Skill Transfer Flashcards
Define near and far transfer. (1 point)
Near transfer is when what is being learned and what it affects are quite similar (if not virtually identical). This is easy (if not trivial).
Far transfer is when there is a bigger difference (on any dimension) between what is being learned and what it affects. This is harder. (e.g. does practising driving on regular roads prepare you for a crash scenario?)
A significant debate over the last 100+ years has been how often far transfer happens in reality. Some argue (Detterman, 1993) that it virtually never happens spontaneously. Others argue that it can happen spontaneously (Halpern, 1998).
Describe the Doctrine of Formal Discipline. (1 point)
The Doctrine of Formal Discipline (Aristotle) holds that the mind is composed of a set of general abilities. The precise content of what is learned is less important than the general principles of learning and problem solving.
The brain is seen as a muscle that can be pumped up with exercise. This is the premise behind education and brain training.
Why is the occurrence of far transfer important to the classical view of education? (1 point)
The classic view of education is that students will be taught general learning and critical thinking skills that will then be transferrable to other domains for life. However, we know that this is an example of far transfer, which is highly difficult. This calls into question the rational of the classical view of education
In this way - Education could be viewed as an attempt to create experts from novices, using far transfer.
Give an example that illustrates the domain-specificity of expertise. (1 point)
Evidence from expertise indicates that it is extremely domain-specific, because it is based on having a large body of highly-organised domain-specific knowledge.
That is, there appears to be little far transfer between different domains of expertise (Detterman, 1993).
EXAMPLE:
One memory champion, Chao Lu (who remembered pi to over 67,000 digits) only had an average digit span (Baddeley et al., 2015, p.481). That is, maybe even what might be considered near transfer is less common than expected.
Why does our understanding of expertise raise problems for the likelihood of far transfer? (1 point)
Evidence from expertise indicates that it is extremely domain-specific, because it is based on having a large body of highly-organised domain-specific knowledge.
That is, there appears to be little far transfer between different domains of expertise (Detterman, 1993).
Because we know that expertise is highly domain specific, and relies on mental representions and chunking obtained within that specific domain. The idea of far trainser, where knowledge or skills obtained in on domain or context can transfer to another, completely contradicts this idea. For example, its not that hard to cocieve that it is not even that far a transfer for someone who can remember 30000 digits of pi to be able to remember a large amount of symbols. Hwoever, when tested he was not able to.
Describe Carraher et al.’s (1985) experiment involving far transfer in Brazilian school children (methods, results, conclusions). (3 points)
METHODS: studied Brazilian school children who also worked as street vendors - Carraher et al. posed as customers and bought things from them, recording the number of correct calculations (e.g. 5 lemons at 35 cruzeiros each = 175)
They then invited the same children to the lab and gave them exactly the same sums as mathematical problems (e.g. 5 x 35 = ?).
RESULTS: 98% correct on the street vs 37% in the lab as math problems. Also when the sums were written as word problems (nearer to their initial learning), performance improved to 74%.
CONCLUSIONS: This was taken as evidence that far transfer failed in this context (and hence one goal of the education system had also failed).
Describe Burrage et al.’s (2008) study looking at the effects of formal education on working memory (participants, methods, results, conclusions). (3 points)
There is some evidence of far transfer:
PARTICITIPANTS: comparing kids whose ages were 2 months to either side of the cut-off date for school entry (i.e. they either went to school or didn’t for the following year – and so were nearly matched for age and other factors).
METHODS: tested on executive function tests, such as auditory working memory.
RESULTS: The kids who went to school did better on executive function tests, such as auditory working memory.
CONCLUSIONS: The authors concluded that formal education improved genernal intellectual development in this area, meaning far transfer had occurred
Describe Lehman et al.’s (1988) study looking at the effects of a university education on general reasoning abilities (participants, methods, results, conclusions). (3 points)
Another defence of the Doctrine of Formal Discipline has come from Lehman et al. (1988).
PARTICIPANTS/METHODS: compared 1st and 3rd year university students (cross-sectionally AND longitudinally) doing law, medicine, psychology, and chemistry on two types of general reasoning.
They predicted medicine & psychology should improve general statistical & methodological reasoning (no effect for chemistry or law). They predicted that law, medicine, & psychology should improve general conditional reasoning (no effect for chemistry).
RESULTS:
CONCLUSION: spontaneous far transfer is possible – but only if you do a psychology degree.
What did Barnett & Ceci (2002) propose was the reason behind differing outcomes amongst studies investigating far transfer? (1 point)
Conclusion: spontaneous far transfer is possible – but only if you do a psychology degree.
Barnett & Ceci (2002) argued that the reason for the inconsistent views on far transfer is because not all transfer is equal.
Describe six “take home” messages from the transfer literature. (3 points)
- Don’t assume that transfer will occur spontaneously
- Define clearly what types of transfer you want from your training
- Specifically design to maximise likelihood of the types of transfer specified occurring
- Turn FAR transfer (hard) into NEAR transfer (easier) if possible
- If you want people to transfer a skill or some knowledge in some way, explicitly tell them this goal. This may involve persuasive communication strategies.
- Treat transfer as a skill in itself that may need training
What were three aspects of skill/ knowledge transfer CONTENT that Barnett & Ceci (2002) proposed to understand the variety of outcomes in far transfer studies? (2 points)
1 . WHAT is the SKILL
2. HOW will change performance
3. will they be prompted? (no prompts = harder)
- Consider specifically WHAT skill or knowledge you want people to transfer
- Consider HOW you want the trainee’s performance to change as a result of the transfer (e.g. improved accuracy, improved speed, improved strategic approach).
- Consider whether trainees need to spontaneously apply the transferred skill to a new domain (no prompts = very hard) – or whether it will be obvious (e.g. they’ll get prompted).
Describe six aspects of skill/ knowledge transfer CONTEXT that Barnett & Ceci (2002) proposed to understand the variety of outcomes in far transfer studies. (3 points)
- KNOWLEGE DOMAIN (near: Psyc2010 SPSS to Psyc3020 SPSS, far: applying 3020 measurement theory to astronomy)
- PHYSICAL CONTEXT (near: UQ lab to QUT lab)
3: TEMPORAL CONTEXT (near: same session i.e, mins later)
- FUNCTIONAL CONTEXT (near: Course workbook to assignment)
- SOCIAL CONTEXT (near: Individually for training and outcome)
- MODALITY (Multi choice quiz in both training and outcome)
Describe Gick & Holyoak’s (1980) experiment investigating problem solving by analogy (methods, results, conclusions). (3 points)
If transfer doesn’t often occur spontaneously then can it happen if we explicitly prompt people?
METHODS: Gick & Holyoak (1980) asked people to solve two problems with the same underlying structure and solution, but different surface elements.
Military problem: how can you attack a fortress at the intersection of mined roads without blowing everyone up?
Medical problem: how can you zap a tumour with radiation without destroying the surrounding tissue?
RESUTLS: Gick & Holyoak’s participants didn’t tend to notice the connection between the two problems spontaneously – but did once they were prompted that there might be one. They were essentially primed to look beyond the “surface elements” of two problems to the same “deep” structure.
CONCLUSIONS: We should treat transfer itself as an acquired skill
Describe three strategies for enabling students to transfer deep knowledge (Willingham, 2009). (2 points)
Examples, Explicitly, Practise
- Use lots of diverse, familiar, non-abstract examples and give people the opportunity to pick out what they have in common. (EXAMPLES
- UNDERLYING PRINICPLE)
- EXPLICITLY tell them to learn the deep structure, not just the superficial feature
- Give people PRACTICE in transferring the deep knowledge to new contexts.
Describe three strategies for improving motor skill transfer (Schmidt & Lee, 2014). (2 points)
Same as for DEEP KNOWLEGE:
- Point out similarities (or differences) among skills (UNDERLYING PRINICPLE)
- EXPLICITLY use verbal cues to emphasize transfer (if 2 things have the same underlying structure – label them the same). E.g. “kips” in gymnastics (this is the same underlying skill whether it is done on horizontal bar, rings, or parallel bars).
- Use variable PRACTISE, e.g. if your physiotherapy client has to practice getting out of a chair then do it using as many different chairs or actions as possible.