Knowledge Acquisition Flashcards

1
Q

Describe five of Willingham’s principles for learning in education together with what needs to be known about the students, and classroom implications. (4 points)

A

1 - Knowledge proceeds skill
2 - Memory is the residue of thought
3 - think under certain circumstances
4 - Context of things we already know
5 - practice

Kill
Me
To
Conquer
Penis

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

According to Willingham (2009), why is it a problem to focus education exclusively on analytical skills at the expense of factual knowledge? (1 point)

A

Willingham argues that acquiring skills like analysis or critical thinking is not possible without the underlying factual knowledge (knowledge proceeds skill)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

According to Willingham (2009), why is it a problem to talk about teaching either factual knowledge OR analytical skills? (1 point)

A

You can’t critically think without something to think about. Critical thinking requires the knowledge to be in meaningful chucks, freeing up space for higher order analysis. They are intermittently intertwined

E.g. you cannot solve problems if you don’t know the basic terms of the problem

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Why might we expect having more background knowledge on a topic would improve memory for that topic? (1 point)

A

Having more background knowledge allows chunking, which minimises cognitive load when acquiring more knowledge - you have developed a better mental representation of the topic that you can slot new information into

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What evidence is there that background knowledge aids memory? (1 point)

A

Experiment that “created” experts to avoid the confound/bias of self selection (e.g. people choose to become experts in a subject because they enjoy it). Participants had to learn about subjects they were unfamiliar with (Van Overschelde & Healy, 2001). These “experts” subsequently learned new facts quicker and more easily than a control group of “novices” who hadn’t learned so much previously.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

What did Van Overschelde & Healy (2001) find about their “created experts”? (1 point)

A

Participants had to learn about subjects they were unfamiliar with (Van Overschelde & Healy, 2001). These “experts” subsequently learned new facts quicker and more easily than a control group of “novices” who hadn’t learned so much previously.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

According to Sweller (1988), what’s the link between learning and cognitive load? (1 point)

A

According to Cognitive Load Theory, it’s also easier to learn new things when you’re under lower cognitive load (Sweller, 1988).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Describe the Wason 4 card problem and its solution. (2 points)

A

It is a problem that is hard, but becomes easy when you know the background knowledge - as if we are seeing the problem through and experts eyes.

You are shown a set of four cards placed on a table, each of which has a number on one side and letter on the other. Which card or cards must be turned over to test the idea that if a card shows an even number on one face, then its opposite face is a vowel?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

How we can make the Wason 4 card problem easy without changing the fundamental problem? (1 point)

A

Each card has an age on one side and a drink on the other. Which card(s) must be turned over to test the idea that if you are drinking alcohol, then you must be over 18?

This is made easier because you have the background knowledge - you know that the person over 18 can have either alcoholic or no alcoholic beverages

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What is the interaction between background knowledge and ease of reading, according to Willingham (2009)? (2 points)

A

Writing generally assumes at least some background knowledge - if everything is explained the writing would become unreadable. Problems can arise when the writer assumes the reader has too much background knowledge

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Describe four reasons why background knowledge helps reading comprehension. (2 points)

A
  1. It provides vocabulary
  2. It allows the bridging of logical gaps writers leave
  3. It allows chunking – which increases room in working memory – which makes it easier to link ideas.
  4. It guides the interpretation of ambiguous sentences.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What is the “fourth grade slump” and why might it be linked to background knowledge? (1 point)

A

Chall & Jacobs (2003) argue that this is a factor in the “4th Grade slump” where children from low Socio-Economic Status (SES) homes are fine until Grade 3 but then start falling behind high SES children.
They argue this is because 4th Grade starts focussing on comprehension over decoding. This requires background knowledge, which low SES children are likely to have less of. (HIGHER SOCIO ECONOMIC FAMILIES TALK MORE AND DICUSS MORE - SEE THE MILLION WORD GAP)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What does Ericsson (2016) argue for the relative roles of knowledge and skill in education? (1 point)

A

Ericsson (2016) argues that education is overly focussed on knowledge (knowing things) rather than skills (doing things). He argues that skills should be the key outcome (and knowledge is just a means to this end).
This is contrary to willingham - Willingham (2009) argues that this doesn’t mean we can therefore neglect knowledge - as acquiring knowledge is always needed for carry out a skill (and in some domains is most of the effort).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

What does Willingham (2009) argue with regard the role of knowledge in skill acquisition? (1 point)

A

Ericsson (2016) argues that education is overly focussed on knowledge (knowing things) rather than skills (doing things). He argues that skills should be the key outcome (and knowledge is just a means to this end).
Willingham (2009) argues that this doesn’t mean we can therefore neglect knowledge - as acquiring knowledge is always needed for carry out a skill (and in some domains is most of the effort).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What is Willingham’s (2009) argument for why thinking about something is crucial to memorising it? (2 points)

A

“Memory is the residue of thought” (Willingham 2009).

What students think about is what they will remember - Willingham argues that we tend to remember more “useless” things because we think about them more (e.g. because they’re more immediately appealing).

Thus we need to find a way to think about the things we want to learn

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

What experiment did Hyde & Jenkins (1973) conduct regarding the semantic processing of words (methods, results, conclusions)? (2 points)

A

Method:) showed participants a list of words (3s intervals) and asked them (amongst other things) if each word contained an E or G (non-semantic processing) OR to rate whether each word was pleasant or unpleasant on a 5 point scale (semantic, meaning-based processing).

Results: The people who were asked to rate the semantic content (meaning) of the words remembered significantly more words.

Conclusions: This is because the rating task MADE THEM THINK about what the word MEANT (unlike, for example, saying what letters are or are not in it).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Why is just considering any possible meaning of an item not necessarily helpful in remembering it? (1 point)

A

To remember something, we need to think about its RELATIVE meaning. The brain assumes that if you think about something then you may well need to think about it again in the future IN THE SAME WAY. -
e.g. the piano experiment (heavy was linked when the sentence was thought of in which the man lifted the piano)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Describe Barclay et al.’s (1974) experiment demonstrating how things are linked with their context when encoded in memory (methods, results, conclusion). (3 points)

A

Method: showed participants a test sentence (e.g. “The man lifted the piano” or “The man tuned the piano”). Then they were given some recall cues to remember the key word (“piano”) in the test sentence (e.g. “Something heavy” or “Something with a nice sound”). The participants were asked to recall the word.
Results: They were better able to remember the word when the recall cue was relevant to the test sentence (“lifted the piano” then “heavy”) than when it was less relevant (“lifted the piano” then “nice sound”).
Conclusion: the word was encoded in memory linked with its specific context in the original sentence.

(how i can remember this is the piano experiment - Barclay college of music - music=piano)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

What implications does the “relevant meanings” research finding (e.g. Barclay et al., 1974) have for educational design? (1 point)

A

we should design learning interventions so people MUST think of the relevant meaning of what is to be learned.

This could pose problems for certain “attention grabbers” in class.
For example, the lecturer drags in a grand piano to help you remember the piano experiment. However the result is that while people never forget the piano being dragged into the lecture, they forget what it was dragged in to help them remember (i.e. Barclay’s experiment).

The same might go for attractive but irrelevant slide illustrations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Why don’t students like school, according to Willingham (2009)? (1 point)

A

Effortful thinking - the type that leads to learning - is hard work

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Describe three ways the brain minimises how much thinking we have to do (Willingham, 2009). (2 points)

A

According to Willingham (2009), the brain saves us from having to think by:
(1) making sure important things like vision and movement don’t require thought
(2) biasing us to use memory and other shortcuts where possible (e.g. heuristics)
(3) by allowing us to automate processes that initially require thought so that they only require effortless memory (e.g. expertise development).

(IN SHORT: vision/movement, heuristics, chunking)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

What happens in the brain when people solve a problem (Willingham, 2009)? (1 point)

A

When you complete a cognitive task that succeeds, your brain is thought to reward itself with a small hit of dopamine (Willingham, 2009). That is, solving a problem is physiologically pleasurable.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

What does Willingham (2009) argue is the key factor influencing whether people like thinking? (1 point)

A

Solving a problem gives a dopamine hit (pleasurable). Working on a problem without progress is frustrating. Also there’s no pleasure in just being told the answer to a problem.
THUS
Too easy is boring; too hard is annoying.
Willingham (2009) argues that people like thinking when the level of difficulty is just right (not too hard, not too easy). He claims that this is more important than the content.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Describe six strategies that could be used to foster the circumstances that encourage thinking in education, according to Willingham (2009). (3 points)

A
  1. appropriate difficulty
  2. Organise everything around questions - this drives curiosity
  3. avoid overloading working memory (as per cognitive load theory)
  4. Time the presentation of questions - not too early (before they have the necessary knowledge) and not too late (after they have been told the answer)
  5. Accept and act on variation in student ability/preparation/prior knowledge.
  6. Use change to regain attention.
25
Q

What does Willingham (2009) argue that all good teachers have in common? (1 point)

A

they leverage story-telling principles in their teaching.

26
Q

Describe the principles of story-telling (Willingham, 2009). (2 points)

A

Story-telling principles – four C’s
Causality
(joining elements with causal links instead of an unrelated list)
Conflict
(some element of struggle, between characters or situations)
Complications
(sufficient nuance to avoid boredom)
Character
(could be conceptual, abstract, or inferred)

27
Q

Britton et al. (1983) manipulated the form of text in a written passage and asked participants to rate how interesting the passage was. What did they find? (1 point)

A

people found stories more interesting than other forms of text (like expository prose). (despite the actual information was matched across text forms)

28
Q

Why are good stories easy to remember (Willingham, 2009)? (1 point)

A

Willingham (2009) argued that good stories are easy to remember because you must think about the meaning throughout.

29
Q

Kim (1999) manipulated level of inference required when reading some text. What did they find? (1 point)

A

Kim (1999) found that people rated text, which required medium-difficulty inferences to be made, as more interesting than texts, where a harder or easier level of inference was required.

Speaks to the principle of NOT TOO HARD, NOT TOO EASY cognitive load

30
Q

Why might the causal structure of stories make them easier to remember? (1 point)

A

if you can remember one bit then you’re more likely to remember a consequential bit.

31
Q

Why might it be the case that students have to learn shallow knowledge before they can grasp deep knowledge (Willingham, 2009)? (2 points)

A

The shallow knowledge (e.g. lists, definitions) may provide the chunking needed to get to grips with the deep knowledge (e.g. underlying abstract principles).

32
Q

Describe three strategies that may help students understand complex content. (3 points)

A
  1. use an incremental approach, gradually building from easy to harder material
  2. provide multiple familiar concrete applications that can be compared to help extract the underlying principles (provide examples)
  3. get students to generate questions about content because this has also been found to aid understanding
33
Q

What is the key problem with students’ tendency to use less effortful study strategies? (1 point)

A

In general, if your study feels easy then you probably taking much longer to learn something than you need to. If it feels effortful then it’s more likely to be valuable (“desirable difficulties”; Bjork & Bjork, 1992). This is consistent with the idea that thinking about something is the route to remembering it (and thinking is effortful).

34
Q

To what does the phrase “desirable difficulties” refer (Bjork & Bjork, 1992)? (1 point)

A

In general, if your study feels easy then you probably taking much longer to learn something than you need to. If it feels effortful then it’s more likely to be valuable (“desirable difficulties”; Bjork & Bjork, 1992). This is consistent with the idea that thinking about something is the route to remembering it (and thinking is effortful).

35
Q

Describe the three condition testing effect experiment conducted by Roediger & Karpicke (2006) (methods, results, conclusions). (3 points)

A

Methods: asked participants to memorize a scientific passage by:
1. Repeated study (they read the text four times, no test; labelled SSSS on graph);
2. Single test (they read the text three times, one recall test: SSST); OR
3. Repeated test (they read the text once – then were tested on their recall three times: STTT).

Results: Then they were tested after 5 mins and after 1 week. Repeated study was best after 5 mins; but repeated testing was 50% higher than repeated study after 1 week.

Conclusions: Testing aids memory (the TESTING EFFECT)

36
Q

Give three reasons why students might prefer repeated study strategies over repeated testing strategies. (2 points)

A
  1. It is less effortful
  2. They notice the short term benefits (good recall after 5min)
  3. They THINK they will remember more long term (even though they won’t)

(In Roediger & Karpicke’s (2006) experiment, participants in the repeated study incorrectly THOUGHT they would remember more after 1 week than those in the repeated test condition.)

37
Q

What did Pashler et al. (2007) find when comparing a test trial with feedback with an extra learning trial? (1 point)

A

an extra test trial with feedback was better than an extra learning trial for remembering something.

38
Q

Describe the 4 condition experiment by Karpicke & Roediger (2008), comparing testing and presentation methods of learning (methods, results). (2 points)

A

Methods: compared four combinations of testing and presentation (see figure) for foreign language vocabulary (Swahili)
1, Continue to present and test items, 2. drop correct items and test others, 3. Continue presenting, no testing 4. Drop correct items, no testing

Results: Initially, all groups performed the same.
BUT the groups that involved testing were much better after 1 week (also, no advantage to dropping successfully learned words to allow more time on unlearned words).

39
Q

What is the testing effect? (1 point)

A

The “testing effect” refers to the general finding that long-term memory is improved when the information is retrieved (e.g. tested in a recall quiz).

40
Q

How is the testing effect thought to work? (1 point)

A

The testing effect is thought to be because retrieval leads to the development of a better retrieval structure, which improves access to the information in memory.

41
Q

What was the most popular method of study, according to Karpicke et al.’s (2009) study? (1 point)

A

Karpicke et al. (2009) found that 57% of students preferred the less effective strategy of just re-studying the source. Only 18% said they would try and recall the information (as in a test).

42
Q

What’s the generation effect? (1 point)

A

information is more likely to be remembered if it’s generated by the learner rather than just presented to the learner (Slamecka & Graf, 1978).

(Similar to the TESTING EFFECT)

43
Q

To what extent does repetitive exposure lead to learning? (1 point)

A

Simple repetition, assuming the learner makes no effort to structure what they’re learning, is not effective for learning, especially for complex information (Baddeley et al., 2015, page 116).

For example - can’t remember details of coins

44
Q

What did Rubin and Kontis (1983) discover using US coins – and what implications does this have? (2 points)

A

people couldn’t remember something (the details on coins) that they would have seen thousands of times and would consider a very familiar sight.

45
Q

What was memory champion Feng Wang’s method for learning 300 random digits? (1 point)

A

Feng Wang was able to remember 300 random digits in 2011 (i.e. better than Steve Faloon’s world record of 82 digits).

He used the METHOD OF LOCI

He did this by associating each number from 00 to 99 with an image.
He then constructed a “memory palace” – an imagined mansion with many locations.
Each set of 4 numbers was therefore encoded as 2 images, which were stored in one location in the memory palace (e.g. 6389 – 63 = banana; 89 = monk. Imagine monk splitting banana in a pot (location along his mental route through his palace).

46
Q

What is the Method of Loci and how do you implement it? (1 point)

A

associating each number from 00 to 99 with an image.
He then constructed a “memory palace” – an imagined mansion with many locations.
Each set of 4 numbers was therefore encoded as 2 images, which were stored in one location in the memory palace (e.g. 6389 – 63 = banana; 89 = monk. Imagine monk splitting banana in a pot (location along his mental route through his palace).

Massen et al. (2009) found that a route to work was found to be better than locations around the home, perhaps because the order of locations is more defined.

47
Q

What did Massen et al. (2009) find were the best locations to use in the Method of Loci? (1 point)

A

Massen et al. (2009) found that a route to work was found to be better than locations around the home, perhaps because the order of locations is more defined.

48
Q

What did Bower (1973) find regarding when the Method of Loci was compared to a no-strategy control? (1 point)

A

Bower (1973, cited in Baddeley et al., 2015) asked participants to remember 100 words. When they used the Method of Loci, they remembered 72% of the words – but only 28% when they didn’t.

49
Q

What did De Beni et al (1997) find when they compared the Method of Loci with rehearsal – for both written and orally-delivered essays? (1 point)

A

De Beni (1997) showed the Method of Loci was more effective than rehearsal for when a 2000 word passage was read out – but this advantage disappeared if the passage was read (perhaps the visual nature of writing interfered with visual imagery of locations).

50
Q

How does the Pegword system work? (1 point)

A

Step 1 – memorize the ten pegwords
Step 2 – imagine the first word interacting with “bun”; the second word interacting with “shoe”.

One – bun
Two – shoe
Three – tree
Four – door
Five – hive
Six – sticks
Seven – heaven
Eight – gate
Nine – wine
Ten - hen

51
Q

How does the Pegword system compare with the Method of Loci (Wang & Thomas, 2000)? (1 point)

A

The Pegword System was found to be just as effective as the Method of Loci (Wang & Thomas, 2000).

52
Q

What are the limitations of the Pegword system? (1 point)

A
  1. You need to put in a lot of work to remember the pegwords reliably enough.
  2. it’s harder to use for abstract, hard to visualize words (“morality”) than easy to visualize words (“car”).
53
Q

Describe the method of remembering names (with your own example) that was demonstrated by Morris et al. (1978) to lead to 80% correct recall. (1 point)

A

Step 1: Search for an imageable substitute for the person’s name. Jo Brown = Joey (baby kangaroo) + Brown (colour).
Step 2: Select some prominent feature of the person’s face – and link that feature with the name substitute (e.g. baby kangaroo or JOEY nestled in my eyebrows, which are BROWN in colour).
This method led to 80% recall (Morris et al., 1978).

54
Q

Under what circumstances did Morris et al. (2005) find that the imagery method of remembering names did not work – and why? (1 point)

A

Under party conditions (where it was hard to construct these images), Morris et al. (2005) found better memory was found with expanded retrieval (try and recall the name at increasing intervals, without imagery = 24 names) than the imagery method (12 names) (where a no strategy control was 16 names).

Because even those who are aware of the
potential effectiveness of the mnemonic and somewhat familiar with its use often find it
too demanding to put into practice when interacting with a new acquaintance.

55
Q

Give your own example of a verbal mnemonic. (1 point)

A

COME ON BRUTHA

56
Q

What is the Story Mnemonic method? (1 point)

A

The Story Mnemonic involves creating a story out of a list of unrelated words that need to be remembered.
Bower and Clark (1969) found this led to 93% of 120 words being remembered (compared with 13% for controls).

57
Q

What are the advantages and disadvantages of the Story Mnemonic method? (1 point)

A

Advantages: doesn’t require previous learning (e.g. of peg words or locations).

Disadvantages: takes time to create the story (e.g. may not work under time constraints).

58
Q

Give three reasons why mnemonic memory methods can be very effective (Ericsson, 1988). (2 points)

A
  1. Meaningful encoding (relating information to pre-existing knowledge)
  2. Accessible retrieval structure
  3. (Practise) Memory expert achieve their performance via practice of these techniques, which speeds up both encoding and retrieval.
59
Q

Give three reasons why mnemonic memory methods can be very effective (Ericsson, 1988). (2 points)

A
  1. Meaningful encoding (relating information to pre-existing knowledge)
  2. Accessible retrieval structure
  3. (Practise)Memory expert achieve their performance via practice of these techniques, which speeds up both encoding and retrieval.