Food Choice Flashcards
What is the difference between preference and liking?
– Preference is relative (would you prefer to eat a stale bread crust or a deep fried spider?)
– Liking is absolute (you would probably like neither)
What is the omnivores paradox?
The omnivores paradox: there’s an enormous range of foods out there but only a subset is fit to eat. Our problem is determining what is fit to eat
1. There are around 0.5 M species of plant and 2 M species of animal
2. Probably the majority (80%) of plant and animal species are edible, and many fungi and algae etc too
3. Yet only around 2-400 species of plant and animal are eaten - limited historically by availability, and now by commercial imperatives, but most importantly by biopsychosocial factors.
4. One class of biopsychosocial factor are the innate systems that guide our food choice.
5. These innate systems help us select what is safe and they influence what our culture calls food, as well as our individual food choices.
Why study food choice? (5)
– Dietary choice has a major effect on health
– Consequently, we need to know if and how we might alter peoples preferences and how we can generate healthy food choices
• Apart from better health, one consequence of this would be to save substantial sums of public money currently spent on diet-related diseases (about 1 billion$/year in Australia)
• And understanding food preferences is also vital for making money
• To create new food and drink products, or to increase sales volume, you need to understand
• What people like
• How to make them like it
• How to make them like it more
• The food business is big business in Australia
What affects our innate preferences?
Genetic and environmental influences generally interact
Our predisposition to learn from our and other peoples experience with food is the most important genetic disposition
What is our preference for sweet food?
• In almost every animal studied (excepting certain carnivores, where the sweet receptor gene is dysfunctional), a sweetened food will be taken in preference to a non-sweetened one
• Why? In the environment, a sweet food usually signals calories (energy) and sweet foods are typically quite rare (being honey, honey ants, fruits)
• Our liking for sweet foods has had many effects
Is our preference for sweet in our genes?
– Premature babies preferred sucking a sucrose impregnated nipple to a plain one
– Day old neonates and even babies with cortical damage (hydrocephalus) showed an ‘ingestive facial expression’ when given sucrose
Does experience have a role in our preference for sweet? (5)
• Perhaps experience exerts a more subtle effect on our preference for sweetness
– Amniotic exposure: such exposure is powerful (vanilla breast milk formula; garlic, aniseed) but no evidence to date
– Pre-lacteal sucrose (prior to a milk feed to get them to suck); These babies prefer sucrose to those who have not had it, but the effect wanes rapidly (neophobia?)
• However, rat studies suggest no effect on preference for early exposure
– Rat pups given 0%, 12%, 48% sucrose (matched for energy)
– Later all the pups preferred the 48% solution suggesting that experience had little effect on preference
– Context - sweet is good in some foods but not in others
• If experience affected sweetness preference, then such preference should increase with age (exposure)
– Yet sweet preference actually decreases with age, with a peak at around 15
What is the physiology behind our preference for sweet?
• The tongue has one type of sweetness receptor, all of which pass their signals to the chorda tympani nerve
• This nerve has more fibers dedicated to ‘sweet’(i.e., more labelled lines) than to any other taste
• These fibers pass via the NST to several areas of the brain that seem to drive our liking for this taste
– Periaqueductal grey and the nucleus acumbens shell
• These are both brain reward areas
• These areas are rich in endogenous opioids, which is why
– Rats fed sucrose can endure more pain on the hot plate test, a phenomenon that is naloxone sensitive
– Sweet tastes in children reduce pain during circumcision and injections
– Naloxone reduces sweet liking in adults
– And ‘opiate-like’ withdrawal syndrome in mice
– Brain stem ingestive areas
• Drive reflexive ingestive expressions and behaviour
What is the genetic evidence for our preference for sweet? (5)
• The continued consumption of sweetened foods in fructose intolerance
– Fructose intolerance is caused by an inability to move fructose across the gut wall
– Even with the consequences many people with this condition continue on occasions to eat sugar rich foods
• The failure to find any difference in sucrose consumption/liking between MZ’s & DZ’s
– There was such strong selection pressure for liking sweet things most of us are quite similar in this regard
• Preference for sucrose can be bred in, at least in rats
– Most of us (and rats) have an inverted U shaped liking response to sucrose as a function of its concentration
– The concentration of peak liking can be shifted upwards by selectively breeding the rats who like very sweet things
• Sucrose liking is reduced in ‘supertasters’
– They have more sweetness receptors so they need less sugar to get the same ‘hit’ as someone with fewer receptors
• All cultures to which sweet food has been introduced have readily consumed it and continue to do so
• In conclusion – we are ‘hard-wired’ to like sweet things, although the environment can modulate this somewhat (i.e., context)
What 3 physiological effects does salt deprivation have?
• Salt deprivation results in two things…
– Dehydration (the body expels water to boost blood salt concentration)
• This triggers the kidney-renin-angiotensin system, which induces a desire to consume salt
– Sensory change
– Salt deprivation results in no change in the taste nerve’s sensitivity to salt, but a lower response to high concentrations
– The same also occurs for certain brain neurons
• The effect of this is too make the animal/person prefer a higher salt concentration than before
Do babies like salt?
• Babies may not be initially sensitive to salt
– Babies have readily consumed high salt formula after mixing errors, they just can not taste it
• However, by four months they readily consume
and react to salt
– Interestingly, mothers who were dehydrated during pregnancy have children who show a preference for higher salt concentrations than controls
• This might be another example of epigenetic changes, in which the expression of particular genes is changed in the foetus based upon the environmental conditions that the foetus may face when born
What evidence do we have in humans that salt craving is under direct physiological control? (4)
– Adrenal tumour patient; altered kidneys capacity to retain salt, body loses salt, kid liked salty food
– Salt preferences peak in adolescence suggesting hormonal influence
– Salt ingestion (greater preference) in cold climates and blood pressure
– Exercise and salt preference (participants add more salt to soup for up to 12hrs after vigorous sweat-inducing exercise)
How much salt do we need?
• A preference for salty food is a good thing when it is in short supply
• Recommended daily allowance in adults is 6g – roughly equivalent to the salt in one slice of pizza
• Most adults consume around 12g/day, which exerts an upward effect on blood pressure, increasing risk of stroke and other diseases
Most of our excess salt consumption occurs passively in processed food
– It is added as a bulking agent (often to processed meats) as this allows a food to contain more water (cost reduction)
– It is a useful preservative
– It provides the flavour to many processed foods (notably breads)
Can experience exert any effect over preference/liking for salt?
• Yes. Several longitudinal studies indicate that low salt diets reduce preference for saltier
– Parallel examples seem to exist for fat (going from normal to low-fat milk) and sweet
Can babies taste bitter (and sour) and why?
• Just as neonates find sweet tastes appealing they demonstrate facial expressions indicative of disgust when given bitter tastes
• These expressions are nearly identical to those obtained in animals and in children with cortical damage, suggesting an innate origin
• Why should this be so?
• Primary reasons is that most plant alkaloids (e.g. nicotine, atropine, solanine) are poisons and most taste bitter (poisonous animals bitter and colourful too)
• So it is a useful ‘safety-net’ to reject a food if it is bitter