Test 3 Readings Flashcards
scripts
In adults, infants memory tends to be organised into schemas or scripts, for routine events. Each script is a generic or abstract knowledge structure that represents the temporal and causal sequences of events in very specific contexts.
Younger infants tend to blend their memory of novel events into their script but older infants are able to keep them seperate.
This tendency may rely on the saliency of the novel event on whether or not it is deemed atypical by the child and kept seperate.
Parental Interaction Style and the Development of Episodic Memories
Parents tend to ask young children fairly specific questions about shared past events, such as “Where did we go yesterday?”,“Who did we see?” and “Who was there with us?” (Hudson, 1990). Repeated experience of such questions may help young children to organise events into the correct temporal and causal order, and to learn which aspects of events are the most important to recall. Parents who ask more of these specific questions have children with better memories.
Two types of parent-interaction styles:
o Eleaborative:
Mothers consistently elaborated on the
information that their child recalled and
then evaluated it.
Questions like “Where were our seats?”
and “What was the stage set up like?”
o Non-Elaborative:
Mothers tended to switch topics and to
provide less narrative structure, and
seldom used elaboration and evaluation.
Asked the same question repeatedly
“What kinds of animals did you see? And
what else? And what else?.”
• Children of the elaborative mothers tended
to remember more material at 58 and 70
months. Reese et al. suggested that
maternal elaborativeness was a key factor
in children’s developing memory abilities.
For example, maternal elaborativeness
might be expected to lead to more
organised and detailed memories, and
might facilitate children’s developing
understanding of time.This conversational
style also allows opportunities for mothers
and their chil- dren to agree and disagree
about the past.This negotiation could help
the child to understand that the self has a
unique perspective on the past.
The Development of Eye-Witness Memory
Eye-witness memory: a special kind of episodic memory
Eye-witness memory: a special kind of episodic memory is memory for events that may not have appeared significant at the time that they were experienced.
Adults have poor memory of events as eye-witnesses so it is important to know if children are equally poor or worse at recalling events.
Its’s important to know whether the abuse really occurred, or whether ‘memories’ of abuse have been created as a result of repeated suggestive questioning by adults.
The Accuracy of Children’s Eye-Witness Testimony
(A) The Role of Leading Questions
Repeated suggestive questioning created more false memories in children than adults.
Unbiased leading questions such as “Did the bicycle belong to (a) the mother, (b) the boy, or (c) the girl?” were as likely to produce false memories as biased (mis)leading questions such as “The mother owned the bike, didn’t she?”
This is an important result, as it suggests that the mechanisms that result in false memories may be general mechanisms to do with the way that the developing memory system functions, rather than specific mechanisms related to false memories of negative events.
False reports of abuse did not increase when anatomically detailed dolls were provided to enable the children to show as well as tell what had happened.
Younger children were more susceptible to leading questions than the older children. Under the influence of misleading questions about abuse, the three-year-olds tended to make errors of commission (that is, they agreed to things that had not happened) 20% of the time.
Overall, Eisen et al. did not find that memory or suggestibility differed in maladjusted children compared to typically-developing children. Abuse status was not related to the children’s eye-witness memory per- formance
In general, more accurate memory was shown by the older children, and by the more intelligent children, while less accurate memory was shown by children rated as having poor global adaptive functioning.Thus age, IQ and overall psychopathology rather than abuse status was linked to children’s eye-witness memory performance.
The eye-witness testimony of young children is in general highly accurate. However, children provide more detailed memories when the questioner has taken time to build rapport and uses open-ended questions.
Responses to misleading questions were also largely accurate. Children in both age groups were able correctly to reject misleading features most of the time, with correct denials on 60% of misleading questions for the three-year-olds after a three- week delay, and on 65% of misleading questions for the six-year-olds. Intrusions (‘remembering’ features that had not in fact occurred) were also at similar levels in the two groups after the three-week delay, being 26% for the three-year-olds and 32% for the six-year-olds. Ornstein et al. concluded that young children’s recall of a personally experienced event was surprisingly good.
Levels of suggestibility are higher in younger children the effects of misleading questions are usually to increase inaccurate acquiescence (errors of commission).
Overall, the levels of suggestibility found in different studies appear to vary with factors such as the emotional tone of the interview itself, the child’s desire to please the interviewer, characteristics of both interviewer and child, and whether the child is a participant in the action or not, among others.Almost all studies find some age differences in suggestibility. However, it is also worth noting that leading questions are more likely to result in new disclosures than neutral questions are (Gilstrap & Ceci, 2005). Hence leading questions cannot be dismissed as overall deleterious in eye-witness memory investigations with young children; indeed asking specific questions (rather than misleading questions) can aid younger children’s recall.
Links Between the Development of Episodic Memory and the Development of Eye-Witness Memory
Older children can generally provide more detailed and narratively coherent memories. Ceci and Bruck also suggested that the greater susceptibility of younger children to repeated questioning by adults might be related to the distinction between scripts and personal histories.
Ceci and Bruck (1993) suggested that the over-dependency of younger children on scripted knowledge could mean that suggestions made by the experimenter get included into the children’s script for an event, and are thereafter reported as having actually taken place.
The suggestibility of younger children seems to reduce their report accuracy rather than change their memories. Because younger children sometimes agree with misleading questions, their reports contain more errors. As children get older,they seem to become less susceptible to leading questions and get better at providing narrative detail. In general, the amount of information and the accuracy of the information that children report in a memory interview increases with age, mirroring developments in episodic memory skills (Peterson, 2012).
The researchers found that the correlations between knowledge and memory were highly significant at each delay interval. From this finding, they argued that variability in knowledge in a given domain is associated with corresponding variability in recall.
Eisen et al. (2002) reported that in their study of maltreated children, overall the children with better event memories were also the children who provided more detail in their reports of abuse experiences. Girls also tended to provide more detailed disclosures than boys.This gender difference could reflect the role of verbal ability in constructing coherent narratives of one’s personal history.We also saw in the autobiographical memory section of this chapter that females tend to have earlier memories than males.
The Development of Stratergies for Remembering
strategies that we develop to organize and store information to facilitate its recall later on (i.e., rehearsal)
memory development includes stratergies and capacity (memory span)
Regarding memory strategies, younger children are typically surprisingly confident about their mnemonic abilities.They do not seem to expect that they will need to use mnemonic strategies to improve their recall. Errors on memory tasks doesn’t shake their confidence in being able to do it without remembering stratergies.
most children used a variety of strategies to help them to remember which cup was the dog- house.They looked at and touched the cup hiding the dog significantly more often than the other cups, they looked at the target cup and nodded to themselves “yes”, looked at the other cups and shook their heads “no”, they rested their hand on the target cup, and so on. Wellman et al. also found that recall for the dog’s location was more successful in the children who used these strategies than in the children who didn’t.
The children were told that Big Bird was going to hide, and that they should remember where he was hiding as they would need to find him later when the bell rang.A timer was then set for four minutes, during which time the child took part in a number of distraction activities with other toys, organised by the experimenter.The children frequently checked on Big Bird’s hiding location during this distraction period, for example pointing at the pillow, saying “Big Bird!”, and peeping underneath it. In a control condition in which Big Bird was put on top of the pillow, similar strategies were not observed. DeLoache et al. argued that this showed that the children’s self-reminding behaviours were indeed strategic, as they were adopted as a function of the memory demands of the task.
Evidence for the Strategic use of Organisation by Semantic Category
Organisational mnemonic strategies, such as sorting required grocery items into related groups and using this clustering to aid recall, show a similar developmental pattern to rehearsal. Early strategic use is largely task-driven, and depends on the items to be recalled. Later strategic use is child-driven, and occurs independently of the materials to be remembered.
Schneider argued that the use of organisational strategies in younger children depended on the degree to which the items were associated. For the seven-year-olds, high associativity in itself led to the use of clustering, in a largely involuntary way. In contrast, the ten-year-olds used clustering as a deliberate strategy.The older children were apparently becoming aware of the value of organisational strategies as a mnemonic.
However, when the experimenters asked the children how they went about remembering their classmates’ names, the children were unable to outline particular strategies, suggesting that their use of clustering was involuntary.
They found that recall in this strategic condition was equiva- lent to recall in the free condition, when no instructions were given. Bjorklund and Bjorklund concluded that semantic associations between highly associated items can be activated with little effort, resulting in retrieval that appears to be organised and strategic when in fact it is simply a byproduct of high associativity. High associativity thus automatically guides the structure of recall.
The Development of Multiple Stratergies
Research has also investigated whether young children use more than one strategy for remembering at a time,and whether the use of multiple strategies can benefit children’s recall.
In general, such research finds that children who use more strategies do seem to recall more information.
Reported that even the youngest children used more than one strategy for remembering. Older children used more strategies overall than younger children. The number of strategies used across tasks was significantly but modestly correlated, for younger and for older children. . Finally, strategy use was strongly correlated with successful recall. Memory capacity was also measured in this study (using span tasks), and memory capacity did not predict successful recall. DeMarie and Ferron concluded that strategy use by young children was an important factor in memory development.
Schneider et al. reported that both strategy use and memory capacity increased over time. Strategy use was related to better recall, and children who used more strategies did particularly well.
They concluded that memory development is characterised by a rapid transition from non-strategic to strategic behaviour for most children. Memory development does not consist of a gradual increase in strategy use. Children who were consistently strategic used strategies in more than one test of memory, and the advantages of multiple strategy use were already evident in kindergarten children.
The Novice–Expert Distinction
• The other traditional answers to the question of ‘what is memory development the development of?’ are knowledge and metamemory.
• Knowledge can also be conceptualised as expertise. It is now widely accepted that the knowledge base itself plays an important role in memory efficiency, and that experts organise their knowledge in different ways to novices.The level of prior knowledge has a critical impact on both the encoding and the storage of incoming information. Expertise also affects the efficiency of recall.
• One of the most interesting approaches to studying the influence of prior know- ledge on cognitive development has been to contrast the performance of novices in a domain, who have little prior knowledge, with that of experts, who have a lot of prior knowledge.As such comparisons are usually confounded with age (novices are usually younger than experts), the most developmentally informative contrasts are those in which the experts are younger than the novices. Such contrasts are usu- ally only possible in quite circumscribed domains, for example chess playing, soccer playing and expertise in physics.
• As Brown and DeLoache (1978) once called young children ‘universal novices’, it may come as a surprise to find that young children occasionally display more expertise in circumscribed domains than older children and adults.This can occur because experts and novices in a particular domain are distinguished by differences in experience as well as by differences in age. If you are motivated enough, it is pos- sible to gain a lot of experience in a domain at a relatively young age.
• Domain so interesting that they become veritable experts in dinosaur classification or chess and can hold more expertise than adults.
• Chi (1978)
o Examined the factors that distinguished the mem- ories of chess experts and chess novices. Her group of experts were children aged from six to ten years, and her group of novices were graduate students who could all play chess. Chi measured the memory of both groups for ‘middle game’ chess positions, which involved on average 22 chess pieces.The chess players were allowed to study the chess board for ten seconds, and were then expected to recreate the middle game position from memory. Chi found that the children positioned 9.3 chess pieces accurately on the first trial, compared to 5.9 chess pieces for the adults. She then measured how long it took both groups to learn the entire middle game position.The children took on average 5.6 trials, and the adults 8.4 trials. Expert versus novice performance was significantly different in each case.
o In a replication of Chi’s study which included additional control tasks (Schneider, Gruber, Gold & Opwis, 1993b; see Figure 8.6). Schneider et al. found that recall for random as well as meaningful chess positions was better in experts than in novices. In the control task, in which the position of geometrically shaped wooden pieces had to be reconstructed on a board that did not resemble a chess board, the effect of expertise was eliminated.These findings suggest that expertise involves both qualita- tive differences in the way that knowledge is represented and quantitative differences in the amount of knowledge available.The latter would in this case include know- ledge about the geometrical pattern of the chess board and the form and colour of chess pieces.
o The data on children’s dinosaur expertise also suggests that experts structure their knowledge in qualitatively different ways from novices. Chi, Hutchinson and Robin (1988) found that experts organised their knowledge about dinosaurs in more integrated and locally coherent ways than novices. Their knowledge was more coherent at a global level, representing superordinate information such as ‘meat eater’ versus ‘plant eater’, and also at a sub-structural level, representing infor- mation about shared attributes such as ‘has sharp teeth’ or ‘has a duckbill’.
o Chi and her colleagues pointed out that it was easier to understand the attributes of a dinosaur (sharp teeth, webbed feet, etc.) if one knew how they were related in a causal or correlated structure. The importance of causal relations and relational mappings for conceptual development and knowledge acquisition should already be familiar from previous chapters.
o Expertise may also be more important for memory performance than general cognitive ability, at least when a particular domain like chess or soccer is the object of study. expertise was a stronger predictor of performance than general cognitive ability, regardless of age or IQ.
o ‘practice makes perfect’ does capture something important about the development of expertise.
o Overall, studies of novices and experts show that expertise plays a crucial role in the organisation of memory.The idea that knowledge enrichment leads to the reorganisation of memory has some obvious parallels with Fekete’s (2010) metaphor about wine tasting by novices versus connoisseurs. Fekete argued that via repeated learning experiences, or acquired expertise, the represen- tation of the environment in the brain is irrevocably transformed, so that the connoisseur finds intricacies of flavour that are not available to the novice, even though they are tasting the same wine.
• Anyone can become an expert in certain domains if they are motivated enough to learn about them. For the brain, learning and memory are two sides of the same coin. Memory development thus depends on the depth of the knowledge base as well as on the use of explicit strategies such as rehearsal. High levels of expertise can even compensate for low levels of general intelligence in some memory tasks, such as recall tasks. The storage components of memory thus play a clear role in individual differences. It was argued that these developmental changes in epi- sodic memory may in turn explain some of the developments seen in other memory systems, such as the decrease in suggestibility found in eye-witness memory and the decline in ‘infantile amnesia’.
other
language helps in rehearsing one’s own experiences or in recounting them to someone else, and these verbal narratives help to order memories temporally and establish them more firmly.
Abstract knowledge structures such as scripts for describing the temporal and causal sequences of events depend in part on language development, but language is not our only symbolic system. Words stand for or represent concepts and events in the everyday world, and of course we use them
as symbols to encode our experiences. However, we also use other symbols to encode and communicate our experiences, such as pictorial ones. These symbols also represent or stand for objects or events, and include drawings, photographs and sculptures. All of these symbols bring to mind something other than themselves. Children, too, use a number of symbolic systems in addition to language, for example in communication.Young children make gestures, they point to things and they engage in symbolic (pretend) play. They also use culturally determined symbols.These include symbols such as maps and models.The use of many of these forms of symbolic coding enables children to represent information in memory in a form that will be accessible later on.
What is receptive and expressive language?
Receptive language refers to how your child understands language. Expressive language refers to how your child uses words to express himself/herself.
As originally demonstrated by Bartlett (1932), children and adults construct memories, and the process of construction depends on prior knowledge and personal interpretation. It also depends on how much sense the mem- oriser can make of the temporal structure of their experiences.Very young children, for example, may not structure their experience in memorable ways, particularly if they do not understand particular experiences (e.g., being born, someone dying, being sexually abused), or if they do not have a clear tem-
poral framework.Very young children are also still acquiring language,and language itself is important for memory. For example, language helps in rehearsing one’s own experiences or in recounting them to someone else, and these verbal narratives help to order memories temporally and establish them more firmly.As with the devel- opment of theory of mind, the development of memory clearly cannot be isolated from the development of other cognitive processes. Remembering is embedded in larger social and cognitive activities.Thus the knowledge structures and learning skills that young children bring to their experiences are likely to be a critical factor in explaining memory development.
other
language helps in rehearsing one’s own experiences or in recounting them to someone else, and these verbal narratives help to order memories temporally and establish them more firmly.
Abstract knowledge structures such as scripts for describing the temporal and causal sequences of events depend in part on language development, but language is not our only symbolic system. Words stand for or represent concepts and events in the everyday world, and of course we use them
as symbols to encode our experiences. However, we also use other symbols to encode and communicate our experiences, such as pictorial ones. These symbols also represent or stand for objects or events, and include drawings, photographs and sculptures. All of these symbols bring to mind something other than themselves. Children, too, use a number of symbolic systems in addition to language, for example in communication. Young children make gestures, they point to things and they engage in symbolic (pretend) play. They also use culturally determined symbols.These include symbols such as maps and models. The use of many of these forms of symbolic coding enables children to represent information in memory in a form that will be accessible later on.
What is receptive and expressive language?
Receptive language refers to how your child understands language. Expressive language refers to how your child uses words to express himself/herself.
As originally demonstrated by Bartlett (1932), children and adults construct memories, and the process of construction depends on prior knowledge and personal interpretation. It also depends on how much sense the memoriser can make of the temporal structure of their experiences. Very young children, for example, may not structure their experience in memorable ways, particularly if they do not understand particular experiences (e.g., being born, someone dying, being sexually abused), or if they do not have a clear temporal framework. Very young children are also still acquiring language, and language itself is important for memory. For example, language helps in rehearsing one’s own experiences or in recounting them to someone else, and these verbal narratives help to order memories temporally and establish them more firmly. As with the development of theory of mind, the development of memory clearly cannot be isolated from the development of other cognitive processes. Remembering is embedded in larger social and cognitive activities. Thus the knowledge structures and learning skills that young children bring to their experiences are likely to be a critical factor in explaining memory development.
Conducting Successful Memory Interviews with Children
Zajac & Brown, 2017
To conduct successful formal interviews with children the interviewer must consider the child’s developmental capacities and vulnerabilities, set aside expectations from everyday conversations with children, consider what questions are asked and how they are asked which influence children’s responses (amount and quality).
To achieve this, it requires constant evaluation, self-reflection, and professional development.
A myriad of factors influence whether they provide information, how much and its accuracy:
1. Context (in which they’re questioned)
2. Topic (of the questions)
3. Child (developmental factors)
4. Questions (what and how the questions
are asked)
context
Formality: the location, clothing, attempts of rapport and the language the interviewer uses influence how children respond and how they perceive the interviewer.
Children are socialized from a very young age that adults are knowledgeable in conversations, to rely on adults to direct the conversation, reinforce their responses and engrain them with the severity of the situation when talking to professionals.
(A) The child’s unique role as an informant
In contrast, to everyday conversations the child is now the person with the expertise and knowledge about an event, a role reversal, which leads to two main problems:
Fail to recognize that the interviewer doesn’t have the same knowledge that they do and therefore what “kind” of information they need to understand their experience. They also tend to provide brief accurate summaries of events rather than going into detail because this have what they’ve been taught is socially conventional. They may be reluctant to correct an interviewer when they are wrong due to previous expectations that adults are more knowledgeable than them.
Used to their interactions with adults being taught and then tested. They are used to adult directed conversations with positive reinforcement when they answer a question directly. This may be reflected in their tendency to not respond to a difficult question with IDK or I don’t remember (especially when questions are phrased in a yes or no format) which are ways we can see the boundaries of their memory and ensure we do not elicit an inaccurate account.
Solution:
Is to set out ground rules before questioning begins.
For them to be effective they need to be practiced and reinforced (i.e., ask them difficult/unanswerable questions on an unrelated topic to see if they say IDK or reminding them on the ground rules throughout the interview and reinforce them when they say IDK).
(B) Misgivings and Misunderstandings about
procedure
Children can be uncomfortable with the manner in which they are asked questions in a formal setting.
Frustrated with being interviewed or being asked the same questions over and over again.
May not understand legal proceedings or hold misconceptions (i.e., that being interviewed means they’re in trouble, that they cannot say IDK or disagree with the interviewer).
Solution: always assume children are naïve to legal proceedings and the reason for the interview.
(C) High Stakes:
Children may be aware about the severity of the interview and understand that their answers have consequences which may influence their willingness to answer questions. For various reasons:
o Fear of retribution
o Involves loved ones
o Feelings of complicity, embarrassment or
shame
o Fear they will not be believed
o Worries about family disruption.
- Topic
The subject of the event children need to describe can influence what they recall in various ways.
(A) Knowledge
Events they understand are better encoded, organised and later recalled.
When events are unfamiliar or not well understood children can pull out pieces of information from the event which are familiar and replace the gaps with “knowledge” from past events, experiences or stereotypes.
(B) Salience
They salience of information about an event for children will be different to what is considered important or interesting for adults.
Salient information is subjective to the child and easier to recall later.
(C) Stress
Memory processes influence all types of memory equally (stressful or not; salient or not), they all suffer memory decay or recall issues.
Do not presume which events would be more stressful or distressing for a child or how this may impact their memory! Their naivety will lessen the salience of stressful events they do not understand.
Conversely, do not underestimate the stress experienced by children in mundane events or activities.
(D) Multiple Events
Specific details of repeated events can be harder to recall because children develop scripts (what typically happens in a category of events) which makes it harder to differentiate similarities and differences between specific events.
Solution:
Begin line of questioning broadly about scripts to generate information and spread activation.
Then ask them to choose a specific instance which they recall very well and get them to discuss that event with you.
Then ask about the differences and similarities between that event and what usually happens (script).
(E) Delay
Time between event and interview can reduce the accuracy of information recalled.
In forensic settings children may be asked to recall smaller events which lead up to the main event months or years prior.
Delays: reduce the amount and quality of information recalled (especially without retrieval cues) and the more likely their accounts will be biased by external cues (other people’s input etc.) or internal cues (imagining what might have happened).
Delays provide time for children to worry about the upcoming interview which will influence their willingness to talk and share information with the interviewer.
Child
Age
Most reliable indicator interviewers have on what type of questions children can answer.
The developmental stage of the child influences the quality and quantity of information they can recall.
Younger children recall fewer items of lower quality than older children. However, older children can make more memory errors because they can use their superior knowledge to fill in gaps of their memory with other information.
These are general guides which do not tell us about the specific skills or abilities of a child. Developmental skills within and between age groups is highly variable.
It is impossible to answer questions like “at what age can children…” because the answer is “it depends on…”.
Age allows us to collate the number of influences which impact on the information children can recall and how this can vary across contexts; not their ability to recall information.
Not children in contact with forensic services will differ importantly from typically developing children! Delayed relative to normal children but when matched on intelligence and asked age appropriate questions can provide similar information to their typically developing peers
(B) Memory Ability
Memory is NOT a recording device:
Recording (encoding) of events is good but not perfect.
Information which is encoded is influenced by individuals motivations, expectations and biases at the time of encoding.
It depends on how events are perceived and interpreted.
Not all information is encoded into memory and what does varies in strength.
Influenced by stress, attention, emotion, perceived importance, knowledge, goals, expectations and amount of cognitive resources available.
Memory is NOT a playback of events:
Memory is reconstructed and influenced by internal and external factors and the time of recall.
We piece together information and fill in the gaps.
Influenced by the strength of the encoded information, spreading of activation, retrieval cues and strategies’ used and the cognitive resources left over influences the amount and quality of information recalled.
Three factors which make children prone to memory errors:
o Limited understanding of the events they experience influences what is encoded and later recalled. It makes them prone to misinterpretations of events and magical thinking (blaming events on their own thoughts or actions).
o Children are less likely to use effective retrieval strategies’. Which strategy is used, how efficiently it is used influence how much and what is recalled. These develop with experience and become automatic overtime. Thus, younger children can use the same retrieval strategy as older children but still recall less information. Practice (age) also makes them less reliant on interviewers for retrieval cues and reduce chances of contamination.
o Source memory errors: where children struggle to recall where their information came from. They mix up sources and recall information from other sources then their own experience and incorporate it into their recalled memory.