the fact that the task is not difficult means that there are processing resources left over and that these resources end up processing the flanker, even when it is not wanted, leading to distraction. In the high-load condition, however, the central task of finding the target is difficult and there are few processing resources left over. Therefore, the flanker is not processed and does not cause a distraction.
Green and Bavelier’s twist on Lavie’s research (2003)
They recruited people who had a lot of experience playing first-person video games versus people with little to no experience, and compared their performance in Lavie’s flanker tasks. The non-gamers showed the same pattern of results as found by Lavie: distraction from the incompatible flanker in the low-load condition but not the high-load. However, the gamer showed distraction from the flanker in both conditions. This means that people with video game experience are better able to distribute their attention outside of the central task, even under high-load conditions, leading them to process the distracting flanker.
So, what model is correct?
The role of attentional load suggests that neither the early or late theories are completely right or wrong. Attention may have a movable filter that can be applied more strictly based on the demands of a task.
Automatic processing
Processing that happens even without the allocation of selective attention, typically for highly familiar stimuli or tasks (often the result of practice and learning).