** Intelligence tutorial test Flashcards
- What is heritability? What is the estimated heritability of intelligence?
Heritability is the proportion of variability in a phenotype that is accounted for by variation in genotype.
Most studies estimate the heritability of IQ as somewhere between .4 and .8 (generally less for children).
However, there is no single “true” value for the heritability of intelligence, as the heritability of a trait depends on the relative variances of the predictors – genotype and environment. In humans, who don’t live in a controlled environment, variability of this predictor is uncontrolled.
- Which SES groups show higher heritability of intelligence according to the “heritability by SES” hypothesis? Which countries and age groups show the strongest and weakest evidence for the hypothesis?
High-SES individuals show higher heritability of intelligence than low-SES individuals.
This has been demonstrated on samples in America, but studies of European countries such as Britain and the Netherlands have failed to confirm this interaction consistently.
Many studies have documented the SES by heritability interaction in children, but only one of several has been able to observe it in adults. This suggests the interaction may not persist beyond childhood.
- Compared to other methods, are adoption studies likely to over-estimate or under-estimate the role of the environment in predicting intelligence? Why?
Adoption studies are likely to underestimate the role of environment due to the restricted social class range of adoptive homes, which are generally high SES and more supportive of intellectual growth than nonadoptive homes.
The restriction of range (as much as 70% in some studies) means that the possible magnitude of correlations between adoptive parents’ IQ and that of their children is curtailed.
Since environment has a larger impact on outcomes among lower SES individuals, removing them from the sample 1) reduces the variance of environment 2) reduces the average impact of environment, thereby causing a reduction in the measured role of environment.
Thus excluding participants from the lowest SES levels biases the results by omitting the portion of the distribution for which environmental effects are known to be strongest.
- What possible explanation for the “heritability by SES” hypothesis is suggested in the reading? Describe the study that supports this explanation.
One explanation is that low-SES children do not get to develop their genetic potential.
Support for this hypothesis is offered by the Infant Health and Development Program (IHDP) – a broad-based intervention program designed to improve the cognitive function and school performance of approx. 1,000 low birth weight infants. When tested at 96 months, the heritabilities of the intervention group were significantly higher than the control group on 7/8 of a wide battery of tests, including the WISC and Raven’s.
- What is the research evidence for specific genes underlying differences in intelligence? Is this different for the normal ranges of intelligence and the prediction of mental retardation?
There is scant evidence that intelligence can be accounted for by specific genes. A genome-wide scan using 7,000 subjects (Butcher et al., 2008) found only six genetic markers (SNPs) associated with cognitive ability, and only one of these was statistically significant. When the six markers were considered together they barely explained 1% of the variance in general cognitive ability.
Prediction of mental retardation is more successful – 282 individual genes responsible for specific forms of mental retardation have been identified (Inlow & Restifo, 2004).
- The article suggests several reasons why it may be difficult to identify the specific genes responsible for genetic variation in intelligence. Give at least three of these reasons.
- Technology is not advanced enough to be able to find trends in genetic sequences, identify specific genes, and their associations with traits and behaviours.
- The number of genes involved in an outcome as complex as intelligence is very large, and therefore the contribution of any individual locus is just as small as the number of genes is large and thus very difficult to detect without huge samples.
- A linear specification of the genetic sequence may not capture all of the information contained in the genome. Statistical interactions or nonlinear associations among genes, or between genes and particular environments, could foil an effort to understand intelligence by simply adding up the small effects of many genes.
- What are the effects of breastfeeding on IQ? Are there confounding variables that might explain these effects?
Studies have reported an increase in IQ of as much as 6 points for infants born with normal weight and as much as 8 points for those born prematurely. The advantage seems to persist into adulthood.
Social class and IQ may be confounding variables. When social class and IQ of the mother is controlled for, a metaanalysis generated the reduced score of a 3-point effect of breastfeeding on IQ. Another found essentially no effect on academic achievement scores when the mother’s IQ was controlled for except for a modest effect for children breastfed for more than seven months.
- The paper describes two genetic/biological studies that account for some of the effect of breastfeeding on the child’s IQ. Describe: (a) the genetic contingency in the effects of breastfeeding on IQ; and (b) the animal modeling study
An important study indicates that breastfeeding is effective in raising IQ by about six points, but only for the large portion of the population having one of two alleles at a particular site that regulates fatty acids and is influenced by breast milk.
Human breast milk contains fatty acids that are not found in formula and that have been shown to prevent neurological deficits in mice.
- What is the effect of adoption on the IQ of the adopted child? What explanation does the paper give for this effect?
It raises it, on average, by 12 points (compared siblings left with birth parents or children adopted by lower SES parents).
This is likely to environmental differences that are associated with social class, given that adoption typically moves children from lower to higher SES homes.
In these high SES homes, children are in an environment more likely be supportive of intellectual growth than that of nonadoptive families.
- What are the two main differences between high and low SES home environments according to the Hart and Risley (1995) study?
- The amount parents talk to children. Hart and Risley (1995) showed that the child of professional parents has heard 30 million words by the age of three, the child of working-class parents has heard 20 million words, and the vocabulary is much richer for the higher SES child. The child of unemployed African American mothers has heard 10 million words by the age of three.
- The ratio of encouraging comments made to children versus reprimands. The child of professional parents received six encouragements for every reprimand, the child of working-class parents received two encouragements per reprimand, and the child of unemployed African-American mothers received two reprimands per encouragement.
- According to the paper, do twin studies over-estimate or under-estimate the heritability of intelligence? Why does the paper propose that this is the case?
Twin studies tend to overestimate heritability of intelligence.
This is because of a bias in samples towards higher SES individuals. The reasons for this are that lower SES individuals are difficult to recruit and the lower SES individuals who volunteer may resemble higher SES individuals on variables relevant to overestimation of heritability effects.
Since heritability is higher in high SES children, who dominate the sample, the heritability estimate is skewed upwards.
- What are the effects of birth order on IQ? What evidence is there that these effects are social rather than biological?
Despite many studies on this subject there is a lack of consensus that birth order affects IQ, although a recent –and particularly rigorous – study indicates an IQ advantage of 3 points for firstborn children over later-born children.
Evidence that these effects are social rather than biological is provided by studies showing that second-born children in families in which the firstborn child died early in life have IQs as high as firstborns at age 18. Thus genetic or gestational factors do not account for the difference in IQ. A possible explanation for a birth-order effect on IQ is that the intellectual environment of the firstborn is superior to that of the later-born because the firstborn has the full attention of the parents for a period of time.
- Name at least three sources of evidence that attending school increases IQ scores.
- Natural experiments in which children are deprived of school for an extended period of time show deficits in IQ of as much as 2 SD.
- A child who enters fifth grade approximately a year earlier than a child of nearly the same age who enters fourth grade will have a Verbal IQ more than 5 points higher at the end of the school year (Cahan & Cohen, 1989) and as much as 9 percentiles higher in eighth grade.
- A natural experiment was created in Norway when an extra two years of schooling beyond the seventh grade began to be required. Effects on IQ were substantial at age 19.
- What are the effects of early intervention programs on IQ? What are the other effects of early intervention programs?
The best prekindergarten programs for lower SES children have a substantial effect on IQ, but this typically fades by late primary school, perhaps because the environments of the children do not remain enriched.
If lower-SES children are placed in average or above-average elementary schools following the prekindergarten interventions, children have scored an average IQ 10 points higher than those of controls when they were adolescents. Another study reported IQs of 4.5 points higher than those of controls when individuals were 21 years old.
By adulthood, individuals who had participated in interventions such as the Abecedarian project were about half as likely to have repeated a grade in school or to have been assigned to special education classes and were far more likely to have completed high school, attended college, and even to own their own home.
- Which three aspects of intelligence are stimulant drugs known to affect?
Stimulant drugs have been shown to give modest enhancements in attention, working memory and executive function in healthy, normal adults.