Hoofdstuk 7 Flashcards
Photoreceptors
Specialized light-detecting cells, in multicellular animals.
Retina
A membrane lining the rear interior of the eyeball. The photoreceptors lie in here.
Vitreous humor
A clear gelatinous substance, through which light easily passes.
Cornea
A transparent tissue that, because of its convex (outward) curvature, helps to focus the light that passes through it.
Iris
The pigmented, donut-shaped, which provides the colour of the eye.
Pupil
Where the light enters interior of the eye, the black appearing centre in the iris. Muscle fibres in the iris enable it to increase or decrease the diameter of the pupil to allow more or less light to enter.
Lens
Adds to the focusing process begun by the cornea. Unlike the cornea, the lens is adjustable; it becomes more spherical when focusing on objects close to the eye and flatter when focusing on those farther away.
How do the cornea, iris and lens help to form images on the retina?
The focusing properties of the cornea and lens bring the light rays back together at a particular point on the retina, thereby forming an image of the object on the retina-> the image is upside down.
Transduction
A process by which a stimulus in the environment generates electrical changes in neurons.
Cones (Kegeltjes)
A photoreceptor cell in one thin layer of the multi-layered retina, that permits sharply focused colour vision in bright light. Cones are most concentrated in the fovea.
Rods (staafjes)
A photoreceptor cell in one thin layer of the multi-layered retina, that permits vision in dim light.
Fovea
The pinhead-size area of the retina that is in the most direct line of sight-> point of central focus. The fovea is specialized for high visual acuity (the ability to distinguish tiny details).
How do cones and rods respond to light?
Cones-> bright light.
Rods-> dim light.
Photochemical
In the outer segment of the photoreceptor lying chemical that reacts to light.
Rhodopsin
The photochemical of the rods. When hit by light, rhodopsin, molecules undergo a structural change that triggers a series of chemical reactions in the rod’s membrane, which in turn causes a change in the electrical charge across the membrane.
Optic nerve
Runs from the back of the eye to the brain.
Blindspot
At the place on the retina where the axons of these neurons converge to form the optic nerve there are no receptor cells.
Cone (photopic) vision
Specialized for high acuity (the ability to see fine detail) and for colour perception-> see fine details during day light.
Rod (scoptic) vision
Specialized for sensitivity (the ability to see in very dim light). It lacks acuity to distinguish colours, it is sensitive enough to allow a person on a clear night to detect a single candle flame from 30 miles away if no other lights are present-> seeing (vague) outlines and figures.
Dark adaptation
The gradual sensitivity that occurs after you enter a darkened room.
Light adaptation
More rapid decrease in sensitivity that occurs after you turn on an bright lamp or step out into sunlight.
Why do we see mostly with cones in bright light and with rods in dim light?
Different sensitivities of rods and cones: Rhodopsin is much more sensitive to light than are the cone phytochemicals-> Bright light causes rhodopsin to break down into two inactive substances, making the rods non-functional-> Therefore we see mostly in bright lights with our cones.
Three-primaries law
Three different wavelengths of light (called primaries) can be used to match any colour that the eye can see if they are mixed in the appropriate proportions. The primaries can be any three wavelengths, provided that one is taken from the long-wave end of the spectrum (red), one dorm the shortwave end (blue or violet) and one from the middle (green or green-yellow).
Law of complementarity
Pairs of wavelengths can be found that, when added together, produce the visual sensation of white. The wavelengths of light in such a pair are complements of each other
Two theories of colour vision:
(1) the trichromatic theory and (2) opponent-process theory.
Trichromatic theory
Colour vision emerges from the combined activity of three different types of receptors, each most sensitive to a different range of wavelengths. Every colour we see is a unique proportion, or ratio, of activity among three types of receptors, then the three-primaries law would be an inevitable result: it would be possible to match any visible colour by varying the relative intensities of three primary lights, each of which acts maximally on a different type of receptor.
There are indeed three types of cones in the human retina, each with a different photochemical that makes it most sensitive to the lights within a particular band of wavelengths.