Module 3 Flashcards

1
Q

Infants’ responses suggestive of higher-level cognitive understandings.
But how much of that understanding is psychological understanding?

§ That infants understand the self and properties of
the self?
§ That infants understand that others act purposefully;
that agents take certain steps to achieve certain
goals, and context can gives clues to what actions
are relevant and irrelevant to those agents’ goals?

To answer this question we will look at …

A

To answer this question we will look at evidence that psychological understanding is available early in life, well before language gives it a boost.

Earlier findings we know of in the mobile task which demonstrates that infants can track causal contingencies in their environment. The link is still between the self-movement of their physical bodies and a visually rewarding object. Meaning in its current form it doesn’t tell us about whether infants discriminate what pertains to their own bodies, and what pertains to non-self-entities (i.e., that the self has unique characteristics).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Are there forms of evidence that infants have self-other awareness? At least 3.

A

(A) Own-Body Perception in Infancy
Bahrick & Watson (1985)

   	Steps: §	3 month and 5-month-old infants were seated on a 
    chair, and they were shown the camera output of two 
    videos; 1) showed a video of their feet moving in real 
    time(contingency/synchrony); 2) showed a video of 
    their feet moving with delayed reactions (imperfect 
    contingency). Which video will infants prefer to look 
    at?
    Results:  §	Longer looking to non-contingent display of self- 
    amongst 5-month-olds (delayed unusual video 
    feedback), as compared to 3-month-olds (younger 
    prefer to watch contingent display, matches).
    Implications: §	Self-perception of body can be seen in the way 
    infants detect a link between proprioceptive 
    information for motion and visual display of that 
    motion §	Younger infants prefer stability from cross-modal 
    information to perceive the world as a consistent and 
    stable place. Older infants slowly become more and 
    more interested in novelty. §	Since both groups are able to discriminate between 
    the two visual images (show a preference) this tells us 
    they are able to make judgements of the self’s 
    properties which they can then relate to the visual 
    input in their environment.
  • self-body perception is available within the first year of
    life.

(B) Mirror Self-Recognition
Recognition of contingency between own action and
video feedback (self-image reactions) suggests that
infants should also be capable of mirror self-
recognition

Humans
Amsterdam (1972)
The Mark or Rouge Test

Q: Does the child, whilst looking in mirror, notices and
touches red mark on face/nose?

Steps:
§ Researchers put a secret red mark on the infants face
and then they’re placed in front of a mirror to see if
they will notice the mark, realize that it is their
reflection, and explicitly try to rub the mark off their
own face.
§ To pass the mirror test, infants need to touch their
nose where the red mark was placed to indicate that
they have EXPLICIT self-recognition (requires an
objective direct response).
Results:
§ There is a robust age-related pattern in how they
respond to the mirror and the mark on their face:
- 3-5-mths: look at mirror and treat image as playmate
(curious about what they see in the mirror, but they
still treat their reflection as another person; don’t
touch the mark).
- 18-20-mths: 42% touch mark on own face (less than
half touch their face)
- 21-24-mths: 63% touch mark on own face (more than
half will reliably and consistently touch their face;
explicit mirror self-recognition is in place by 2 years of
age).
- Has been replicated with all the great apes which all
seem to pass the test.

Non-Human Animals
Non-human animals in complex social groupings (apes, dolphins, certain birds like magpies, elephants) show some mirror recognition.

Plotnik et al. (2006) on Asian Elephants
- Elephants, happy, patty and Maxine.

Do they show:
(A) Physical inspection of the mirror? Yes, they all
touched it and checked behind it using their trunks.
(B) Repetitive mirror testing behaviour? Yes, all made
non-species typical trunk and body movements to
see if their reflection moved in the same way.
(C) Self-directed mirror behaviour? No, only happy
passed the mark test, tried to rub off the white x
painted on their face. She only did this when she was
in front of the mirror and not anywhere else.
Showing it is a very specific behaviour she is
demonstrating.

Implications:
§ Only 1/3 elephants passed the mark test indicating
there is some degree of self-awareness present.
§ It is challenging to interpret negative effects (2/3 not
passing the test) because absence of evidence
doesn’t mean evidence of absence (the use of
grooming behaviours of the trunk as the behavioural
approach to passing the test but in real life they really
do this behaviour, they’re more likely to throw dust or
debris on themselves with their trunk) this behavioural
measure may have underestimated elephants self-
awareness. Other authors which use a more naturalist
mode of behaviour for the specific non-human group
they work with found significantly more success,
cleaner fish are able to pass the rouge test. This
indicates that self-awareness is not zero sum all (have
or don’t have; not a discontinuous cognitive
accomplishment only in humans) it is more gradual
and continuous in nature where different groups of
species have different degrees of self-awareness.

(C) Detecting adult contingent behaviours
Infants as young as three months of age are sensitive to
and can discriminate when adults are making
contingent actions upon, rather than ignorant of, infants
themselves. Infants seem to realise when adults
treating them as social partners and prefer it when
adults do this.

Striano et al. (2005)
3-month-olds

Steps
(A) Normal interaction (visit 1 control)
§ Mother and infants arrived at the lab and the
researcher’s recorded audio of their normal
interactions.
§ One-week later infants and their mothers came back
and were randomly assigned to the non-contingent or
imitation contingent group.
(B) Non-contingent (visit 2 interaction)
§ Parents wore a pair of headphones and were
instructed to press play on the audio recorder and to
repeat the audio of what they had said to their infant
in visit 1. The researcher insisted that the parent
repeats what they said in the first interaction back to
the infant (interaction between parent and infant non-
contingent).
(C) Imitation contingent (visit 2 interaction)
§ Parents were instructed to perfectly mirror infants’
facial gesture, arm movements and vocalisations (if
the infant smiled, cooed etc.). Would 3 months olds
show differential reactions between the two groups
(discriminate)?
Results:
§ 3-month-olds gazed more intently (sustained gazing;
more dilation) in imitation interaction condition. Infants
are able to pick up in conversations if they’re being
treated as social agents, being responded to and
mimicked and prefer this (self-other awareness;
harmony in what they do and what is reflected back to
them = contingent, my action causes your action).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

These three stands of evidence encourage us to consider what mechanisms might support social psychological understanding of others?

(A) & (B)

A

(A) Contingency detection
§ Being sensitive to situations in which one’s
behaviour is followed in time by another stimulus
event (timing of events; immediate vs delayed) or
being sensitive to how much energy someone puts
into an instrumental act and the consequences
obtained could be important.
§ Preference for or sensitivity to certain contingencies
can allow infants to realise that their own behaviours
can exert control over others’ behaviours (harmony
in what they are doing and what they see is
reflected back to them; by changing what they do it
influences what is reflected back to them; my action
causes your action, your action causes my action).
(B) Imitation
§ Mapping actions of other people onto actions of
one’s own bodies (like me hypothesis that my
behaviour is goal orientated therefore other actions
must also be purposeful; self-other psychology
awareness) is also important.
§ Imitation can give insight into what it might internally
be like to those actions, and in so doing give a
glimpse into the minds of others.

*both mechanisms ensure the maintenance of proximity
with caregivers and facilitate attachment bonds (infant-
parent; ensure they learn the required skills to reach
sexual maturity and be functional social beings able to
be harmonious with other beings in their
social or cultural group).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Q: when we (infants and adults) observe others, we assume that other actions are directed to outcomes (that the actor is acting to achieve CERTAIN goals). But so far, the evidence is suggestive.

A
  • Yes, infants can imitate another person’s action upon
    an object even when the action is not seen (i.e.,
    completed) but how can we be sure that infant’s re-
    enactment to ‘pull apart toy with hands’ because it
    done because it is viewed as the most effective way
    to achieve the goal?
  • We only have evidence of the first part of
    teleological framework; purposeful-action not
    evidence of efficiency.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Are there imitation studies that can provide more direct evidence that infants only re-enact an action if they perceive it to be the most effective means to achieve a goal?

Direct evidence (or stronger evidence) using imitation task that infants view actions of others as goal-directed (infants regard other actions in a more careful way as being goal oriented).

Light-box study: Gergely et al. (2002)

A

Light-box study: Gergely et al. (2002)
14-month-olds

Steps
§ Infants were randomly assigned to one of two
conditions: 1) Hands Free condition: There is a light
box and lamp that can be activates on the table in
front of the infant. The experimenter comes in and
sits in front of them with a cloth around their
shoulder. They place their hands on the table and
leans forward to turn the light box on with their
forehead (unusual action being modelled). 2) hands
occupied: the experimenter comes in complaining
that they’re cold, wraps themselves in a blanket with
the hands occupied and turns the lightbox on with
their forehead.
§ To what extent will 14-month-old infants imitate this
unusual behaviour? Will they regard the action
(turning light panel on with forehead) as having a
different meaning in the two conditions?

Results:
§ In hands occupied condition, majority of infants in
this condition turn the light on with their hand
instead of imitating the behaviour (using their
forehead).
§ In the hands-free condition, majority of the infants
imitated that unusual behaviour (turn light on with
forehead).

Implications:
§ Fit with teleological reasoning:
§ Infants assume agents choose the most efficient
means to ends.
§ In hands-occupied condition, agent’s hands hold
blanket and the goal is to turn on light, with
whatever means available to her ( infants pick up on
the goal and perceive the actor to be rational under
constraint and when imitating see no need to copy
turning light on specifically with head because
they’re not constrained and can complete the action
in the most efficient way possible- with their hand).
§ In hands-free condition, agent’s hands are free so
goal must be to turn light on specifically using head
– so copy by turning on light with head (if there
hands are free and the actor is choosing to use the
inefficient method so they assume there must be a
reason for why they use their head so they imitate
it).
§ Infants think about someone’s goal transforms the
way infants perceive their bodily motions as being
purposeful behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

The Debate: Lightbox Study

A

§ Interpretation 1: perceptual saliency and not
teleological perspective
- The blanket completely covered upper body, and is
eye-catching, and this feature may be like a
perceptual distractor which competes with the
head-touch action, drawing the 14-mth-olds’ infant’s
attention away from it (there is a perceptual
difference between the two conditions; the hands
occupied condition having the blanket cover the
upper part of the body is very eye-catching, is
salient enough to cause infants to be distracted and
are not focusing on their forehead movement
relative to when their hands were on the table in the
hands free condition; blanket less salient means
they attend more to the unusual action and thus
more likely to imitate the behaviour).
§ Interpretation 2:
- Differential imitation of same action because of
changes depending on the context which provides
strong evidence that infants understand that others’
actions are goal-directed (teleological view of other
people’s social actions).

Solution

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Solutions to the Debate: Lightbox Study

A

Solution
To rule out the alternative explanation that the effects in the light box study are due to perceptual saliency.
Consider Bisert et al.’s (2012) test of perceptual distraction as shown in picture C (their ‘Hands-free with extra distraction’ condition). They added an additional condition, a “hands free with extra distraction condition” where at the beginning of the experimental phase, the experimenter put a blanket loosely around her shoulders, and then put two smiling faces to left and right of table.
Results:
• Hands occupied: most infants used their hands to
turn on lamp.
• Hands free: most infants imitated the unusual action
to turn the lamp on with their head.
• Hands free with distraction: They found that 50%
imitate and 50% do not imitate the head touch action
(chance levels). Being able to demolish the effect by
adding a distractor in the hands-free condition,
according to Bisert, supports that the original
findings of the lightbox study were due to perceptual
salience being a distractor from imitating social
behaviour (its relatively dumb and not a
sophisticated mental inference about teleological
goal and efficiency).

Alternative Explanation:
§ Does the light-box study show a cognitive effect or a
motor effect?

Remember:
- Hands occupied condition: agent’s arms folded
across chest
- Hands free condition: agent puts hands on table
when performing head touch

Consider this:
- Notice that when infants perform a head touch
themselves, they put their hands on the table next to
the light-box – to maintain a stable position. This
action motorically matches closely the movement of
adult in the hands free but not in the hands occupied
condition (infants imitate the action sequence in a
motor sense rather than being evidence of
sophisticated cognitive reasoning where actors are
goal orientated and efficient, they are actually
focusing on the motor components. Monkey see
monkey do. I will imitate the behaviour as long as it
is within my physical capabilities).
- = motor driving the effect matching visual of others
with own motor capabilities (hands-occupied is too
difficult, being able to lean forward and turn light on
head is not possible in their current motor stage
without hands on the table to stabilize them).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Given challenges with light-box method, how else can we study that infants show differential imitation of same action when context changes (how infants decide what to imitate and what not to imitate)?

  • Yes, use method by Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello
    (2005) : Mouse in the House
A

Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello (2005):

Steps:
1. 12–18-month-old infants were assigned to one of two
conditions.
2. In House condition, Infants were placed in front of a
table with two toy houses and a mouse on the table.
The goal is to put the mouse in the house but the
experimenter made movements (hopping or sliding
motion with sound affects) into the house.
3. In No-House condition, there was no house on the
table. The experimenter would make the movements
and sound affects as the mouse moved around the
table but there was no house at the final location.
4. Did infants (12-18-mths) copy the hopping or sliding
action between the conditions? Is it relevant in some
conditions and irrelevant in others?

Results:
 In House condition, infants imitate by copying the
final goal (putting straight into house) and did not
copy the hopping or sliding.
 In No-House condition, infants copied the motion
and movements (and even sound effects); these
actions themselves treated as goal.
 Infants’ action processing is flexible. They notice the
context behaviour is modelled in and use contextual
cues to determine what aspects of the action stream
are relevant to imitate and what is irrelevant to the
actor’s goal. For example, in the no house condition
infants assume since there is no end goal location
the experimenter has purposefully done the motion
and sound affects for a reason or relevant to the
task. In the other goal, to get into the house is the
end goal and the motion doesn’t matter so they do
not imitate it = flexible.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Studying infants’ responses to accidental actions is also a useful way of investigating flexible understanding of goal-directed actions (how flexible are infants ability to pick up on goal directed actions)

Carpenter et al. (1998):

A

Carpenter et al. (1998):

Do infants (14-18-mths) discriminate between Purposeful (relevant to goal) and Accidental (irrelevant to goal) actions when reproducing others’ actions.

Steps
 Watched adults produce 2-step actions that produce
a result; sometimes an action is intentional “There!”,
and some vocally marked as accidental “Whoops!”
 Infants watched adults interact with a blue wooden
box with a handle to get a toy. The trick is to pull the
handle to get the toy and the wheel is irrelevant to
their goal. They watched adults complete a 2-step
sequence which was either rational (pull handle
“there” spoken) and irrational (touch wheel
“whoops” spoken to indicate its irrelevant to goal).
Results:
 Infants more likely to copy purposeful rather than
accidental actions. Shows infants screen out
meaningless actions (discard information which is
irrelevant to the current goal).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Summary:

1.

A

 Propensity for social interaction may be partly very
early-developing (part innate basis).
 Infants, for example, notice contingencies between
their own bodies and the environment, and will soon
go on to explicitly pass the mirror test of self-
recognition. If infants understand that their actions
are directed to outcomes/goals, and if they treat
others as “like me” and worthwhile imitating, then –
by analogy – should also understand that others’
actions are directed to outcomes/goals.
 Evidence for goal-understanding is diverse but also
challenging to interpret. Nonetheless, scientists are
super interested because infants’ capacity to
understanding goals is a significant marker of infants’
insight into the psychologies, the minds, of human
agents.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Recap:
14–18-month-old infants are able to exploit context to identify which action are most relevant to them in an action stream to imitate. These tasks rely on some degree of motor skills to be able to complete the task. Is there a technique which doesn’t rely on infants having certain motor skills and will allow us to see if the effect applies to younger infants (teleologically; goal directed).

Technique:
Woodward (1998)

A

Technique:
Visual Habituation as a measure of young infants’ perception of actions as goal directed.

Woodward (1998)

  1. Habituate
    - A ball and bear are placed on the table in front of
    the infant. There are then habituated to a repeated
    action sequence (e.g., a hand reaching for the teddy
    bear on the left side of the screen).
    - Infants are either shown the new goal trial or the
    new side trail.
  2. New goal trial:
    - We disrupt agent-object contingent relationship
    (new goal), but preserve motion. For example, the
    agent still reaches for an object on the left side of
    the table which in now the ball (e.g., ball and bear
    were swapped).
  3. New side trial: in this condition the relationship
    between the agent and object is preserve
    preserved, but we disrupt motion e.g., agent grasps
    bear (same) on right side (new). Which of these trails
    will infants pay more attention to?
  4. Results:
    - 6-9-month-olds show selective recovery to new goal
    trials (dishabituation to new goal/end-state; reach for
    the ball instead of the bear).

*the benefit of this paradigm is that we can introduce
control conditions to be really clear that infants are able
to distinguish between inanimate (machine) or animate
objects (humans). Primarily that humans are more likely
have goal-directed action relative to inanimate objects.

Woodward (1998)
Control condition (human hand vs machine claw)
  1. Habituation:
    - 6-month-old infants were shown the hand reach for
    the object (hand or claw) on the left side of the
    screen till they’re bored (habituate).
  2. Test:
    (A) New Side Trial
    - In the new side trial infants are shown the agent
    reach for the same bear but on the other side of the
    table (same goal; different action).
    (B) New “Goal” Trial
    - In the new gal trial infants are shown the agent
    reaching for the ball (new goal) on the same side of
    the table (same action).
  3. Results:
    - Hand condition: infants look longer (dishabituate) to
    the new goal trial (same action new goal).
    - Claw condition: infants look longer (dishabituate) to
    the new side trial (same action but different goal;
    directional properties temporal spatiotemporal
    relationships; where in space is the claw moving to).
    - Infants treat hand and claws different. They’re more
    likely to attribute goals to animate action rather than
    inanimate action (i.e., perceive actions as goal
    orientated).

Note: In Woodward (1998), the ending is revealed to infants (visual information

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

If infants are tracking and generating predictions about the goal of an impending action then in the absence of visual information (conclusion or outcome shown) infants should be able to anticipate or predict what the agents action will be (before it does it; via eye tracking).

Kim & Song (2015)

A

Kim & Song (2015)

Steps:
1. Habituation:
- 6-month-olds sit behind a table with a red cylinder
(right) and a blue triangle/conical object (left), the
agent reaches repeatedly the blue triangular object,
2. Test Trial:
- In the test trail, the objects positions (blue left; red
right). The question is will infants, before the agent
reaches for an object, eyes follow the blue object
and remain there (predict that the goal is the same
and anticipate agents will reach for the blue object).
3. Results:
- 6-month-old infants selectively place their attention
to the goal outcome object before the agent moves
to reach for it.

*findings compliment Woodward’s (1998) study

Note:

All we can say from Woodwards study is that infants are able to track the agents’ actions and identify the end-state or goal from the sequence (get the bear; action is related to a certain object). The Woodward design is “old-skool” classic, but strictly speaking, we cannot precisely use it tell us about means to ends, that infants understand the hand’s actions (e.g., reaching and grasping) are performed IN ORDER TO acquire a certain object.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Do infants understand actions are performed to achieve goals?

  • Yes, there are complementary studies that tell us that in the first year of life, infants understand that certain actions are performed IN ORDER TO acquire a certain object.
  • teleology; with inanimate objects; two balls and a wall; jumping rational and irrational conditions; look longer at irrational/same behaviour in new condition.

We should be careful when choosing our interpretation of the discovery that infants track others’ goal-directed actions.

how do we test which interpretation is correct?

A

A. Rich (mentalistic) interpretation:
- Infants understand that agents act to achieve goals
because they DO understand and consider that
agents have mental states (e.g., beliefs, desires,
fondness, intentions, and emotions) that cause them
to act to achieve certain goals (inconsistent with a
teleological perspective and more consistent with
theory of mind).
B. Lean (non-mentalistic) interpretation:
- Infants understand that agents act to achieve goals
only because they are able to understand end-
states (outcomes), actions as means to end-states,
and that relevant aspects of reality (e.g., a visible
block on road) can constrain efficiency of actions.
The mental states of agents are NOT considered.

*They only understand actions as goal oriented because
they can understand outcomes and how physical
properties may constrain the actions efficiency to
achieve their goal (teleological; no mental state of the
agents’ motivation, belief, intention, feeling etc.).

How do we test which interpretation is correct?
- Using false belief tasks.
- If infants can genuinely understand the concept of
false-belief (representing that people can do things
based on false thoughts, thoughts can be incorrect
and not based on reality yet still guide actors
behaviour), only then might we choose a rich
interpretation that infants have a theory of mind.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

There is some evidence that hints at mentalistic interpretations of infants’ understanding towards goal-directed actions.

What other evidence, other than false belief tasks, can we use to suggest that infants might be mentalizing (TOM) goal directing actions? Yes.

Kuhlmeier et al. (2003)

A

Kuhlmeier et al. (2003)
12-month-olds

Steps:
1. Habituation:
- Infants were randomly assigned to the square
helps movie or the triangle hinders movie
(counterbalanced; triangle is helper and square is
hinderer for half of the infants). In the square helps
movie we see the yellow square help the red circle
struggling to get up the hill. In the triangle hinders
movie the triangle comes down and blocks the red
circles path as they are struggling to get up the hill
and pushes it back down the hill.
2. Test Trial:
- Infants are then either shown two possible
outcomes; 1) the circle approaches the triangle; 2)
the circle approaches the square. Which outcome
will infants look longer at? Can they identify the
appropriate ending?
3. Results/Implications:
- Infants look longer at the approach-helper ending.
The authors conclude that these results reflect
infants ability to reason that the object is more likely
to approach the helper because they will be fonder
of the object that helped them and want to interact
more with them in the future. Infants thus are able to
socially evaluate agents they see around them which
is mentalistic in nature (fondness and distaste
judgments).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Is there convergence evidence that infants will prefer to interact with objects or agents which they socially evaluate as being good relative to objects they consider bad?

Hamlin et al. (2007)

A

Steps:
1. Infants (6- to 10-mths) morally evaluate, show
distaste for, the hinderer after watching the same
helping/hindering movies (hinderer pushing agent
down the hill, helper pushes the agent up the hill).
2. After being familiarised with both events; which
object will infants prefer to interact with when given
the chance? Kind-helpful-square or mean-hinderer-
triangle?
3. Results:
6-moths and10-month-oldd infants both show a
preference for interacting with the helpful agent
(square). This preferential selection indicates that
infants have an innate moral compass which guides
their social evaluations (TOM) which guides their
actions (interacting with moral agents).

We need to be careful about our interpretation of the Hamlin et al. study. The findings could be explained not by a rich explanation or a lean explanation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

We need to be careful about our interpretation of the Hamlin et al. study. The findings could be explained not by a rich explanation or a lean explanation.

Interpretation Debate:
Solution (scarf et al.)

A

(A) Rich:
- TOM; Moralizing social evaluations when interpreting
agents goal directed actions and anticipating how
they think they will act based on their mental states.
(B) Lean:
- Infants are picking the “helper” merely because of a
learnt association bias that is caused by a history of
stimuli reinforcement; circles bouncing motion when
helped up the hill will be attractive to infants but in
the hinderer condition the stimulating bouncing
sound and motion is not present. Thus, infants may
just be responding to which ever object is paired
with the bouncing sound/motion; perceptual
salience critique by Scarf et al. (2012).

Solution:
- If we control and equate the stimuli (bouncing sound
in both conditions; auditory) we can get clearer
results.
- Same procedure, infants are familiarised to the
helper/hinderer movies the question is whether
infants will show a preference for interacting with the
helper or the hinderer object. When perceptual
stimuli is equated across conditions, we see no
effect! Infants are equally as likely to interact with the
hinderer and the helper object (50/50). Scarf et al.
claimed their findings support the lean interpretation
(unsophisticated associations based on the context
the stimuli is presented in their psychological world).

*these authors hold contrasting views, he leaves it up to
us to consider which interpretation we favour.

17
Q

Summary

2.

A

• Infant understanding of the psychological world
appears to be sophisticated: there is growing
evidence of going beyond perceptual
discriminations to track agents’ goals. Here we
learned about the classic work from Woodward, and
how we can test it by measuring looking-time
responses in young infants.
• Infants’ sensitivities to goal-directed actions is
suggestive of rich (mentalistic) interpretations, but
can also be due to lean (non-mentalistic)
interpretations that infants apply teleology and focus
on end-states, means in order to achieve end-states
and the constraints to action.
• So is there nothing to suggest that we could adopt a
rich interpretation when theorising about infants’
performances on certain tasks? Well, there are
studies suggesting that infants attribute fondness
from someone that is helped towards the agent that
is offering help, and that infants might even
themselves prefer or like someone who acts in a
prosocial manner. But even then the evidence is
subject to alternative (very lean) interpretations (not
abstract psychological reasoning when interpreting
goal directed action). We will need to see whether or
not infants can attribute false-beliefs which might
break the tie.

18
Q

Other kinds of social information infants pay attention to that provides evidence that they’re paying attention to the psychological world.

Gaze Monitoring:

A

Gaze Monitoring:
One thing adult do is we pay attention to other people’s gaze. We follow another’s gaze to work out what is capturing that person’s attention. We monitor peoples gaze so much that it makes us uncomfortable when someone’s deliberately avoids eye contact (not making eye contact is a violation of responding to someone in a contingent manner; turn taking interactions; act-react).

19
Q

three studies studying gaze monitoring in infants:

A

We can test gaze monitoring with the “still-face paradigm” which examines infants’ reactions to their parents’ face becoming still (e.g., parent may be gazing away, face is expressionless).

(A)
Steps:
1. Play Episode: the parent and child are engaging in
normal play, and we expect them to respond
positively with smiling, cooing, babbling and eye
contact.
2. Still phase (2 minutes): where the parent suddenly
becomes emotionless and looks away from the
infant. The question is how do infants react to this c
display? We find that infants respond with negative
affect, lack of visual attention, crying, physical
attempts to restart the social interaction with
movements or vocalizations to get their mothers
attention back.
3. Reunion phase (3 minutes): where the mother
resumes normal play behaviour. We are interested to
see if a carry-over effect occurs where the infant
doesn’t resume normal play or if they engage as
normal.
4. How do babies react to a still face? Decline in visual
attention to parent; negative affect; attempt to restart
engagement (such as increased vocalisations, arm
movements)
5. Implications: Infants’ response of to the still-face
paradigm in trying to reattain their mother’s attention
is highly predictive of their own-self recognition and
if they pass the rouge-test (mirror recognition). It’s
also predictive of having positive self-regard; it
implies that humans have this innate desire to
socially interact with others (prosocial) and avoid
social rejection.

Another way to test this in a more naturalistic setting and cause infants less distress is to test whether infants will follow people’s gaze to the object that’s capturing their attention.

(B) Scaife and Bruner (1975)

  • We can simply chart when infants will follow gaze to look in the same direction as the adult / or experimenter.
  • Results:
  • 2-8-months: 30% followed gaze
  • 8-10-months: 65% followed gaze
  • 11-months: 100% followed gaze

We can also conduct experiments to test for gaze following:
*more controlled experiments which test specific line of sight

(B) Brooks & Meltzoff (2002):
 Open-eyes versus closed-eyes conditions
- Steps:
- 12–18-month-old infants’ ability to distinguish
between an open eyes and closed eyes condition.
Infants were seated on their caregivers’ laps with the
experiment who is in front of them and captures their
gaze. Then the experimenter will either look to the
left or right size where an attractive toy is located.
The purpose of this experiment is to see if the infants
will follow their gaze and look at the correct target.
This was done either with the experimenters eyes
open or closed.
- Results:
- In the eyes open condition majority of infants 12-18
months of age will look longer at the target item. In
the closed eye condition it is less likely that the
infant will follow their movements and look at the
targeted item.
- This indicates that infants are able to distinguish
between people with their eyes open or closed and
are more likely to interpret that the object is
interesting and captured their attention when their
eyes are open rather than closed. Furthermore, they
are more likely to follow their gaze and look at the
targeted item.

But, we could be sceptical and say this is a condition effect where infants are only focusing on which direction their head is moving. They may even follow the gaze of someone who has their eyes shut but their head moves. How sophisticated are infants in their understanding of what it means for someone to look at something?

To test this theory, they did to other conditions: Headband or blindfolded

  • Steps:
  • In the blindfold condition an opaque black cloth was
    placed over the experimenters’ eyes and would turn
    their head either to look at the left or the right target
    object.
  • In the headband condition the same band was used
    as a headband across their forehead which left the
    experimenters eyes open.
  • Results:
  • They found that infants were more likely to follow the
    experimenters gaze and look at the targeted object
    if the experimenters’ eyes were open relative to eyes
    closed/covered.
  • Interpretation:
  • The authors interpret this as evidence that infants
    interpret adults looking as being object directed and
    is worthwhile to look at what captures their attention.
    This implies that infants have some sort of
    psychological filter in the way that they interpret the
    actions of other people (cues such as gaze are used
    to infer about other people’s psychological states).
20
Q

Infants may also use facial reactions (of other agents) as clues to their internal mental states (like where someone is looking, how someone is feeling). Adults use feedback from others in order to appraise a current situation and determine how one should respond; this ability is called social referencing.

Social referencing (what is safe to do and what is not) is often tested with the visual cliff paradigm.

Gibson & Walk (1960)

A

Gibson & Walk (1960)
Steps:
1. Nine-month-old infants that can crawl where placed
on a clear glass table with a checkerboard pattern
on one side (immediately on the glass surface and
another on the floor which creates the illusion of a
cliff even though it is a stable surface).
2. The question is whether infants will crawl over the
edge of the cliff which is interpreted as them having
good enough depth perception to know it is not
safe to crawl and what is not and stop crawling when
they reach the “cliff edge”.
3. We can use this paradigm to see whether infants use
social referencing to guide this decision (i.e., to what
extent do they use the emotions expressed on the
caregivers face to decide where is safe to crawl;
modulated by caregivers expression).

21
Q

Two evidence on crawling behaviour being modulated by caregivers’ expression

A

(A) Sorce et al. (1985)
- 9 to 12-mth-olds
- Steps:
- In this study there are two conditions; on the shallow
side infants are on the table and their caregiver
stands across from then on the other side of the cliff
with either a fearful face or happy face.
- Results:
- When parent showed fearful face: 17/17 did NOT
cross
- When parent showed happy face: 14/19 crossed
- Interpretation:
- Crawling behaviour was modulated by another’s
emotional expression in the face; they were more
likely to cross when shown a happy motivating face
rather than fearful face (can pick up on subtle social
cues to guide their behaviour based on the
psychological state they can pick up on in their
caregivers; in times of uncertainty infants turn to
their caregiver and picks up on non-verbal social
cues to determine what to do and not do).
- Visual information about emotion in the face.

(A) Vaish & Striano (2004)
- 12-month-olds
- Emotions can be expressed in both visual and
auditory manner (loudness, tone or pitch).
- Steps:
- Using the visual cliff paradigm to see when and how
fast infants learn to cross the visual cliff across three
conditions: face + voice (caregiver is across the cliff
with a happy face and encouraging oral), face only
(encouraging happy face), voice only (caregiver says
its okay come over in a encouraging tone without
seeing their face).
- Results:
- They are faster at crossing the visual cliff (shallow to
deep side) to obtain an appealing toy in the voice
only and quicker in face + voice condition.
- They still crossed in face only condition but at a
much slower rate.
- Interpretations:
- Their findings highlight that infants use vocal cues to
quickly modulate their behaviour and how they
respond when uncertain (social referencing).
- They argued this is because when infants can focus
on vocals it allows then to distribute the rest of their
attention on examining their visual environment; they
do not have to keep shifting attention between their
face and the table which can clutter their working
memory.
- Social cues are multi-modal cues to guide their
behaviour and inferring about their psychological
states.

22
Q

Two kinds of object-directed pointing:

A

Protoimperative pointing:
- When pointing is used to obtain an object (“Get me
that”); this type of pointing does not strictly
necessitate an understanding of others, because the
function of pointing only serves an instrumental act-
to get that object)
(B) Protodeclarative:
 when pointing is to solicit joint attention by making
other attend to something interesting by looking at
them and then the object to indicate to them that it is
something worth looking at and share attention on
this object; remark on the world, to the other person
and influence their mental states. This is thought to
be a precursor to TOM understanding in infants and
on average it emerges in 12-month-old infants.

*gaze following is crucial for learning language, object
names, new objects and concepts, acquiring new skills via imitation and instruction. It is a form of social learning.

23
Q

There is a study which shows that infants (12-month-olds) use proto-declarative pointing to remark on the world to others, to establish jointness in interactions (to get others to notice something they see but others don’t).

Liszkowski et al. (2004)

A

Liszkowski et al. (2004)

Steps:
1. 12-month-olds are assigned to one of four
conditions: joint attention, face condition, event
condition and ignore condition. The infant is looking
at a curtain wall and the experimenter is seated at a
90 degree angle form the infant as they interact with
one another. The infant is able to see a puppet pop
up behind the curtain wall and the test is to see in
the child will try to grab the adults attention and
direct it towards the exciting stimuli to share
attention.
2. Joint Attention Condition: Experimenter repeatedly
looked back and forth between the event and the
infant’s face, talking excitedly about the stimulus.
3. Face Condition: Experimenter looked at infant’s face,
never looking at event, and talked excitedly but only
about the infant.
4. Event Condition: Experimenter looked only at event,
never looking back to the infant, and did not speak
or show excited emotions.
5. Ignore Condition: Experiment looked only at her
hands, never looking at infant or event.

 Results: 	Infants point to direct the adult's attention to an 
    interesting event, and might even point to elicit from 
    others comments about the event infants 
    themselves are attending to (i.e. they want to share 
    attention and interest).  	In the face, event and ignore condition rather than 
    the joint attention condition where they are already 
    both looking and reacting to the same stimulus 
    event so no need to point at the event. 

Implications:
 This implies that infants are sensitive to the
dynamics of the interactions around them (or lack of)
and use declarative pointing as a tool (in a
psychological manner) to achieve joint attention on
an event they deem worthy of attention. This
supports that the effect goes both ways, infants
follow and direct gaze.

24
Q

It’s a difficult issue when we are discussing the extent to which infants treat their world in a psychological manner because it is subject to so many interpretations. Is it theory of mind or teleology? Its hard to decide.

The best evidence we have to support infants’ ability to engage in theory of mind (mentalizing) is with a false belief task which tests whether infants can attribute beliefs, particularly false beliefs, to others and guide our own actions (this requires a representational understanding of mind and that our psychological understanding may be different to others; that it is one version of reality; that these interpretations wrong or right can still guide action in meaningful ways).

When does such understanding develop?

A

When does such understanding develop?
 Scenario:
 Max has a toy which he places under the box
marked x and then leaves the room to go play
outside. Whilst he is out his mother comes in and
moves the toy to box y. When max comes in where
will he look for the toy? X (pass) or Y (fail). Are they
able to understand that max doesn’t have the
information on the new location of the toy and will
be guided by his false belief to search for the toy in
box x?
 Results:
 By age 6-7 years of age children will consistently
search for the toy in box x; they will also be able to
justify their choice that max will search for it in box x
where he believes that is where it is. 2–3-year-olds
will fail this task and select box y and say that is
here the chocolate is so that is where max will
search. This shows that TOM goes through rapid
development between ages 3-6 years; takes years
to pass it because it is a cognitively challenging
psychological understanding of representational
reality to develop; they’ve gone beyond a
teleological understanding of the world.

25
Q

Can we test if pre-linguistic infants are able to pass false belief tasks? If they can then this would force use to accept that infants are more than teleological (beyond goals) and in terms of representations (mental states). This is why it’s such an important theoretical question.

Violation-of-expectation version of the false-belief task
(Onishi & Baillargeon, 2015)

A

(Onishi & Baillargeon, 2015)
15-month-old infants

Steps:
1. Familiarization:
• Infants were habituated to three events/trials; a
yellow and green boxes are sitting on the table the
screen lifts and they see a researcher pick up an
object and place it in the green box and keeps their
hand there; till they get bored and look away. They
see the next event where the researchers’ hand is
inside the green box; gets bored. The curtain lifts up
again and they see the researchers’ hand in the toy
box where the toy is.
2. Belief-Induction:
• They are either assigned to the true belief green
condition or the false belief green condition. In the
true condition they see the yellow box move left to
right (researchers belief doesn’t change; location of
object doesn’t change). In the false belief green
condition the screen goes up and the researcher
leaves, whilst their gone the toy is moved into the
yellow box (researchers’ belief doesn’t change;
location of object changes).
3. Test:
• Infants are either shown one of two outcomes:
green or yellow outcome. In the green outcome
condition the researcher reaches for the object in
the green box; in the yellow outcome condition the
researcher searchers for the object in the yellow
box.
• Researchers were interested if infants expected
other agents (researchers) to act on their false
beliefs or not. If their TOM is sophisticated and they
engage in representational capacity than they
should expect them to look at the green box; fooled
by their false belief.
• They found that violations of false beliefs (search in
yellow box; should not know this) they look at this
outcome longer than the green box. In the true
belief condition infants also look longer at yellow
box if they know it is in the green box they shouldn’t
be searching for the object in the yellow box.
• The authors make a bold claim: “15-month-olds
realize that others act on the basis of their beliefs
and that these beliefs are representations that may
or may not mirror reality” rich interpretation of what
infants are doing (hold beliefs; representations; true
or false guide behaviour).

26
Q

Debate of Onishi & Baillargeon (2015)’s False-Belief Task Findings:

A

(A) Lean Interpretation:
• They do not hold a representational TOM. Their
looking behaviour may be caused by association
biases. This means that infants in the familiarisation
phase learn the researcher’s three way associations
(agent-toy-green box visual pattern association). In
the induction phase (both condition); infants still have
the three way association is remembered and is not
changed; association holds. Therefore, in the test
phase they are bored by the green box outcome
because the three-way visual association pattern is
familiar and they look longer at the yellow box
condition because the agent is paired with a new
pattern (agent-object-yellow) to process new visual
patterns.

(B) Rich Interpretation:
• “15-month-olds realize that others act on the basis of
their beliefs and that these beliefs are
representations that may or may not mirror reality”
rich interpretation of what infants are doing (hold
beliefs; representations; true or false guide
behaviour).

(C) Infants are applying basic behaviour search rules
rather than TOM.
• For example, people will search for objects where
they last saw it. In both conditions, the researcher
saw the object in the green box last and therefore if
looking for it will look at the green box. Therefore,
when they come back and look at the yellow box
they violated the basic search rule and look longer at
this event.
• Infants may indeed be predicting what the actor will
do next, but basing their predictions on a weaker
form of representation than that of another person’s
belief. Infants learn a behaviour rule stating that
“agents look for objects where they last saw them,”,
and expect agents to search according to the “last
saw” rule, and are surprised (look longer) if agents
do not search according to behaviour search rule.

27
Q

Summary

3.

A

• Gaze following and monitoring can confer
psychological understanding of others, and infants
by the first year of life follow gaze and interpret gaze
as a form of mental act, and even use facial
expression of others as a guide to how to behave
themselves.
• It is possible that infants have psychological insight
into the psychological world, that infants are able to
represent others’ false-belief. But is this really case,
is it so clear-cut? Or can we appeal to simpler, leaner,
explanations? The jury is still out.

28
Q

Overarching Topic Covered in Module 1-3:

A
	Thinking back to module 1-2. They demonstrated that there are a range of studies which support that infant have a relatively sophisticated and complex appreciation of the physical world and objects. Infants understand that actions of people are directed to goals; that agents take certain steps to achieve certain goals; the way context can gives clues to what actions are relevant and irrelevant to those agents’ goals.
	Module 3 is aiming to see whether we have complementary evidence that prelinguistic infants also have a sophisticated and abstract understanding of their psychological world, the world of people, do they cognitions, that people are psychological beings guided by goals, beliefs, mental states.