Lecture 7: Emotional Development Flashcards

1
Q

emotions

A

a combination of physiological and cognitive responses to experiences

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

what responses are involved in emotional experiences?

A
  • Neural response
  • Physiological factors
  • Subjective feelings
  • Emotional expression
  • Urge to take action
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

examples of neural responses

A

amygdala activation & the release of cortisol and adrenaline

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

examples of physiological factors

A

heart racing & nausea

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

examples of subjective feelings

A

recognition of danger & feeling of fear

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

examples of emotional expression

A

eyes wide, eyebrows raised & mouth pulled back

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

examples of the urge to take action

A

run away & lock the door

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

discrete emotions theory

A

Neurological and biological systems have evolved to allow humans to experience and express a set of innate, basic emotions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

basic emotions

A

innate emotions that were important for survival and communication and thus largely automatic

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

evidence for basic emotions

A
  • Basic emotions are universal across cultures
  • Basic emotions are present from infancy
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

what are the basic emotions?

A
  • Happiness
  • Fear
  • Anger
  • Sadness
  • Disgust
  • Surprise
    (inside-out characters plus surprise)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

beyond basic emotions

A
  • Other emotions develop later and/or are not culturally universal
  • Other emotions are variations in intensity or combinations of basic emotions
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

how do we know what babies are feeling?

A

Systems of coding facial cues have been developed to make interpretations of infants’ emotions more objective. They link particular facial expressions and facial muscle movements with particular emotions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

happiness cues

A

smiling, raising cheeks, eyes squinting

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

anger cues

A

strongly furrowed brow that comes down in the centre, open square-shaped mouth, sometimes baring teeth, flared nostrils

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

emotions at birth

A

At birth, infants experience 2 general emotional states

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

what 2 emotional states are present at birth?

A
  • Positive, indicated by approach behaviour
  • Negative/distress, indicated by crying or withdrawal behaviour
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

emergence of basic emotions

A

Basic emotions emerge in a predictable sequence over the first year of life

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

why is happiness adaptive?

A

it motivates us to approach situations that are likely to increase chances of survival

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

when do smiles evoked by biological states first occur?

A

from birth

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

when do social smiles first occur?

A

2-3 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

when do infants first laugh?

A

5 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

development of the source of happiness

A
  • What makes children smile and laugh changes with cognitive development
  • ex. At 5 months old, they laugh at bodily noises, but at 4 years old, they laugh at jokes
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

why is anger adaptive?

A

it helps us defend ourselves against threats and to overcome obstacles to our goals

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

when do infants begin to express anger?

A

4 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

when do infants peak in the tendency to react with anger?

A

24 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

why does the tendency to react with anger decline after 24 months?

A

likely due to kids’ greater ability to express themselves with language and improved emotion regulation skills

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

matching anger to the situation

A
  • As children get older, they are better able to match anger to the situation
  • They are angrier if a hurtful action is intentional vs. unintentional
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

what context do social smiles emerge in?

A

usually in interactions with parents

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

benefit of social smiles

A

promote care from caregivers and foster bonding

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

why is fear adaptive?

A

it motivates escape from danger or solicits protection from caregivers

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

when do infants begin to express fear?

A

7 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

when does a fear of strangers and separation anxiety emerge?

A

8 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
34
Q

when does separation anxiety begin to decline?

A

15 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
35
Q

development of the source of fear

A

What scares children changes with cognitive development

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
36
Q

fear at 3-5-years-old

A

children fear imaginary creatures

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
37
Q

fear at 7+-years-old

A

fear related to everyday situations

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
38
Q

when do sadness, surprise, and disgust emerge

A

in the first year

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
39
Q

function of surprise

A

indicates that the world is working contrary to expectations and is thus important for learning

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
40
Q

function of sadness

A

elicits care and comfort from their caregiver in reaction to a loss

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
41
Q

what is necessary for the emergence of sadness?

A

object permanence

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
42
Q

what usually causes sadness in infants?

A

being separated from their parents

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
43
Q

why is disgust adaptive?

A

it helps us avoid potential poisons or bacteria

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
44
Q

what are the first expressions of disgust directed towards?

A

food

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
45
Q

when do self-conscious emotions emerge?

A

once a child has a sense of self as separate from other people & an appreciation of what adults expect of them

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
46
Q

when does a sense of self as separate from other people emerge?

A

around 18 months

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
47
Q

examples of self-conscious emotions

A

guilt, shame, embarrassment, pride, and empathy

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
48
Q

when do self-conscious emotions emerge?

A

around 2 years

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
49
Q

culture and self-conscious emotions

A

Culture influences the frequency and types of self-conscious emotions that are most likely to be experienced

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
50
Q

self-conscious emotions in individualistic cultures

A

more likely to experience pride

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
51
Q

self-conscious emotions in collectivist cultures

A

more likely to experience guilt and shame

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
52
Q

guilt vs. shame

A
  • Guilt and shame are often elicited by similar situations but are distinct emotional reactions
  • Generally, guilt is healthier than shame
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
53
Q

guilt

A

feelings of regret about one’s behaviour associated with the desire to fix the consequences of that behaviour

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
54
Q

shame

A

a self-focused feeling of personal failure associated with the desire to hide

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
55
Q

when can expressions of guilt and shame be distinguished?

A

around 2 years

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
56
Q

guilt and shame in 2-year-olds study

A
  • When 2-year-olds play with a doll that has been rigged so that one leg falls off during play, they show different reactions
  • Guilt: trying to fix the doll and quickly telling the adult about the accident
  • Shame: didn’t try to fix the doll, avoided the adult, and delayed telling them about the accident
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
57
Q

parental influence and guilt vs. shame

A
  • The child is more likely to feel guilt if the parent emphasizes the badness of the action
  • The child is more likely to feel shame if the parent experiences the badness of the child
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
58
Q

how does emotional recognition begin?

A

Identifying emotions in adults’ faces comes before identifying one’s own emotions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
59
Q

facial recognition at 3 months

A

can distinguish facial expressions of happiness, surprise, and anger
(i.e. habituated to pictures of happy faces and then dishabituate when presented with a picture of a surprised face)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
60
Q

facial recognition at 7 months

A

can distinguish expressions of fear and sadness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
61
Q

social referencing

A

use of parents’ facial expressions and tone of voice to decide how to deal with novel/ambiguous situations

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
62
Q

what enables social referencing?

A

Recognizing parents’ emotions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
63
Q

social referencing and the visual cliff study

A
  • 0% of babies cross if the parent looks scared
  • 75% of babies cross if the parent looks happy
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
64
Q

social referencing and the visual cliff takeaways

A
  • Children can distinguish between emotional expressions
  • Children rely on parents’ reactions to figure out how to react to a situation themselves (social referencing)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
65
Q

when do children learn to label emotions?

A

3 years

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
66
Q

initial emotion labelling abilities

A

are simply described as feeling good or bad

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
67
Q

labelling emotions throughout childhood

A

The ability to label emotions improves over early childhood

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
68
Q

when do children begin to understand mixed emotions?

A

5 years old

69
Q

when do children begin to understand that a person’s facial expressions do not necessarily match what they’re feeling?

A

5 years old

70
Q

understanding real vs. fake emotions study

A

3- and 5-year-old hear a story about a child forgetting her favourite toy for a sleepover but that she doesn’t want to show how she feels

71
Q

understanding real vs. fake emotions study findings in 3-year-olds

A

think that the child will be showing sadness

72
Q

understanding real vs. fake emotions study findings in 5-year-olds

A

know that the child will be sad but will be showing happiness on her face

73
Q

why do children improve in their abilities to detect fake emotions?

A

greater understanding of display rules

74
Q

display rules

A

Social norms about when, where, and how much one should show emotions and which emotions are appropriate in a given context

75
Q

why are display rules important?

A

for successful social interactions

76
Q

implications of understanding real vs. fake emotions

A

Understanding false emotions allows children to fake emotions themselves and lie

77
Q

emotional regulation

A

A set of conscious and unconscious processes used to manage emotional experiences and expressions

78
Q

development of emotional regulation

A

Develops gradually during childhood

79
Q

co-regulation

A

parents regulate infants’ distress through soothing or distraction

80
Q

why is co-regulation necessary?

A

because infants cannot regulate their own emotions

81
Q

when do infants begin to show rudimentary emotional regulation skills?

A

5 months

82
Q

how do 5-month-olds regulate their emotions?

A

self-comforting behaviours & self-distraction

83
Q

self-comforting behaviours

A

repetitive actions that create a mildly positive sensation

84
Q

self-distraction

A

looking away from the upsetting stimulus

85
Q

what emotional regulation behaviour do children rely on most in the first few months of life

A

they learn to rely more on self-distraction rather than self-comforting behaviours

86
Q

emotional regulation behaviour in middle childhood

A

Beginning in middle childhood (6-8 years), children also rely on cognitive strategies and problem-solving

87
Q

Children that have good emotional regulation skills:

A
  • Have higher well-being
  • Are more socially skilled and are liked better by their peers and teachers
    (poor regulation skills put kids at risk of being bullied)
  • Do better in school
88
Q

why does emotional regulation improve?

A
  • Motor development
  • Increased parental expectation that child should be able to manage their emotional arousal
  • Cognitive development
89
Q

emotional regulation and motor development

A

Greater ability to control bodily movements enables self-soothing and distraction in infancy

90
Q

parental expectations and emotional regulation

A

Children internalize this expectation and comply

91
Q

cognitive development and emotional regulation

A

Improved attention and inhibition enable better emotional regulation skills

92
Q

adolescents and moodiness and study method

A
  • experience-sampling method
  • Adolescents and adults wore a pager that beeped at random times
  • It reported on mood when it beeped
93
Q

adolescents and moodiness study findings

A
  • adolescents report more frequent high-intensity emotions than adults
  • Both more intense negative and positive emotions
  • Intense moods don’t last as long compared to adults
94
Q

adolescents and moodiness study takeaway

A

Adolescents are indeed more moody than adults

95
Q

emotional changes in adolescents study method

A
  • adolescents rated emotions during each day of the school week for 3 weeks
  • They did this for 5 years
96
Q

emotional changes in adolescents study findings

A
  • Happiness decreases over adolescence
  • Sadness and anxiety increase, especially for girls
  • Anger increases and then decreases towards the end of adolescence
97
Q

emotional changes in adolescents study implications

A
  • Gender differences in emotional experience in adolescents
  • An increase in negative emotions during adolescence is normal
  • Struggles to cope with these changes can lead to the development of depression and anxiety disorders
  • It can be difficult to distinguish between normal changes in adolescents’ emotional experiences vs. mental health issues
98
Q

risk-taking in adolescents

A
  • Impulsivity increases during early adolescence, peaks in middle/late adolescence, and then declines in adulthood
  • This is found across cultures and historical times
99
Q

motor vehicle deaths

A

motor vehicles peak in 21-24-year-olds, and are also very high in 16-20-year-olds

100
Q

why do adolescents take more risks?

A

The result of changes to the limbic system & the prefrontal cortex, both of which are involved in decision-making

101
Q

limbic system

A

involved in emotional and reward processing

102
Q

prefrontal cortex

A

involved in goal-directed behaviour, deliberation, and inhibitory control

103
Q

changes to the limbic system in adolescence

A

Reward processing in the limbic systemic is heightened in adolescence due to the synaptogenesis of dopamine receptors

104
Q

changes to the PFC in adolescence

A

Synaptic pruning and myelination in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) occur until the mid-20s

105
Q

myelination

A

thickening of the myelin sheath surrounding axons, which increases the speed of neural signal transmission

106
Q

what is an immature PFC associated with?

A

difficulties with inhibition, impulse control, and planning

107
Q

implications of risk-taking in adolescence

A
  • Adolescents take more risks because of the maturational imbalance between their limbic system and prefrontal cortex
  • But, risk-taking is also a good thing. It promotes independence by trying new experiences
108
Q

what is nucleus accumbens activation associated with?

A

The degree of nucleus accumbens activation during reward anticipation is positively correlated with self-reported risk-taking in daily life

109
Q

temperament

A

Individual differences in emotion, self-regulation, activity levels, and attention that are consistent over time and across contexts

110
Q

basis of temperament

A

Present from infancy thus thought to be genetically-based

111
Q

why do kids show very different reactions to the same situation?

A

they have different temperaments

112
Q

who described the type approach to temperament?

A

Thomas et al.

113
Q

3 categories of babies in the type approach to temperament

A
  • easy babies
  • difficult babies
  • slow-to-warm-up babies
114
Q

easy babies

A

adjust easily to new situations, quickly establish daily routines, such as sleep and eating, and generally are cheerful in mood and easy to calm

115
Q

what percentage of babies are easy?

A

40%

116
Q

difficult babies

A

slow to adjust to new experiences, tend to react negatively and intensely to novel stimuli and events, irregular in their daily routines, and bodily functions

117
Q

what percentage of babies are difficult?

A

10%

118
Q

slow-to-warm-up babies

A

somewhat difficult at first, but become easier over time as they have repeated contact with new objects, people, and situations

119
Q

what percentage of babies are slow-to-warm-up?

A

15%

120
Q

what prompted the dimensional approach to temperament?

A

Many children did not fit into one of Thomas et al.,’s categories

121
Q

5 dimensions of temperament

A
  • smiling and laughing
  • distress (in infancy)/anger (in childhood)
  • fear
  • attention span
  • activity level
122
Q

smiling and laughing

A

positive emotional response to a change in a stimulus

123
Q

Distress (in infancy)/anger (in childhood)

A

negative emotional response related to having an ongoing task interrupted or blocked

124
Q

fear

A

the tendency to experience unease or nervousness in new situations

125
Q

attention span

A

attention to an object or task for an extended period

126
Q

activity level

A

rate and extent of gross motor body movements

127
Q

how is the dimension approach to temperament assessed?

A
  • Parent and/or teacher responses to questions assessing each dimension
  • Observing how kids react to lab tasks designed to assess each dimension
128
Q

consistency of temperament over time

A
  • Temperament is largely consistent/stable over time (influence of genetics)
  • But, some changes in temperament over time are possible (influence of the caregiver)
129
Q

temperament twin studies

A

Identical twins have more similar temperaments than fraternal twins

130
Q

when is changing temperament easiest?

A

when kids are younger

131
Q

implications of temperament

A
  • Children contribute to their emotional development through their temperament
  • Some children are easier to parent than others
132
Q

the influence of family in emotional development

A

Family, especially parents, play a huge role in children’s emotional development

133
Q

how do parents influence emotional development?

A
  • Parent’s expression of emotions (indirect influence on emotional development)
  • Parent’s reactions to children’s emotions (direct influence on emotional development)
134
Q

parents’ expression of emotions

A

Parents’ emotional expressions serve as a model of when and how to express emotions

135
Q

Children who grow up with parents who tend to not show emotions tend to:

A
  • Not express emotions themselves (they learn to see emotions as bad)
  • Have trouble identifying and understanding emotions in themselves and others
  • Struggle with regulating intense emotions
136
Q

Children who grow up with parents who express a high level of positive emotion tend to:

A
  • Express more positive emotions themselves
  • Have higher well-being
  • Have better social skills
137
Q

Children who grow up with parents who express a high level of negative emotion tend to:

A
  • Experience and express more negative emotions themselves
  • Be less socially competent
  • Have poorer emotion regulation skills
138
Q

parents’ reactions to childrens’ emotions

A

Parents’ reactions to their children’s emotions directly influence children’s emotional development

139
Q

mirroring

A
  • Behaviours in which a parent reflects the emotions of their child to them
  • Contingent, quick response to the infant
140
Q

how is mirroring conveyed?

A

Conveyed through verbal and non-verbal cues

141
Q

what characterizes mirroring?

A

warmth

142
Q

why is mirroring important?

A
  • It validates and normalizes the child’s emotions
  • Helps the child identify and understand their emotions
143
Q

still-face paradigm

A

Lab procedure in which a parent goes through a repetitive sequence with their child in which they have
2 minutes to play with the infant & 2 minutes of still face (no reaction to the infant)

144
Q

results of the still-face paradigm

A

Infants quickly become distressed in reaction to still-face and this distress increases with each still-face episode

145
Q

implications of the still-face paradigm

A
  • Infants are attuned to parents’ emotions
  • They are distressed when the parent doesn’t react as expected
  • They often engage in self-distraction
146
Q

emotional coaching

A

The use of discussion and other forms of instruction to teach children how to cope with, regulate, and appropriately express emotions

147
Q

emotional coaching across cultures

A

What is seen as appropriate depends on the culture

148
Q

example of emotional coaching

A

“You seem anxious about the test. Let’s walk through the first step that can get us going in the right direction.”

149
Q

Supportive/sensitive reaction

A

-mirroring + emotional coaching
- Is the ideal way to react to children’s emotions

150
Q

why is a supportive reaction important?

A
  • Validates a child’s emotions
  • Helps the child understand their emotions
  • Fosters emotional regulation
  • Associated with higher self-esteem
  • Fosters social competence
  • Associated with better performance in school
151
Q

example of a supportive/sensitive reaction

A

“You seem worried and upset about the test. I sometimes feel the same way when I have a big thing to do at work. Let’s walk through the first step that can get us going in the right direction…”

152
Q

dismissive reaction

A

coaching but no mirroring

153
Q

example of a dismissive reaction

A

“You’re fine. There’s no need to be nervous. You’ll just study and it’ll be ok”

154
Q

over-validating reaction

A

mirroring but no coaching

155
Q

example of an over-validating reaction

A

Parent looks anxious. “OMG! The test is next week! You must be so nervous. You have so much material to study. Where do we even begin?”

156
Q

critical reaction

A

no mirroring or emotional coaching

157
Q

example of a critical reaction

A

In an angry tone. “What’s wrong with you? You always get like this before a test and then you get a bad grade.”

158
Q

implications of lack of supportive/sensitive reaction

A

Children who grow up with parents who habitually do not show a support/sensitive reaction tend to be less emotionally and socially competent

159
Q

why do parents react the way they do?

A
  • cultural differences
  • Generational differences in norms for emotional expression
  • Family reactions to emotions when parents themselves were children (intergenerational transmission of emotional regulation)
  • Parents’ mood and emotions in the moment
160
Q

cultural differences and emotional reactions

A
  • Emotional expression is more encouraged in independent vs. interdependent cultures
  • Reactions to specific emotions differ by culture
161
Q

differential susceptibility hypothesis

A
  • Some children are highly sensitive to both negative and positive environmental conditions
  • Sensitive temperament + negative home environment = negative outcomes
  • Sensitive temperament + positive home environment = positive outcomes
162
Q

what hypothesis is known as the dandelion and orchid theory?

A

the differential susceptibility hypothesis

163
Q

dandelion and orchid theory

A
  • Dandelions are hearty and can live in any conditions (less sensitive temperament)
  • Orchids can only live in certain conditions (sensitive temperament)
164
Q

Children with more difficult/negative temperaments have:

A
  • More behavioural problems if raised with low-quality childcare
  • But, they have the lowest levels of behavioural problems if raised by high-quality childcare
165
Q

Children with impulsive temperaments have:

A
  • Higher levels of alcohol abuse in adolescence if raised in harsh families
  • But, have the lowest levels of alcohol abuse if raised in positive family environments
166
Q

implications of differential susceptibility

A
  • Children’s temperament and the environment they grow up in jointly determine their outcomes
  • When all kids benefit most from sensitive parenting, it is particularly important for children who are more temperamentally sensitive to their environment (orchids)
167
Q

example of mirroring

A

a baby looks upset. The parent also furrows their eyebrows and frowns.

168
Q

emotional development timeline

A
  • at birth: infants experience & positive emotional states, they show biological/reflexive smiles
  • 2 months: social smiles emerge
  • 3 months: can distinguish between faces of happiness, surprise, and anger
  • 4 months: begin to express anger
  • 5 months: begin to laugh (mainly at bodily noises), show rudimentary emotional regulation skills (self-soothing & self-distraction)
  • 7 months: begin to express fear, can distinguish between expressions of fear and sadness
  • 8 months: fear of strangers & separation anxiety emerge
  • 15 months: separation anxiety declines
  • 18 months: a sense of self as separate from others emerges
  • 2 years: peak in the tendency to react with anger, guilt & shame emerge, guilt & shame can be distinguished
  • 3 years: rudimentary ability to identify and label emotions in the self and others (good vs. bad), fear imaginary creatures
  • 4 years: laugh at jokes
  • 5 years: understand that people can have mixed emotions, begin to understand that a person’s facial expressions don’t necessarily match their feeligns
  • 6-8 years: rely on cognitive strategies & problem-solving to regulate emotions, fear relates to everyday situations