Lecture 15: The Experimental Research Strategy ll Flashcards
removing the confound
- We can eliminate some confounding variables (ex. Change the testing room, take away the element of surprise)
- But, we cannot eliminate all confounding variables
holding the confound constant
- If we cannot remove the confounded variable, we can try to hold it constant across conditions
- Ex. caffeine, surprise, music teacher
- Holding a variable constant eliminates its potential to become a confound
- By standardizing the environment and procedures (no noise, presence of music teacher), most environmental variables are held constant
- We can also standardize the confound to a range of values (ex. Only using 30-35 year-old female participants)
problems with holding the confound constant
- Too strict control can be unreasonable
- A trade-off between standardizing and external validity
- Cannot generalize beyond this sample
using a placebo control group
- Sometimes, the experimental method itself can become a confounding variable
- To control for this, we use a placebo-control group
matching a confound across conditions
- If we cannot remove or hold the confounded variable constant, we can try to match levels of it across conditions (balanced)
- But, matching based on fixed values can limit the generalizability (threat to external validity)
- We can use counterbalancing of variables to reduce effects due to different average values
- When averages are used, counterbalancing of other factors can be beneficial
problems with matching across conditions
- Requires a lot of time and effort
- Reduces participant sample to choose from
when is matching across conditions recommended?
for specific sets of variables that pose serious threats to internal validity
randomizing participants assigned to conditions
- Randomly assign participants to treatment conditions so that the extraneous variables related to participants will balance out across the conditions
- The aim is to disrupt any systematic relationship between the extraneous and independent variables (to prevent the EV from becoming a CV)
- Powerful method for controlling many environmental and participant variables simultaneously rather than individually
randomizing participants example study
- Participants were recruited for an experiment on one of 3 testing days
- Each participant was assigned randomly to “Intervention” or “Control” conditions
- Assign each participant randomly as they appear on testing day
how does randomly assigning participants to conditions distribute extraneous variables?
using unpredictable and unbiased procedures (ex. coin toss)
the downside of randomly assigning participants to conditions
- Does not guarantee control
- It’s still possible that all people with similar backgrounds (potential CVs) are assigned to one condition
- But, with large enough numbers, randomizing guarantees a balanced result (Groups of >=20 participants per condition)
two aspects of manipulation checks
- check the manipulation
- include an exit questionnaire
checking the manipulation
take measures of the IV to make sure your manipulation did what you wanted to do (e.g., sad vs. happy mood)
exit questionnaire
tests whether the participants were aware of the manipulation(s) and purpose of the experiment
when are manipulation checks especially importnat?
- participant manipulations
- subtle manipulations
- placebo controls
- simulations
participant manipulations
- Difficult to know if worked (especially compared to environmental manipulations)
- Include a measure of IV (e.g., mood, frustration, stress) to assess if worked
subtle manipulations
- Difficult to know if participants noticed
- Exit questionnaire
- Ex. “Did you notice the expression on the experimenter’s face when she gave you the instructions?”
placebo controls
- Did participants believe the placebo was real?
- Exit questionnaire
- Ex. “What treatment did you receive? Did you feel it was effective? Were you aware you were being deceived?”
simulations
- Difficult to know if participants perceive the environment as real
- Exit questionnaire
- Ex. “What did you think when the other participants answered incorrectly? To what extent did you think about the fact that you were in an experiment?”
possible reasons that an experiment didn’t work
- IV is not sensitive enough
- DV is not sensitive enough
- IV has floor or ceiling effects
- DV has floor or ceiling effects
- Measurement error
- Insufficient power
- Hypothesis is wrong
example of an IV is not sensitive enough
IV= 2 foods to test preferences: chocolate or beets
possible solutions to an IV that is not sensitive enough
include more foods
example of a DV that is not sensitive enough
2 levels of preference to rate: yes or no
possible solution to a DV that is not sensitive enough
use a rating scale (7-point)
example of an IV with floor or ceiling effects
chocolate IV is at ceiling; beets IV is at floor
possible solution to an IV with floor or ceiling effects
include test items not as preferred/avoided
example of a DV with floor or ceiling effects
“yes” response is at the ceiling and “no” response is at the floor
possible solution to a DV with floor or ceiling effects
include responses in the middle range