Social and Reproductive Behaviour Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

The experimental wedding:

A

· Linda and Nic, both science writers
· Studied their ‘love hormones’ during their wedding in Devon.
· Measured oxytocin, vasopressin, cortisol, and testosterone, before and after the ceremony

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Data from the experimental wedding:

A

· Oxytocin (the ‘love hormone’) levels were up
· Vassopressin ( the “possession hormone”) went down in Nic.
· Cortisol (the “stress hormone”) was up in Linda before and after (even higher), whereas in Nic they went down after the ceremony.
· Testosterone levels doubled in Nic.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Castration and hormone replacement:

A

· Testis transplantation restores normal development in roosters.
· Transplanted testis were not connected to blood supply or neuronal networks.
· Their effect was mediate by chemicals released to the blood stream:
- Hormones

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Testis transplant from farm animals was also tested in humans with mixed results:

A

· Brinkley’s surgeries were a success for some time, but ethical, methodological, and safety aspects made this enterprise unsustainable.
· Note: Viagra, introduced in 1998, produced $1B sales that year, highlighting the market for sexual enhancers.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What are hormones?:

A

· Hormone - signalling molecular that can carry messages to distant targets through the blood stream.
· Neurohormone - an hormone released by neurons. Targets neighbouring or distant cells.
· Target - organs/cells that can detect hormone/s and it is affected by it/them.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Hormone classes:

A

· Steroid hormones - derived from cholesterol, they can easily travel across cell membranes. e.g., cortisol and progesterone.
· Amine hormones - derived from the amino acid tyrosine. Cannot easily cross the cell membrane. e.g., thyroid hormone (TH).
· Peptide and protein hormones - amino acid chains. Cannot travel through cell membrane = activate membrane receptors. E.g., oxytocin, vasopressin (peptides), prolactin, insulin (protein).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Where are hormones produced?:

A

· Ovaries - estrogen and progesterone
· Testes - testosterone
· Pituitary gland - growth hormone
· Thyroid gland - thyroxine
· Adrenal gland - adrenaline
- Pancreas - insulin

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Genetic sex:

A

· Offspring genetic sex depends on the sex chromosome carried by the sperm and ovum that generates them.
· Genetic sex depends on the father sperm cells, which carry X or Y sex chromosomes

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Development of sex organs:

A

· All the information to develop bodies of either sex is present in the 22 nonsex and the X chromosomes.
· Exposure to sex hormones, both before and after birth, is responsible for sexual dimorphism.
· The Y chromosome controls the development of the glands that produce the male sex hormones.
· Sex organs: gonads (ovaries and testes), internal sex organs, and external genitalia.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Gonads:

A

· Gonads (testes or ovaries) are the first to develop: produce ova or sperm, and hormones.
· Sex-determining region Y (SRY) gene (from Y chromosome) express SRY protein that differentiates gonads into testes.
· Lack of SRY results in ovaries development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Internal sex organs:

A

· During the first two months of gestation, fetus can develop into either male or female.
· At month three, if testes are present and producing hormones (anti-Müllerian hormone and androgens), the internal sex organs develop into male ones.
- Female internal organs do not need the presence of any other hormone to develop.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

External genitalia:

A

· As with internal sex organs, external genitalia do not need hormonal influence to develop into female organs.
· Dihydrotestosterone (androgen produce by testes) develops external genitalia into male version

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Sexual maturation:

A

· So far we revised the primary sex characteristics, present at birth.
· Secondary sex characteristics develop during puberty and are influenced by hormones.
· The hypothalamus release gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH), which ultimately stimulates hormone release by testes or ovaries.
· Testes release testosterone.
· Ovaries release estradiol.
· Gonadotrophins (testosterone and estradiol) are responsible for development of secondary sexual characteristics in males and females, respectively.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Hormonal control of sexual behaviour:

A

· Hormones not only control sexual development, but also interact directly with the nervous system to affect sexual behaviour.
· E.g., hormones control the female reproductive cycle: the menstrual cycle. (estrous cycle in non-primate mammals).
· In non-primate females, sexual behaviour is linked to ovulation. Primate females mate at any time during their menstrual cycle.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Hormones and sexual behaviour in male rodents:

A

· Male rodents sexual behaviour: mounts, intromission, and ejaculation.
· Depends on testosterone levels: castrated male rats injected with testosterone reinstate sexual behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Hormones and sexual behaviour in female rodents:

A

· Sexual behaviour in female rodents: lordosis.
· The female initiates copulation. When receptive, it will approach the male.
· Sexual behaviour depends on estradiol and progesterone. Ovariectomised rats (ovaries removed) display no sexual behaviour.
· ER: estradiol receptor. ER -/-: ER knockout rats
· Similar effects with progesterone receptor KO females (Lydon et al, 1995)

17
Q

Neural control of sexual behaviour - tools:

A

· Retro-tracing to define the circuit that control sexual organs (e.g., Marson & Murphy, 2006). Injection of pseudorabies virus (retrograde tracing) in sexual organs (penis, vagina, clitoris).
· Activation of Fos, a marker of neuronal activity, in key brain regions.
· Identify of neurons containing sex hormone receptors: estrogen and progesterone or testosterone.

18
Q

Neural control of sexual behaviour - males:

A

· Spinal mechanisms.
- Men with complete spinal cord transection above the 10th thoracic segment can ejaculate.
· A group of neurons in the lumbar region (spinal ejaculation generator) lumbar spinothalamic (LSt) cells control ejaculation.
· Destruction of LSt cells in rats abolishes ejaculation, without affecting mounts or intromissions.
· Brain mechanisms - excite or inhibit spinal circuits
· Fos - neuronal activation marker

19
Q

Neuronal controls of sexual behaviour - females:

A

· Contrary to males, females do not have a spinal circuit controlling sexual behaviour.
· Brain mechanisms:
- Neurons contain estrogen and progesterone receptors - VMH and medial amygdala
· Fos: neuronal activation marker

20
Q

Parental behaviour:

A

· Most mammalian species show parental behaviour.
· Hormonal and neuronal control, mostly based on rodents.
· Most research on maternal behaviour.
· Mice/rats pups at birth:
- Blind.
- Do not regulate their own temperature.
- Cannot release urine and faeces.
· Nest building is one of the first maternal behaviours during gestation.

21
Q

Maternal behaviour:

A

· Birth assistance by pulling the pups gently.
· Nursing.
· Periodical licks pups’ anogenital region to stimulate urination and defecation (a way of recycling water – very useful under low water availability).
· Pups retrieval if they leave or are removed from the nest.
- Maternal behaviour is influenced by prenatal hormones, but passage of pups through the birth canal also helps (Yeo & Keverne, 1986)

22
Q

Maternal behaviour 2:

A

· Hormones can influence maternal behaviour, but do not control it.
- I.e., progesterone, the main pregnancy hormone, can facilitate nest building. But, nest building continues after birth, when progesterone is significantly lower.
· Medial preoptic area (MPA; involved in male sexual behaviour) is crucial for maternal behaviour.
· The VTA-NAC pathway, involved in the reward system, is also necessary for maternal behaviour. It is activated when mothers encounter pups.
· Encountering pups is more rewarding than cocaine in lactating females (Ferris et al., 2005).
· Human mothers show activation of the reward system when presented with pictures of their babies (Bartels & Zeki, 2004)

23
Q

Paternal behaviour 2:

A

· A few mammalian species show paternal care for the offspring.
· Monogamous prairie voles share offspring care, whereas polygamous male meadow voles leave the female after mating.
· Size of MPA is less sexually dimorphic in prairie voles than in meadow voles.
· MPA lesions disrupts paternal behaviour in rats and prairie voles.

24
Q

Affiliative behaviours:

A

· Positive social behaviours within the same or different species.
· Can involve individuals of the same or different sex.
- Formation of pair bonds in voles.
- Prosocial behaviours in humans.
- The neuropeptides oxytocin (OXT) and vasopressin (VP) are key for complex social behaviours.

25
Q

Oxytocin and vasopressin:

A

· The neuropeptides oxytocin (OXT) and vasopressin (VP) are produced in the hypothalamus.
· They can be released from the posterior pituitary gland as hormones.
· Or from axons projecting to specific brain regions, as a neuromodulator or neurotransmitter.

26
Q

Pair bonding:

A

· Only 3-5% of mammals are monogamous. Explain in humans and other species.
· Biparental species: males and females raise the young.
· Voles are a prime example:
- Prairie voles: monogamous – bond for life.
- Meadow voles: polygamous or promiscuous – male leaves the female after mating.
· These rodents have been extensively studied to identify the neurobiological bases of affiliative social responses.

27
Q

Hormones influencing pair bonding:

A

· Exposure to a partner while injected with VP or OXT increased the preference for that partner.
1. Male and female paired for 1h. One of them receives intraventricular administration of OXT or VP.
2. Then animals are submitted to a partner preference test (180 min) where they can choose to spend time alone, with a stranger, or with the partner they were exposed during drug administration.

28
Q

Neurobiology of pair bonds:

A
  • Pair bonding is associated with the density of VP receptors in the rewards areas of the brain
29
Q

What about oxytocin receptors?:

A
  • OXT receptors are highly expressed in PFC and Nacc in prairie voles
30
Q

Promiscuous to monogamous?:

A
  • Overexpression of vasopressin receptor in the ventral pallidum (V1aR-vp) enhanced mate preference in meadow voles
31
Q

Formation of pair bonds in humans:

A

· Oxytocin and vasopressin seem to influence pair bonding in humans too, but manipulating these carry ethical concerns, so we do not know for sure.
· OXT intra-nasal caused relaxation and anxiety reduction in humans (Heinrichs et al., 2003).
· Maternal and romantic love activated regions of the brain rich in vasopressin and oxytocin receptors.

32
Q

Prosocial behaviour:

A

· The term “prosocial” is associated with a wide range of positive social behaviors, including trust, cooperation, care, empathy, and altruism—all of which are key for forming and maintaining adaptive human social relationships.

33
Q

Role of oxytocin in prosocial behaviour:

A

· Administration of oxytocin has subtle effects in social behaviours in humans:
- Trust
- Empathy
- Social approach
- Altruism
· Oxytocin has effects on brain regions related to reward and fear related processing.
· Growing interest in translating oxytocin administration for the treatment of psychiatric conditions ranging from anxiety disorders to autism spectrum disorder.
· Oxytocin effects on behaviour are highly influenced by individual and contextual conditions.

34
Q

Oxytocin and trust:

A

· Subjects given oxytocin show significantly higher MU transfer levels.

35
Q

Oxytocin and altruism:

A

· Altruism: non-reciprocal prosocial acts which are aimed at improving the welfare of another individual at a personal cost to the altruist.
· Experiment 1:
1. Saliva samples to measure internal OXT.
2. Participants received 10 €1 coins.
3. Social or environmental donation task.
- Positive correlation between OXT levels and social donation.
- No effect in the ecological frame.
· Experiment 2:
4. Saliva samples to measure internal OXT.
5. Participants received 10 €1 coins.
6. Social or environmental donation task.
- OXT administration increased donations in the social frame, but decreased those in the ecological frame.

36
Q

Oxytocin and empathy:

A

· Empathy: cognitive (recognizing emotional states in others) or emotional (sharing experiences of emotional states perceived in others).
· Experiment:
1. OXT or placebo, intranasal.
2. 45’ later, “Multifaceted Empathy Test” (MET).
- OXT administration increased empathy ratings in all dimensions

37
Q

Oxytocin and social approach:

A

· Approach behaviour to others was measured after intranasal OXT in women.
· Experiment:
1. OXT or placebo, intranasal.
2. 45’ later, Stop Distance Paradigm with female or male experimenters.
· OXT administration decreased the social distance that female participants kept between themselves and an unfamiliar friendly male experimenter.

38
Q

Role of oxytocin in prosocial behaviour:

A

· The involvement of oxytocin in prosocial behaviour seems to be more complex than simply increasing one dimension specifically.
· Expression of OXT and OXT receptor maps into areas involved in anticipatory, appetitive, and aversive cognitive states.