Chapter 8: Liking, Loving, and Interpersonal Sensitivity Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

What are the four most typical reasons that people give for liking their friends?

A

People like most:

  1. Those whose beliefs and interests are similar to their own;
  2. Those who have some skills, abilities, or competencies;
  3. Those with some pleasant or admirable qualities, such as loyalty, reasonableness, honesty, and kindness;
  4. And those who like them in return.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

The four aspects of interpersonal attraction can be gathered under what sweeping generalization?

A

We like people whose behavior provides us with maximum reward at minimum cost.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What are the problems with a general reward-cost theory of human attraction?

A
  • Critically, the world isn’t that simple.
  • For example, a reward-cost theory would lead us to suspect that, all other things being equal, we will like people who live in close proximity to us because we can get the same reward at less cost by traveling a short distance than we can by traveling a great distance. It’s true that people have more friends who live close by than friends who live far away; but this doesn’t mean it’s their physical proximity that makes them attractive—it may simply make it easier to get to know them, and once we get to know them, we tend to like them.
  • It’s also been proven that people who go through punishing initiations are more likely to enjoy the group. Where’s the reward in that?
  • Lastly, to label everything (e.g. being right, fitting in) as rewards tends to obscure the important differences between them. For the social psychologist, a far more important task is to determine the conditions under which one or the other course of action will be taken.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Does praise and reward always lead to liking?

A

It’s uncertain. Consider this: you’re delivering a thesis to a room of university students. One student seems engaged and awed the entire time, and comes up to tell you that your theory is mind-blowing and you’re super smart. Another student is shaking their head the entire time and scowling at you. Afterwards, they tell you all the things wrong with your theory. When you get home, you reflect on the criticism and make your theory even better. Who will you like more? That is, is the reward of a praise greater than the reward of new insights gained through criticism?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

How can ulterior motives change the way we perceive reward? Use the example of Nancy’s blueprints.

A

If Nancy’s having a bad day and produces some sloppy blueprints, and her boss comes around and says, “Nice work, Nancy,” how will she interpret this? On the one hand, she could think he’s actually praising her and trying to cheer her up, which will lead to her liking him more. But she could also contribute this comment to ulterior motives: being sarcastic or manipulative, which could reduce her liking of him. As situations become complex, the general reward-cost theory decreases in value because a slight change in the social context in which the reward is provided can change a reward into a punishment.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

What are the different situations that demonstrate how we don’t like to feel manipulated, and how that affects attraction?

A
  • Edward Jones conducted an experiment in which a colleague was interviewing women. The women received either positive, negative, or neutral evaluations. For half of the women, they were told that the interviewer was a grad student who would be recruiting them as volunteers for their own study. Women who were evaluated positively rated the interviewer as being more likeable. But those who thought there was an ulterior motive rated the interviewer as less likeable.
  • Brehm and Cole conducted a study in which the participants were to give their first impressions of one another. The other participant was actually a confederate. The confederate asked to get up and leave for a moment before the experiment began. In one condition, they came back with nothing. In the other condition, they came back with a soft drink and gave it to the participant. The participants were then asked to help the confederate with a boring task. The students who had been given the drink were actually less likely to help.
  • These two studies demonstrate the point that praise and favours are not transsituational; whether they function as rewards depends on situational variations, some of which can be extremely subtle.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What are some studies which support the theory that you get people to like you by asking them to do you a favour?

A
  • Jecker and Landy’s experiment had participants go through a task that would allow them to win some money. One third of the participants were asked by the researcher to return their winnings because the fund was running short. Others were asked by the secretary to return it as a donation to the psychology department. The last third didn’t have to return it. The exit questionnaire showed that those who returned the money to the researcher liked him best. Because they did him a favor, they succeeded in convincing themselves that he was a decent, deserving fellow.
  • Lerner and Simmons had groups of participants observe a student who appeared to be receiving a series of electric shocks. After watching for a while, some participants were allowed to vote, by private ballot, on whether the “victim” should continue to receive electric shocks. Others weren’t allowed to vote. All those who were allowed to voted for termination of the shocks; some of the voting participants succeeded in terminating the shocks, while others did not. Those people who succeeded in terminating the shocks came to like the victim the most. The people who tried but failed to terminate the shocks liked him about as much as those who didn’t vote at all.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

How does a person’s competence influence how much you’ll like them, and what is the pratfall effect?

A
  • There is a great deal of apparently paradoxical evidence in the research literature demonstrating that, in problem-solving groups, the participants who are considered the most competent and to have the best ideas tend not to be the ones who are best liked.
  • How can we explain this paradox? One possibility is that, although we like to be around competent people, a person who has a great deal of ability may make us uncomfortable. If this were true, we might like the person more were he or she to show some evidence of fallibility.
  • In an experiment by Aronson with Willerman and Floyd, they found that: the superior person who committed a blunder was rated most attractive, the average person who committed the same blunder was rated least attractive, the perfect person (no blunder) was second in attractiveness, and the mediocre person (no blunder) finished third.
  • This phenomenon has been dubbed the pratfall effect.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

When you first meet a potential partner, what’s the most important aspect in determining whether or not you’ll like them?

A
  • As much as we don’t want it to be so, it’s physical attractiveness.
  • However, as Gregory White has found, in long-term relationships it’s the similarity of the attractiveness of the members of the couple that was crucial in determining whether a relationship had staying power.
  • As well, for women, factors such as socioeconomic status can outweigh attractiveness when selecting a long-term mate. This may be due to women’s biological dispositions to make sure her partner can care for her and her kids in the long-run.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

How can attractiveness lead us to make mis-attributions?

A
  • Karen Dion had women examine reports of children (written by their teachers) who were apparently misbehaving. Attached to each report was a photo of the child, either an attractive or less attractive boy or girl. The women tended to place more blame on the less attractive children and to infer that this incident was typical of their everyday behaviour. When the child was pictured as good-looking, however, they tended to excuse the disruptive behavior.
  • Richard Lerner found that 6th graders tended to rate their more attractive classmates as also being more competent.
  • Hunsberger and Cavanagh found that students rated good-looking teachers as nicer, happier, less punitive, and more effective than their less attractive teachers.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

How can attractiveness work against a person’s likability? Use the example of Aronson and Sigall’s experiment.

A
  • Aronson and Sigall had a woman who appeared either physically attractive or unattractive. She posed as a graduate student who was interviewing college men. At the end, she gave each student her own clinical evaluation of him. Half received highly favorable evaluations and half received unfavorable evaluations.
  • When the evaluator looked unattractive, the men didn’t seem to care much; they liked her a fair amount. When she was beautiful, they liked her a great deal when she gave them a favorable evaluation but, when she gave them an unfavorable evaluation, they disliked her more than in any of the other conditions.
  • Although the men who were evaluated negatively by the attractive woman said they didn’t like her, they did express a great desire to return to interact with her in a future experiment.
  • Our guess was that the negative evaluations from the beautiful woman were so important to the men that they wanted the opportunity to return so as to induce her to change her mind about them.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Why is agreement attractive?

A

There are at least two major reasons. First, it’s obvious to most of us that people who share our attitudes and opinions on important issues are uncommonly intelligent, thoughtful individuals. It’s always rewarding and interesting to hang out with intelligent and thoughtful people. Second, they provide us with a kind of social validation for our beliefs; they provide us with the feeling that we’re right. This is rewarding; hence, we like people who agree with us.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

How does our knowledge of whether or not someone likes us influence how much we’ll like them?

A

Predictably, if you think that someone likes you, you’ll most likely act more warmly towards that person.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

What is the effect of self-esteem on attraction?

A

We like to be liked—and the more insecure we feel, the more we like someone who likes us. One of the implications is that people who are secure about themselves are less “needy”; that is, they are less likely to accept overtures from just any person who comes along.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

How have researchers demonstrated the physiological effects of rejection?

A
  • After a Cyberball experiment, participants were asked to rate the desirability of a variety of foods and beverages: hot soup, hot coffee, an apple, crackers, and coke. Subjects who had been excluded during the ball-tossing game rated the hot soup and hot coffee as significantly more desirable than the control participants did. Apparently, one symptom of social exclusion is the physical sensation of being cold.
  • Other Cyberball experiments have shown that subjects who were excluded during the game showed increased activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and right anterior insula, areas of the brain involved in the experience of pain. But if they had been given a dose of Tylenol prior to playing the game these regions didn’t show heightened activity.
  • In a study, women were placed into an fMRI and received mild electric shocks to their ankles. There was a great deal of activity in regions of the brain associated with physical arousal, regulating negative emotions, and anticipating pain. For women who were able to hold their husband’s hand, the pain-anticipating neural activity plummeted. Women who held a stranger’s hand also had reduced neural activity, but not nearly as much as those who held their husband’s hand. This experiment goes to show that physical contact and companionship is good for us psychologically and physiologically.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

What is gain-loss theory? What does it say about how much you’ll like a person who consistently flatters you?

A
  • Imagine that you’re at a party talking to a woman. You leave and then come back to find her talking about you to another person, and decide to eavesdrop. You have this encounter with the same woman over a number of parties.
  • There are four situations that could occur: (1) You overhear the person saying exclusively positive things; (2) you overhear her saying exclusively negative things; (3) her first couple of evaluations are negative, but they gradually become increasingly positive until they equal her statements in the exclusively positive situation and then level off; and (4) her first couple of evaluations are positive, but they gradually become more negative until they equal her statements in the exclusively negative situation and then level off.
  • Which situation would render the person most attractive to you? Reward-cost theory would predict the first situation.
  • However, gain-loss theory predicts that increases in positive, rewarding behavior from another person have more impact on an individual than constantly rewarding behavior from that person.
  • In experiments, Aronson has found this. Liking produced by increases in positivity are stronger than consistent positivity; gradual negativity is stronger than constant negativity.
17
Q

What are the two conditions necessary for the gain-loss effect to be operative?

The Gain and Loss of Esteem

A
  1. It’s not just any sequence of positive or negative statements that constitutes a gain or loss; there must be an integrated sequence implying a change of heart.
  2. The change of heart must be gradual. An abrupt about-face is viewed by the stimulus person with confusion and suspicion, especially if it occurs on the basis of scant evidence.
18
Q

Define:

exchange relationship

The Gain and Loss of Esteem: The Quest for Communal Relationships

A

The people involved are concerned about making sure that some sort of equity is achieved, that there is fairness in the distribution of the rewards and costs to each of the partners. In this kind of relationship, if there is a major imbalance, both people become unhappy; the person on the short end usually feels angry or depressed, and the person on the long end usually feels guilty.

19
Q

Define:

communal relationship

The Gain and Loss of Esteem: The Quest for Communal Relationships

A

Neither of the partners involved is keeping score. Rather, a person will be inclined to give of herself or himself in response to the other’s need and will readily receive the same kind of care when he or she is feeling needy. They have faith that, over the long haul, some semblance of equity will fall into place.

20
Q

How did the study by Clark, Mills, and Corocoran (which involved flashing lights) demonstrate the distinction between an exchange and a communal relationship?

The Gain and Loss of Esteem: The Quest for Communal Relationships

A
  • In this experiment, each participant was paired with either a close friend or a stranger. They were then led into a room and told that their partner would be performing some tasks. For half the participants, they were told that the lights would flash if the partner was doing well, meaning they would both eventually get a shared rewared. The other half was told that the lights would flash if their partner needed help.
  • Researchers looked at how often the participants looked at the lights. If the partner was a stranger (exchange relationship), they spent far more time looking at the lights when they were told that it meant they might be getting a reward; if the partner was a close friend (communal relationship), they spent far more time looking at the lights when they thought it meant their partner might need help.
21
Q

What are the two most important factors in determining who falls in love, and why?

Love and Intimacy: What Do We Know About Love?

A
  • The biggest factor is proximity: those who are geographically nearest to you are most likely to become dearest to you, as well.
  • The second most important factor is similarity: we fall in love with people who look like us, and who have similar values, attitudes, beliefs, and personalities.
22
Q

Define:

passionate love

Love and Intimacy: Defining Love

A

It’s characterized by strong emotions, sexual desire, and intense preoccupation with the beloved. Its onset is usually rapid rather than gradual, and, alas, almost inevitably, its fiery intensity cools over time.

23
Q

Define:

companionate love

Love and Intimacy: Defining Love

A

A milder, more stable experience marked by feelings of mutual trust, dependability, and warmth. Compared with the typically short-lived intensity of romantic passion, companionate love generally lasts longer and deepens over time.

24
Q

What is the:

triangle of love

Love and Intimacy: Defining Love

A

This theory was proposed by Sternberg, and he proposes that love consists of three components: passion (euphoria and sexual excitement), intimacy (feeling free to talk about anything, feeling close to and understood by the loved one), and commitment (needing to be with the other person, feeling loyal). Love can consist of one component alone or of any combination of these three parts.

25
Q

What are the two types of love that Sternberg says develops in a long-lasting, commited relationship?

Love and Intimacy: Defining Love

A
  • When pure passion blossoms into a combination of passion and intimacy, Sternberg calls this romantic love.
  • In Sternberg’s system, the ultimate goal is consummate love—the blending of all three components. But this is achieved only rarely.
  • The implication of the triangle of love is that, as a loving couple becomes increasingly accustomed to each other, there is the strong possibility that passion is likely to become the victim of routine and they may get stuck in a companionate state. It’s not a terrible place to be stuck, but it falls short of the ideal—consummate love.
26
Q

Why are long-term relationships not very rewarding, and why do we tend to hurt the ones we love the most?

Love and Intimacy: Gain-Loss Theory and its Implications for Close Relationships

A
  • One possibility is that, once we have grown certain of the rewarding behavior of our long-term partner, that person may become less powerful as a source of reward than a stranger. A long-term lover or spouse is probably behaving near ceiling level and, therefore, cannot provide us with much of a gain.
  • To put it another way, once we have learned to expect love, support, and praise from a mate, such behavior is not likely to represent a gain in that person’s esteem for us.
  • By the same token, a loved one has great potential to hurt us. The closer the relationship and the greater the past history of invariant esteem and reward, the more devastating is the withdrawal of that person’s esteem. In effect, then, the long-term lover has power to hurt the one he or she loves—but very little power to offer an important reward.
27
Q

How did Floyd demonstrate the impact that gain or loss has on how individuals respond to close friends or strangers (using her study with trinkets)?

Love and Intimacy: Gain-Loss Theory and its Implications for Close Relationships

A
  • In this experiment, each child was paired with either a friend or a stranger. Each pair played games in which they earned trinkets. They were instructed to share these with their partner. The perceived stinginess of the sharer was manipulated by the experimenter. Some children were led to believe that the friend/stranger was being generous, and others that the friend/stranger was being stingy.
  • Each “receiving” child was then allowed to earn several trinkets and asked to share them with their partner. They were relatively stingy to stingy strangers and generous to friends.
  • The children showed the most generosity in the gain and loss conditions—that is, they gave more trinkets to generous strangers, when it looked as though they might be gaining a friend, and stingy friends, when it looked as though they might be losing one.
28
Q

What is the most important factor as a relationship becomes more intimate?

Love and Intimacy: Gain-Loss Theory and its Implications for Close Relationships

A

Authenticity: our ability to give up trying to make a good impression and begin to reveal things about ourselves that are honest, even if unsavory. It implies a willingness to communicate a wide range of feelings to our friends and loved ones, under appropriate circumstances and in ways that reflect our caring.

29
Q

What is:

straight talk

Intimacy, Authenticity, and Communication

A

A person’s clear statement of his or her feelings and concerns without accusing, blaming, judging, or ridiculing the other person. Straight talk is effective precisely because it enables the recipient to listen nondefensively.

30
Q

Why don’t people use straight talk more often?

Intimacy, Authenticity, and Communication

A
  • Growing up in a competitive society, most of us have learned how to protect ourselves by making ourselves relatively invulnerable. Thus, when we are hurt, we have learned not to show it. Rather, we have learned either to avoid the person who hurt us or to lash out at him or her with anger, judgment, or ridicule, which in turn makes the other person defensive or produces a counterattack, and the argument escalates.
  • This strategy may be useful and in some situations even essential, but in many circumstances it is inappropriate, dysfunctional, and counterproductive.
  • As well, in many situations, people aren’t fully aware of their own needs, wants, and feelings. Instead, they may have a vague feeling of discomfort or unhappiness that they can’t easily pinpoint. Often, they misattribute that vague feeling.
31
Q

Why must feelings be expressed directly and openly?

Characteristics of Effective Communication: The Importance of Immediacy

A
  • So that the receiver can gain immediate feedback on how their words and behaviours are being interpreted. They are better able to gain insight into the impact of our actions and statements to consider their options for meeting their own needs, as well as their partner’s.
    • The receiver can either continue to behave in that way, or stop behaving in that way. The behavior may be so important they don’t want to give it up. Conversely, the other person’s feelings may be so important that they choose to give up the behavior. In the absence of knowledge about how the behaviour made the other person feel, there is no choice.
  • Frequently, in providing feedback, the sharer discovers something about themselves and their own needs.
  • The direct expression of a feeling keeps it from escalating, having to be supressed, and then exploding at a different time when the receiver isn’t even doing anything wrong but still receiving this risidual anger.
32
Q

How can we be open and critical but minimize the hurt we do unto others at the same time?

Characteristics of Effective Communication: Feelings Versus Judgment

A
  • The key to effective communication rests on our willingness to express feelings rather than judgments. Saying something like, “I feel that you’re being a jerk,” isn’t actually expressing a feeling, but a judgment expressed in the terminology of feelings.
  • When we feel annoyed at someone, we have to stop and ask how we are feeling. Often times, our hostility towards others is just masked jealousy or insecurity. By admitting our own feelings instead of judging others, we come to understand ourselves a lot better.
33
Q

How did Fincham and Bradbury demonstrate the importance of straight talk in consummate love?

Characteristics of Effective Communication: Communication and Consummate Love

A
  • When lovers express their feelings without judging the other person as being wrong, insensitive, or uncaring, escalation rarely follows.
  • Researchers studied 130 newly married couples over time and found that those couples who made dispositional attributions early in their marriages became increasingly unhappy with their spouses. In contrast, couples who engaged in straight talk and made situational attributions became increasingly happy with their marriages.