Module 5: Child Memory Flashcards

1
Q

Children’s event memory:

*three groups of factors that influence children report on their experience.

A
  1. Event: Factors associated
    with the event itself (i.e.,
    type: single or reoccurring event, recent or
    past, positive, neutral, or negative event)
  2. Child: factors about the child.
  3. Questioning: how they are questioned.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

(3) Child factors
What children bring to an interview

  1. (2)
  2. (4)
  3. (6)
A
1. parent-child conversation styles
   > elaborative
   > directive
2. age (young/old)
   > amount
   > accuracy
   > coherency
   > suggestibility
3. cognitive development
   > memory
   > knowledge 
   > source monitoring
   > theory of mind
   > language
   > social
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

(1) Parent-Child Conversations:

A

• Infants are dependent on their caregivers
to reach maturity and strongly influenced
by their nested systems (family, community
and opportunities).
• The style in which parents talk to their
children. Question and respond to them
influences their development.
• Parent-Child Conversations shape multiple
skills (Salmon & Reese; 2015):

   • Memory function (encoding)
   • Memory retrieval (recall)
   • Narrative style (how they talk about 
      events)
   • Elaborative style (linked to children's 
     memories being richer memories 
     currently at questioning and in the 
     future; better encoding of memories, 
     recall of them and language skills to 
     recount and communicate them to 
     others).

Two types of conversational style parents can adopt:

  1. Elaborative:
    • Where parents use “WH” words (what,
    where, why, when, who etc.) and open-
    ended questions to encourage the child
    to think about and generate their own
    responses.
    • What did we see at the zoo? What was
    he doing? What was he eating.
    • Small contributions children make to
    conversation by providing additional
    information. Responding and building
    on children’s contributions to get to the
    next step in the narrative.
    • Parents are modelling to the child what
    type of information may be interesting
    about an event to provide when people
    ask you about it.
  2. Directive:
    • In contrast, a more directive style of
    conversation where they have open-
    ended questions, but the narrative of
    the story is guided by the questions the
    parent asks rather than what
    information the child provides. Can
    jump all over the place rather than
    focusing on a single topic/event.
    • Tell dad what we saw at the zoo? What
    else did we see? What else did you
    see? What were they doing?
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

(2) Age differences in memory

A

• Brown et al. (2012; 2018)
• Memory is a construct. Therefore, there
are many many ways that it can be
operationalized making it hard to do
comparisons across studies (quantity:
amount vs quality: accuracy or
coherency of what is accounted or
suggestibility on its proneness to error).
• IV: Interview Question Styles & Young
(4-6) or Old (7-11)
• Method: children experienced an event
at the school (alone) then six weeks
later were interviewed in the lab about
their experience at school.
• DV:
• Number of units recalled in terms of the
amount and coherency.
• Older children report more details than
younger children.
• Older children report an event with
greater coherence than younger
children (follows a single narrative;
easier to follow).

• DV:
• Older children had greater accuracy in
recalling an event (small but significant
difference that occurred across types of
interview questions but didn’t hold
when broken down into types of
interview questions). Less memory
errors in adult children but it’s important
to note that the youngest kids were 85%
accurate.
• Suggestive questions (children being
able to say no, that isn’t right to
suggestive questions) we see that older
children are more accurate in their
response to suggestive questions
(younger children are more
suggestible).

In summary:
General trends that as children age their memory (recall of events):
• Amount recalled increases
• Accuracy (depends on how they’re 
   questioned)
• Closed questions increases (multichoice, 
   yes/no; gets better with development)
• Open questions (no age difference trend; 
   older and children are similar)
• Overall increases (across question type 
   there is a general trend of accuracy 
   increasing as they age)
• Coherency increases (constructing and 
   narrating events to others)
• Suggestibility decreases across 
   development.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

(3) Cognitive development

> 6

A

 A range of cognitive skills are employed
when we talk about our past experiences

o Memory
• Encoding (how their experiences are
explained), storage (how the information is
consolidated and connected to other
memories in the network), retrieval
strategies (how the information is recalled;
what other information is selected in the
search and what they select: IMPORTANT
TO READ THE TEXTBOOK CONTENT ON
THIS)

o Knowledge
• General, specific, abstract (how we
understand and explain events to others at
different levels of abstraction)

o Source-monitoring
• where our knowledge is from (process by
which we identify and understand where
our knowledge comes from)

o Theory of Mind
• our understanding of other’s beliefs,
motivations, knowledge, etc. (extent to
which we understand other people’s minds
influences how we perceive and event,
encode it and how it is communicated to
others).

o Language
• Receptive and expressive (how we encode
it, make sense of it, and communicate it;
COVERED IN THE TEXTBOOK)

o Social
• Tailoring to the audience (tailor what
information is important or appropriate to
the current situation they’re in)
• Conversational conventions (understanding
when it is appropriate to give a log
summary or short summary).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Age caveat

A

Is more complex than this. It is a general milestone (averages across children) in development which indicates what a skills child should have but we need to remember that these milestone may not reflect reality (all development; individual differences exist):

• Variability across and within age groups is
typical (variability within age groups is
normal)

• Abuse/neglect can lead to developmental
delays (applying it to special populations
where developmental delays occur these
milestones may not be as accurate)

• Age summarises a range of abilities, which
might vary in a particular child in different
ways (skills better than others in each child)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Knowledge

(2) studies

A

o Memories are not like a recorder or video
tape of real events that can be recalled with
vivid accuracy.
o Memories are rebuilt each time we recall
them (reconstructed; each time its recalled
and prone to bias)
o Reconstructive process
• Recall (what seems to go together or is
activated during the search)
• Inference/filling in gaps (fill in gaps of our
own memory with own assumptions, biases
or knowledge at recall)
o Draws on
• Knowledge/expectations (realistic;
experience with that category of events)
• Typicality/atypicality
• Ease of access/familiarity (familiarity heuristic
where easier to recall information is
perceived to be more accurate)

Helping and hindering
Example (1)
 Sutherland et al (2003)
• Study where 5–6-year-olds all experienced
an event with a pirate. There were (4) goals
to this experience: becoming a real pirate,
making a treasure map, finding the treasure
and sailing the pirate ship.
• What extent does provide children with
knowledge or activating knowledge they
already have influences how they later recall
and explain the event.
• There were three groups of children: the day
before the event had a discussion with the
experimenter- specific (information about
pirates that would directly map onto the
event they were about to experience),
general (more generally about pirates not
linking to the future experience), irrelevant
(talked about fire fighters rather than pirates).
• The next day hey visited the pirate and then
five days later were interviewed to see how
much information they would recall. Does
activation of knowledge relevant or
irrelevant to event influence later recall?
Does the type of knowledge activate matter?
• Results:
• The specific group recalled more information
than the irrelevant group (rehearsal or
reinforced information latter recalled)
• The general group did worse (recalled less
information) to the specific and the irrelevant
group (general knowledge activation
undermines memory recall of events).

• Conclusions
• Prior knowledge can make things easier to
understand and remember. It can either help
or hinder recall. The type of knowledge
matters (general inhibit specific detail recall;
specific knowledge can help later recall).

Example (2)
 Prior knowledge may also lead to error
when the prior knowledge conflicts or
contrasts the event
• Ornstein et al (1998)
• 4 and 6-year-old children had a health
check-up with their doctor and were
interviewed 12 weeks later about their
experience.
• The health check up either included typical
features and used to experiencing
(height/breathing/hearing checks) or atypical
(knee reflexivity/head measured/blood
pressure taken).
• IV: typical or atypical features
• DV: recall of typical or atypical features that
did or didn’t happen (error) and response to
suggestive questions (deny that didn’t
happen)
• Results:
• Correct recall of experienced typical
features was higher than atypical features
(correct inclusions)
• Children incorrectly reported typical non-
experienced items more than atypical (more
likely to falsely report something typical
happened which didn’t than atypic =
incorrect inclusions).
• Correct denials of non-experienced atypical
features was higher than for non-
experienced typical features (correct denials)

*Knowledge helps recall when it aligns with
our expectations of how events normally go
but hinders recall (increase error or number
of incorrect inclusions) when it contrasts our
expectations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Memory for familiar events

*what processes cause this?

A

 As children mature they begin to form dual
representations of an familiar or repeated
events
• Script: What usually happens (expectations
in general what happens for this type of
event; high level generic information)
• Episodic: What happened on a particular
occasion (memory trace of specific
experiences/even; more detailed with
sensory details of a specific event)
 Scripts can guide both retrieval of past
information (reconstruction) and planning or
prediction of future events (reduces
cognitive energy spent; mental shortcut to
free up resources to identify new
information to put into our network; salient
or unique information of new events).

*Can be helpful or undermine accurate
memory if a particular event diverges from
our script. Across child development they
are learning how to differentiate information
meant to be in scripts or episodic memory.
Younger children are particularly vulnerable
to mixing them up. Forming scripts is harder
for younger children than older children.

Scripts and Recall
Example:

 Single Event
• Thomas is three and off to his first birthday
party which is batman themed. They play
pass the parcel, gives him a ben 10 watch
and eats fish n chips. When he is asked
about the party he can recall the events with
a specific detail “he won”.
• One off occasion where he can recall
details of the event.

 Repeated event:
• Another party which is frozen theme, played
pin the carrot on olaf, gave her a jigsaw
puzzle and ate popcorn whilst watching the
film.
• He might begin to see that when recalling
events there are patterns in what’s present
each time or the questions people ask him
about it (food, theme, games, gift etc.).

 Three weeks later:
• Harder for him to recall specific details but
can use his script to rebuild his experience.
Use them as retrieval cues to search and
find memories; spread activation to trigger
another schema and reconstruct the event.
Patterns and differences between the
events (movie vs game; won or loss) sort
information into common and uncommon
aspects of the event.

 12 weeks later:
• Jamie batman themed, fish n chips,
swimming pool, sticker book gift. However,
this time the details of both batman parties
can be activated and confuse the
information from the two events. Familiar
details (overlap) will be details easier
recalled but differences harder to recall.
• Salience of new events could be easier to
recall. Memory errors can occur, can be
corrected as they activate more memories
or kept and overtime through repetition
becomes a part of the memory.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Markers of script vs episodic recall
*There are language markers children use
which indicate if they’re using script or
episodic description of events.

A
Script
> Condensed version or outline (overview)
> Sparse content (focus on similarities across 
   events)
> Similarity rather than uniqueness 
   emphasised
> “would”, “usually”
> Impersonal pronouns (you go to party, play 
   games and eat food)
> Timeless present tense 
> Chronologically ordered (drawing on 
   genera knowledge)
Episodic
> Specific account (differences between 
   events)
> Richer content
> Distinctive features emphasised (personal 
  details)
> Emotions, goals, reactions
> Personal pronouns (I, he she)
> Specific past tense
? Chronologically ordered (may reflect 
> salience; can vary across accounts; what 
   ever is most salient to them is where they’ll 
   start)

Examples:
 What happens when you go to a b-day
party (general):
• You get to.
• Invitation, go to party, bring present, play,
blow out candles, sing happy birthday and
blow out cake.

 What happened at keslises party (specifc):
• I went to the top of the junglerama
playground with Thomas. I … then I.. We had
a minion cake, and tattoo.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Connolly & Lindsay (2001)
*Study looks at how children manage the
differences between predictable
(consistent) and unpredictable (novel) characteristics of events.

A

 Method:
• 4, 6, 8 year old’s either experienced a
single or repeated (4x) event. It either had
fixed or variable components. Single event
had to be fixed but repeated exposure
where details were either fixed or variable.
• They were then asked suggestive
questions and given misinformation 4 days
later and then the following day they were
interviewed about the event.
 Results:
• Red: fixed events: children who had
multiple experiences of the same event
with fixed features were more
suggestible than children with a single
event (better at consolidating information;
easier to recall and say no to suggestions).
• Yellow: variable events: reverse effect
where children in a single event were
better able to recall details and avoid
suggestions relative to those with multiple
experiences (scripts help with constant
information but hinder recall of variable
information; episodic information variable
to error).

*Less suggestible and less influenced by
misinformation for fixed components than
variable

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Source Monitoring

A

Source monitoring (SM)
• Knowing how you know something
(process; being able to track the source of
your knowledge, experience, dream, word
of mouth, watched on tv, thought about)
• Important for distinguishing memory from
other sources of information (e.g., scripts;
identify accurate or inaccurate sources of
knowledge; I saw it or is it general
knowledge)
• Is hard for everyone across all stages of the
lifespan but especially so for infants
(challenging cognitive skill which leads to
error in memory of events)

Example: Observed vs experienced
 Roberts & Blades (1999)
• Can 4 (young) and 10-year-old children (old)
distinguish between events they had
observed or directly experienced?
• They watched live & video events and then
after a 1-week delay they were interviewed
about their memory of the event.
• The Live Event differed from the Video
Events (similar and dissimilar) on the theme,
stimuli, actions etc. with some similarities.
 Results:
• Source confusion greater for similar than
different events (similar video events more
confused with live events relative to
different video events)
• Occurred in free recall and to specific
questions (spontaneous errors and question
response errors)
• Older children more accurate than younger
(representations kept separate and could
track source of information better than
younger infants)
• Older children less source confusion than
younger (better source monitoring in older
children)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

interviewing is a ___ process…

A

interviewing is a two-way process where the interviewer influences the child, and the child influences the interviewer. Which highlights the need for the researcher to be aware of the developmental needs of the child they are interviewing (double-sided arrow reflects this).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Context:
Conversations with children and comparing everyday conversations (socialization) vs formal assessments

Food for thought:
Think about how a child may respond and report about events across these contexts when the interviewer is parent, teacher, doctor or judge/interviewer/police and how would the conversation unfold? What questions would be asked?

A

Changing contexts for children’s event memory will influence:

> Details (more important in some settings 
  than other; more detailed or brief 
  overview)
> Responses 
> Accuracy (more important in some 
   settings than other; charges or 
   medication)
> Roles (of child)
> Expertise (who has power; teacher has 
   more knowledge than child, but doctor or 
   police rely on child for knowledge about 
   an event)
> Consequences (of response more severe 
   in formal settings)

*Context matters! Will they answer, how
they answer, what information they provide
and how accurate it is. Expectations from
everyday life conversations clash with
expectations about conversations in formal
settings.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

How do adults talk to children?

A

> Structure and guide the conversation
heavily
• via modelling
• Helping them the learn to understand
the question being asked and the best
way to answer it
Use closed questions (closed questions
are more common in everyday situations;
multi-choice or yes/no)
Teaching and testing (closed questions
allow adults to provide feedback and a
teaching experience)
Shape conventional responses
(encouraged to be brief in everyday
conversations; social convention; broad
summary of what happened with fewer
details)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Waterman, Blades & Spencer (2000)

The way we ask questions influence whether children respond and how they respond

A

> Children were asked a series of questions
which were either closed/open or
sensible/non-sensible.
• Open sensible (what colour is a banana)
• Open non-sensible (where do circles
live)
• Closed sensible (is a rabbit faster than a
turtle)
• Closed non-sensible (is a box louder
than a knee)
Will children answer questions that are
non-sensible and is the way the question is
asked influence how children respond?
In the sensible Q’s there is a correct
answer to test accuracy but in the non-
sensible Q’s an accurate response would
be the child saying the Q makes no sense,
IDK, what or huh?
Results:
• Children are good at answering sensible
open ended and sensible closed questions
(i.e., can answer sensible questions
irrespective of their structure).
• Children were good at rejecting
nonsensible open-ended questions (i.e.,
say idk, what, or that question is silly)
• Children were poor at answering non-
sensible closed questions over 70% of
children tried to answer the question.
The way in which a Q is asked influences
whether a child will try to answer it (i.e., will
try to answer an unanswerable Q if it is
asked in a closed way)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Answering difficult questions

*How well can children identify when a question is unanswerable

A

• Many ways in which questions can be
unanswerable or difficult
• Not knowing (they’ve never had that
experience so don’t know)
• Not understanding the question (complex
syntax or grammar)
• Managing interviewer error (accidental or
purposeful suggestive or misleading
questions; q’s about this that did not
happen)
• Forensic interviews and courtroom – critical
that children do not acquiesce, guess, or
reframe the question (do not follow the q’s,
agree with things that are not correct or do
not signal when they do not understand)
• Ground Rules of conversations (its okay to
say you do not know; if the Q is tricky and
you do not understand it tell me and do not
guess; if I make a mistake its important that
you tell me its not right) said before the
interview.
• How do children understand and respond
to difficult questions and ground rules, and
does this change across contexts in which it
is assessed?

Procedure
Children aged 3-12 years were asked to complete three tasks:
The three Ground Rule Competency tasks were:
1. Explanation: Children were introduced to
Abby the Alien and asked to help her
understand how to answer questions. They
had to explain to her what it means to say
don’t know/don’t understand/correct
someone, and why that is important, and
when you should not do this
2. Application to story: Children were read a
brief story about Johnny, who found a
caterpillar and brought it home to keep in a
jar. IN the morning it was gone, and
Johnny’s mum asked him questions to try
and figure out what might have happened.
Children had to say what Johnny would
answer (some questions were answerable
from the picture, some should elicit
dk/du/cm responses).
3. Application to self: Children were asked to
tell Abby about their day, from waking until
getting to school, and answer questions
from the research assistant. Some were
answerable, some should elicit dk/du/cm

Results:
§ Rules:
• Maximum score is 14.
• 12 year old’s are only just above 50% at
8/14. this indicates that for a lot of the
questions they tried to answer them instead
of signaling they were unanswerable.
• Clear age trend where they get better at
signaling there is a problem with the
question.
• Responding is similar across don’t know,
don’t understand or correct me questions.
However, they respond better to don’t know
and correct me questions relative to don’t
understand questions.

Tasks
• They did worse on the story task relative to
the other tasks (shocked the researchers
thought this would be the easier task; they
made more guesses rather than saying IDK
etc.).
• Children get better with age at responding
to difficult questions but even by age 12
they sill make 50% of errors.
• There are differences in using rules to
answer unanswerable questions. They said
Idk more and that’s not right rather than I
don’t understand.
• The way questions are asked and the
context they are asked in influences the
response you get. When children are
asked about themselves they are more
likely to be able to correctly manage
difficult questions from the interviewer
relative to when they are asked to explain
the rules or talk abut a story.

Together these two studies show that children may answer anything
• Because children attempt to answer a
question does not mean they understand it
• Children may not interpret the question as
it was intended and answer as they assume
the interviewer meant it (take what they
know and fill in the blanks or reframe it)
• The structure of a question influences
likelihood of response
• The type of information children are being
asked to provide influences whether and
how they answer
• Evidence of understanding and
performance varies across contexts

17
Q

Question types
Structure – open vs closed (type of question)

Different types of questions elicit different types of memory processes:

A

Question types
Structure – open vs closed (type of question)

Different types of questions elicit different types of memory processes:
> Open questions (what happened? Tell me
everything. Fill in everything yourself)
• Recall memory
> Closed questions
• Recognition memory
• Multichoice or yes/no format

Pros and Cons of different types of remembering

> Recall (Tell me everything you can 
   remember about the car)
o Child directed (child is in control of the 
   remembering process; they have expertise; 
   decide what is relevant or not)
o Lengthy responses (provide longer 
  answers, paragraphs)
o Cues further recall (remembering of 
   information elicits recall of other related 
   information; spread of activation)
o	More accurate
o	More organized

> Recognition (Was the car blue?)
o Interviewer directed (the interviewer is in
charge of deciding what information is
relevant to recall)
o Brief responses (short one-word answers)
o Limited further recall (doesn’t elicit further
recall because the interviewer tends to ask
multiple recognition questions; sit and way
for next question)
o Less accurate
o Less organized (directed by
interviewers thought process rather
than child’s)

*Types of questions influences what
information is recalled and how it is
communicated and its quality

Example:
Brown et al (2013)
• 6-8 year old children were apart of a 
  staged event at school and were then 
  interviewed about it 6 weeks later. 
• Interview’s had multiple formats of 
  questions in a natural format.
• Results:
• Amount of information recalled:
• When asked brad open ended questions 
  (tell me everything you can remember) they 
  recalled the highest amount of correct 
  information.
• When asked focused recall questions 
  (closed; where was it what did he say) 
  children provided less correct information 
  about the event.
• Suggestive (yes/no or multichoice) 
  questions meant children provided very 
  little details.

• Accuracy of the information recalled
• When asked broad open ended questions
child’s responses are more accurate (80%),
then direct questions, then closed and
finally suggestive questions elicited the
most inaccurate responses (60%).
• We see very few age related differences in
broad, direct (recall), but bigger age
differences when asked closed or
suggestive questions (recognition) where
older infants recalled more accurate
information.
• Question type influences accuracy
between and within age groups

18
Q

Question types influence on information recalled summary

A
Recall Prompts:
> Open-ended (broad or direct)
> Interviewer invites person to tell 
   everything they remember (not specific to 
   one category)
> Person reports salient info (to them)
> More details
> Greater accuracy
> Gist of event (summary information about 
   the event and is more structured recount 
   of events)
> Narrative-based response (phrases, 
   sentences or paragraphs structure)
> More errors of omission (leaving details 
   out)
> Interviewee led
> More coherent 
Recognition prompts 
> Closed-ended
> Interviewer cues what he/she wants 
   person to report (interviewer is in charge 
   of information sharing process)
> Person responds to interviewer’s “ clue” 
   (retrieval cues provided rather than spread 
   of activation)
> Limited details
> Less accuracy
> More specific details (depending on what 
   you ask)
> Brief (one word or phrase) responses 
> More errors of commission (inaccurate 
   information provided)
> Interviewer led
> Less coherent
19
Q

Suggestibility
specific questioning strategy that is problematic and causes commission errors
How children might become inaccurate

Forms of suggestibility

A

Forms of suggestibility
Can come in many forms. Influences that are internally or externally generated which influence how people answer questions and
change memory over time (permanent or temporally impact on memories). Social processes in the moment (conformity etc.) or cognitive processes where their memory is overwritten by activation or response given.

> Pre- or post-event misinformation
• Before event misinformation which shapes
how they interpret event or provided
incorrect information about the event after
it has occurred which influences how we
recall information about the event.
Leading or misleading questions
• Suggestibility can come in the form of
leading questions (question about what did
happen based on interviewer not
interviewee) or misleading (question about
something that didn’t happen but is asked
in a way that implies that it did).
Encouragement to fill in memory gaps
• Guess, imagine, speculate
• Can you guess, think as hard as you can,
can you imagine what happened?
Selective reinforcement of responding
• Biased interviewer who gets more
enthusiastic or encouraging (reinforces) a
specific type of answer that reinforces what
they think happened. Neutral or
disparaging of answers which are
inconsistent with what they thought
happened.
Stereotype induction
• Where an impression is created of a
particular person which may influence
whether or not they share information
consistent or inconsistent with that
stereotype.
Repeated questions
• Depends on type (tell me what you
remember is fine; not introducing new
content and only encourages child to think
about it; asking suggestive or closed
questions leads children in to changing
their answer and giving the answer you
want; with content in them it implies the
correct answer)
• Ask same question till they give the answer
you want (i.e., idk; can you think really hard
and this pressures them to fill in the blanks)
Expressing doubt
• Are you sure? Did it really happen like this?

*Suggestibility increases if you have more than 1 of these factors in an interview (more
errors)

Example:
Leichtman & Ceci (1995)
> The induce a stereotype about a person
they briefly interacted with and examined
how this impacted their later recall of the
event.
> Same came to school visited them at
school; he came into the room said hi,
walked around and left.
> Other children talked about him ripping
books, broke dolls and put chocolate ice
cream on a bear. People and professionals
rated those who provides false information
as more accurate. It highlights how you
can ask questions to get the response you
want.
> Procedure:
> Children 3—6 years old who watched Sam
came into the school then 4 weeks later
were asked about it.
> Four conditions:
• Control group: no suggestion
• Stereotype: children were told by
researcher that Sam was clumsy, made
mistakes and broke things.
• Suggestion only: Suggestive questions did
he break things, get dolls dirty etc.
• Stereotype and suggestion: both
> Interviewed with free and cued recall
questions 10 weeks after the event.
> Results:
• Significant decline in accuracy of what is
reported when suggestive influence
increases children were exposed to.
• Control: accurate (95%)
• Stereotype: less accurate (80%)
• Suggestive (70%)
• Stereotype and suggestive (63%)

*Even one suggestive influence impairs
accuracy of children’s response (recall of an
event) but in combination it only gets worse.

Example:
Repeated Suggestive Interviewing
> Doubt, think harder because the other kids
said something different.
> About Miss Baker who came into school
and did a presentation about baking
cookies, put too much salt in and gave
them stickers.
> Before or after; verbal or visual suggestive
influences.
> Are you sure? Think really hard, did she
put the sticker on your knee?
> Repeating questions, casting doubt,
referencing other children’s responses
changes their answers and makes them
less confident in their recall of the event
(tentatively agreeing or complying with the
adult; maybe one kid did that in contrast to
the beginning where they said no to
suggestive questions)

20
Q

Can we rely on interviews with children to assess their knowledge?

A

It depends
> We need to think about a lot of factors:
• Child factors, types of questions used and
context which will influence the quality of
information presented.
• Need to acknowledge and minimize these
influences when interviewing children and
interpret their responses in reference to
these factors.

21
Q

Key Messages:
> Lecture (1)

> Lecture (2)

A

> Lecture (1)
• There are lots of cognitive skills which
contribute to a child talking about a past
event.
• These different skills can facilitate accurate
recollection of events or introduce error.
• When children recall events, we need to
reflect on what cognitive components
influence their recall to ascertain its
accuracy.

> Lecture (2)
• Impact of what and how questions are 
  asked influences whether a child will 
  respond and what information they provide.
• Some questions improve responding and 
  more are known for introducing error.
22
Q

Questions:
1. James tells his mother that they watched
the penguins get fed, and one of them
dropped a fish and the other penguin
raced in an grabbed it. When she
mentioned it to a parent who helped on
the trip, the parent said that the penguins
were sleeping, and they didn’t see them.
What could explain James’ account?

A

§ James:
• Source Monitoring:
o error where James is getting confused
with the information he read about the
penguins prior to the trip and his own
experience.
o Two pieces of information which share
the same theme, but the sources are
mixed and integrated.
o Confusing two different trips of their
own experience.
o Information from other kids experiences
get bended into his own memory of the
event.
• Script knowledge:
o Where he has had multiple experiences
at the zoo and has developed a general
idea of what usually happens at the zoo
and thereby struggles to pick out
information about that specific trip. Hard
to identify the similarities and
differences between events.
o Fill in gaps with another knowledge or
to make a better story.
§ James Mother
• Suggestive Questions:
• Closed questions, suggestive questions
where the mothers’ expectations about
what she expected him to see at the
zoo influences the questions she asks
and the responses James gives.
• He doesn’t correct his mother when she
gets it wrong and when later recalling
the event mixing up what happened
with information gained from the
questions.

23
Q
  1. Devise 3 questions that you would ask
    James to elicit a comprehensive
    description of his class trip. Explain why
    you would ask each and what kind of
    response you would expect from James
A

> “tell me all you can remember about the
class trip to the zoo”
• Keep the question broad and open-ended
to encourage the child to elaborate on
their own experience and what pieces of
information they recall.
• This ensures the interaction is child
directed, they recall more information,
better quality with fewer errors.
Then base the next two questions off of
their first answer.

24
Q
  1. What problems might arise, if a police
    officer wanted to talk to the children
    about their visit, because someone stole
    a visitor’s handbag during the time of
    their visit?
A

• Asking closed-ended questions to identify
whether they saw an event i.e., did you see
the bag get stolen? Could create memory
errors for children.
• Children are socialized to understand the
consequences of speaking with authority
like police and may be nervous or unwilling
to speak to police about the event and can
misinterpret the situation as them being in
trouble.
• Mismatch of everyday conversation
expectations do not match the rules of
speaking to a police officer. The role
reversal of expertise, used to being brief
with less detail, answering and getting
confirmation from the adult on whether they
were right, adult directed.
• Since the police officer was not present in
the classroom or at the trip to the zoo, they
may struggle to disentangle information
that has come from knowledge vs
experience.
• Police officer may assume that a child
would have witnessed the event when they
were not in the area. They had smaller
groups doing their own activities and when
talking to each other about what they saw
confuse the sources and blend them
together (following a line of enquiring
based on a false premise).

*Different expectations between everyday
question vs formal (accuracy over a good
Story).

25
Q
  1. Ask your neighbor about their sibling or
    another close relative. What kind of
    information did you get?
A

• How many? Age? Gender? How close you
are with them? Name? (Standard
answers/questions you would expect to
hear when asked to talk about a relative). =
adult expectations
• Child response he’s fat so he can’t run

*Building rapport with a child prior to
questioning. Highlights children do not
interpret the question the same as adults,
will find their own interpretation and answer
that and what information is salient to them
will be different to typical details adults
would include.

26
Q
  1. If James was 4 what might we predict his
    responses would look like? Amount of
    detail. Response to open questions.
    Response to closed questions. Response
    to suggestive questions. What about if he
    was 10?
A

> Age 4:
• Would be better at answering open-
focused questions relative to broad-open
(what details is important to include)
• Closed questions have no age differences
in amount but accuracy worse
• Fewer details with less accuracy
• More susceptible to suggestive questions
• Less accurate with closed relative to open
questions
Age 10:
• More details, more information
• Closed questions have no age differences
in amount but accuracy better
• Better at open-broad
• Less accurate with closed relative to open
questions

27
Q
  1. What would you expect from James
    when answering each of these sets of
    questions?
A
> Top: 
•	Recall Questions
•	More coherency elaboration, more 
        accurate, more recalled, more 
        organized and child directed recall 
        about the event.
>  Bottom:
•	Recognition questions
•	More errors, less accuracy, less 
        coherent, less recalled, less 
        elaboration, less organized because 
        adult directed.
28
Q
  1. James’ group saw the giraffes being fed
    by a zookeeper. A giraffe knocked over
    the bucket at one point, trying to get to
    the feed. One of the stories that James
    read after going to the zoo was about a
    new zookeeper who kept making
    mistakes as he learned his job. James’
    mother asked him about the trip each
    night at dinner that week, and asked what
    the zookeeper did to feed the giraffes.
    When James described their trip to his
    grandparents that weekend he talked
    about the new zookeeper, and said he
    knocked the bucket over because he put
    it in the wrong place. How would you
    explain why he came to integrate this
    detail into his account?
A

• Stereotype formed from post-event
suggestion where source monitoring error
occurs.
• Source monitoring error where repeated
questions where he may think that his
answer is wrong and change it to please his
mother, his mothers’ questions get
confused with what really happened. He
recall what his answers to the question are,
more accessible/salient, and becomes
ingrained into his long-term memory error
rather than back to the event.
• Accessibility error what he said not what he
did. Chinese whispers where the story gets
distorted with each retelling.

29
Q
  1. Jane had a special necklace on her
    dresser, that was given to her by her
    grandmother. Her grandmother is coming
    to visit and her mother wants her to wear
    the necklace, but it is missing. She told
    her mother that her sister came into her
    room one night and took the necklace. If
    you were asked to consult on the case,
    what things would you consider in
    advising whether Jane’s claim might be
    true vs false memory?
A

• Her age and what developmental stage
and the associated cognitive skills which
may impact her retelling.
• What types of questions were asked, what
information was she exposed to before and
after the interview.
• Prior knowledge; stolen items, sister is
naughty and taken things before, crime tv
shows etc. that may form scripts, source
monitoring error or stereotypes on what a
thief is or her sister as a “naughty” kid.
• Suggestive, repeated, closed/open.
• Delay between event and interview
increases error.
• Mess with interpretation of the event,
encoded and recalled.
• Older she is the more reliable they are
(generally).