Forensic Psychology Flashcards
Canter et al (2004)
Analysed data from 100 murders with reference to the 39 characteristics of organised and disorganised criminals. Found support for there being an organised but not disorganised criminal. Undermines distiction of two types of criminal and therefore the entire top-down approach
Canter and Heritage (1990)
Content analysis of 66 sexual assault cases using small space analysis. Characteristics were identified as well as patterns that were individual to each offender, which can be used to predict how an offenders behaviour may change or if two offences are committed by the same person. Supports bottom-up approach.
Lundrigan and Canter (2001)
Information from 120 murder cases revealed that the places where the bodies were disposed created a centre of gravity around a killer’s base. The data was analysed using small space analysis. Supports the idea that the analysis of geographical locations of a certain events can lead to important information in a case.
Copson (1995)
Surveryed 48 police forces, finding that the officers judged the information given by the profiler was useful 83% of the time, but only leads to accurate identification in 3% of cases. Criticises the usefulness of the bottom up approach.
Lombroso (1876)
Examied the skills of 383 dead cand 3839 living criminials. There was no control group. He developed the avatistic characteristics which were facial and cranial characteristics. Concluded that 40% of criminal acts are committed by people with these features.
Goring (1913)
Conducted a study of 3000 criminals and 3000 non-criminals and found no evidence for criminals being a distinct group with different features. Much more scientific and contracts Lombroso’s theory.
Lange (1930)
Investigated 13 MZ and 17 DZ twins with one sibling serving time in prison. Of the MZ 10 of the other siblings had also served time, compared to 2 of the DZ group. Supports genetics as a factor in criminal behaviour.
Crowe (1972)
Adopted children who had a biological criminal parent had a 50% chance of also having a criminal record at 18, a control group only had a 5% chance. Supports genetics as a factor in criminal behaviour.
Tiihonen et al (2014)
Genetic analysis of 900 criminals found MAOA and CDH13 genes as factors in violent crime. MAOA is linked to dopamine and seritonin and SCDH13 is linked to ADHD and substance abuse. Individuals with both were 13x more likely to have a history of violent behaviour. Supports genetics as a cause of offending behaviour.
Raine et al (2000)
Found an 11% reduction in the volume of grey matter in the pre-frontal cortex of people with APD. Supports neural differences.
Keysers et al (2011)
Only when criminals were told to empathise with a person in pain was there an empathy reaction in the brain. Suggests a neural ability to turn off their empathy in criminals. Supports neural differences in a non-deterministic way.
Eysenck and Eysenck (1977)
Compared 2070 male prisoners score on EPI to 2422 male controls. Across all ages prisoners cored higher on extroversion and neuroticism. Supports Eysenck’s theory of personality.
Farrington et al (1982)
Meta-analysis revelaing that offenders score higher on psychoticism but not neuroticism or extroversion. Criticises Eysenck’s theory.
Kohlberg et al (1973)
Gave the Heinz dilemma to a group of violent teenagers. Found lower moral development than non-violent participants. Supports moral development as a factor in criminal behaviour.
Thornton and Reid (1982)
Found pre-conventional development common in crimes where criminals thought they would not be caught, like robberies. More impulse driven crimes have little association with a reasoning level. Suggests moral reasoning is not a sufficient explanation of offending behaviour.