Attachment Flashcards
What are attachments?
A close, reciprocal, emotional relationship and bond between two people, characterised by mutual affection and the desire to maintain proximity. Each sees the other as essential for their own emotional security.
Why do infants form attachments?
Short term benefits:
- children are helpless at birth so need caregivers
- caregivers provide food / protection / safety
- desire for closeness will promote survival
Long term benefits:
- enable us to learn how to behave in relationships
- we learn how to behave in relationships
- we learn how a parent should behave
List 3 behaviours that demonstrate attachment
Proximity
Separation distress
Secure-base behaviour
What is reciprocity?
Where an infant responds to the action of another person, or where the actions of one partner elicits a response from the other
What is interactional synchrony
Where an infant mirrors the actions, for example, an infant imitating behaviours and/or the emotions of caregivers
Outline Meltzoff and Moore’s study on infant-caregiver interactions
Aim - to examine caregiver-infant interactions
Method - an adult model would display one of 3 facial expressions or hand movements. The child’s response would be filmed.
Results - there was an association between infant behaviour and the adult model
Evaluate infant-caregiver interactions
A limitation of this research is there are problems with testing infant behaviour. Infants’ mouths are in fairly consistent motion and the expressions that are tested occur frequently, which makes it difficult to distinguish between general activity and imitated behaviour. This means that we cannot be certain that the behaviours seen in caregiver-infant interactions have a special meaning.
A further limitation is that simply observing a behaviour does not tell us its developmental importance. Feldman points out that ideas like synchrony and reciprocity simply give names to patterns of observable caregiver and baby behaviours. These are robust phenomena in the sense that they can be reliably observed, but they still may not tell us the purpose of these behaviours. This means that we cannot be certain from observational research alone that reciprocity and synchrony are important for a child’s development. However, there is evidence from other lines of research to suggest that early interactions are important. For example, Isabella et al found that achievement of interactional synchrony predicted the development of a good quality attachment. This means that, on balance, caregiver-infant interaction is probably important in development.
A limitation of this study is that other studies have failed to replicate the findings of these studies. A study by Koepke et al failed to replicate these findings. Marian et al replicated a similar study and found infants couldn’t distinguish live from videotaped interactions with their mothers, suggesting the infants are not actually responding to their caregivers. Therefore, the earlier studies’ findings were not replicated in the later studies.
One strength of research on this topic is that caregiver-infant interactions are usually filmed in a lab. This means that other activity, that might distract a baby, can be controlled. Furthermore, filming means that observations can be analysed later, making it unlikely that researchers will miss seeing key behaviours. In addition, having filmed interactions means that more than one observer can record data and establish the inter-rater reliability of observations. Finally, babies don’t know they are being observed, so their behaviour doesn’t change in response to observation. Therefore, the data collected in such research should have good reliability and validity.
Outline the role of the father
- Grossman - a longitudinal study. Looked at both parents behaviour and its relationship to the quality of children’s attachment into their teens
- Quality of infant attachments with mothers related to children’s attachment in adolescence but not fathers, suggesting the father is less important
- However, the quality of fathers play in infancy was related to the quality of adolescence attachments
- Suggests fathers have a role to do with play and stimulation rather than nurturing
- In Shaffer and Emerson’s study, fathers were the first joint attachment figure in 35% of infants - fathers are important
Outline fathers as primary carers
- When fathers take on the role as primary caregiver, they adopt behaviors more typical of mothers
- Field filmed 4-month old babies in interactions with caregiver mothers, caregiver father, and secondary caregiver fathers.
- Caregiver fathers, like mothers, spent more time smiling, imitating, and holding infants than secondary caregiver fathers
- This behavior is very important in building an attachment with the infant
- The key is the level of responsiveness in the parent, not the gender
Why aren’t children without fathers different if fathers have a distinct role?
- Grossman found fathers as secondary attachments have an important role
- However, studies have found children that grow up in same-sex families do not develop any differently from those in heterosexual families, suggestion fathers role as secondary attachment figure is not important
Outline research that suggests fathers are important as attachment figures
Shaffer and Emerson’s research showed fathers are important secondary attachment figure. They found that the majority of babies formed secondary attachments to other family members, including fathers, a few weeks or months after the mother. In 75% of infants studied, an attachment was formed with the father by 18 months.
Grossman suggested a fathers role is different, but equally important. The quality of a fathers play wit infants was related to the quality of adolescent attachments, suggesting fathers have a different role, that is more to do with play and stimulation and less to do with nurturing.
Field’s research showed that the gender of the person in the primary caregiver role is less important than responsiveness. Primary caregiver fathers, like mothers, spent more time smiling, imitation, and holding infants than secondary caregiver fathers. This shows fathers can be the more nurturing attachment figure. The key to the attachment relationship is the level of responsiveness, not gender.
Outline research against the idea that fathers are important in attachment
MacCallum and Golombok highlight that the fathers role must be optional. They found that children growing up in single or same-sex parent families do not develop differently from those in two parent heterosexual families, which suggests the fathers role as secondary attachment figure is not important.
Grossman found differences suggesting he father attachment was less important. The quality of infant attachment with mothers but not fathers was related to children’s attachments in adolescence.
Taylor suggests that father lack the higher levels of oestrogen needed to be truly nurturing caregivers. Female hormones, such as oestrogen, create higher levels of nurturing and therefore women are biologically predisposed to be the primary attachment figure.
Evaluate the role of the father
A limitation to research is the inconsistent findings on the role of the father in attachment, which may be due to researchers being interested in different research questions. Some psychologists are interested in understanding the role of fathers as secondary attachment figures whereas others are more concerned with fathers as primary caregivers. This is a limitation as it makes it difficult to offer a simple answer as to the ‘role of the father’. It really depends on what specific role is being discussed.
A limitation of research into the role of fathers is that findings vary according to the methodology used. Longitudinal studies such as that of Grossman et al have suggested that fathers as secondary attachment figures have an important and distinct role in their children’s development, involving play and stimulation. However, if fathers have a distinctive and important role we would expect that children growing up in single-mother and lesbian-parent families would turn out different from those in two-parent heterosexual families. In fact, studies consistently show that these children do not develop differently from children in ‘normal’ families. This means the question as to whether fathers have a distinctive role remains unanswered. However, these lines of research may not be in conflict. It could be that fathers typically take on distinctive roles in 2 parent heterosexual families, but that parents in single-mother and lesbian-parent families simply adapt to accommodate the role played by fathers. This means that the question of a distinctive role for fathers is clear after all. When present, fathers adopt a distinctive role, but families can adapt to not having a father.
A limitation of this research is that it doesn’t explain why fathers don’t generally become primary attachment figures. This could be the result of traditional gender roles in which women are expected to be more caring and nurturing than men, and therefore men don’t feel like they should act like that, or it could be because of female hormones such as oestrogen, which create high levels of nurturing and therefore females are biologically predisposed to be the primary attachment figure.
Describe Schaffers 4 stages of attachment
Asocial (0-8 weeks):
- An infant shows similar responses to objects and people, although they do display a preference for faces/eyes
Indiscriminate attachment (2-7 months):
- An infant shows a preference for human company over non-human company
- Have the ability to distinguish between different people, but are comforted indiscriminately
Specific/discriminate attachment (7-12 months):
- An infant shows a preference for one caregiver, displaying separation and stranger anxiety
- Looks to a particular person for security, comfort, and protection
- Uses familiar adults as a safe base
Multiple attachments (1 year onwards):
- Attachment behaviours are displayed towards several different people like siblings, grandparents..
Outline Schaffer and Emerson’s study on the formation of early attachments
Aim - to examine the formation of early attachments
Method - 60 babies ( 31 male, 29 female) from working class families in glasgow. Visited the babies in their home every month for the first 12 months, and then again at 18 months. They interviewed the mothers and observed the babies for separation and stranger anxiety.
Results - at 25-32 weeks 50% of the children showed separation anxiety towards their mothers, as expected with the discriminant attachment stage. By 40 weeks 80% of the children had a discriminate attachment and 30% had started to form multiple attachments
Conclusion - the results provide some support for Schaffer’s stages of attachment.
Evaluate Schaffer and Emerson’s research on the formation of early attachments
One limitation of Schaffer and Emerson’s stages is the validity of the measures they used to assess attachment in the asocial stage. Young babies have poor coordination and are fairly immobile. If babies less than 2 months old felt anxiety in everyday situations they might have displayed this in quite subtle, hard to observe ways. This made it difficult for mothers to observe and report back to researchers on signs of anxiety and attachment in this age group. This means that babies may actually be quite social but, because of flawed methods, they appear to be asocial.
A limitation of this study is that the sample was biased. Firstly, the sample was from a working class population so the findings may not apply to other social groups. Secondly, the sample was from the 1960s, and parental care has changed a lot since then. Therefore, if a similar study was conducted today, the findings may well be different.
One strength of Schaffer and Emerson’s research is that it has good external validity. Most of the observations were made by parents during ordinary activities and reported to the researchers. The alternative would have been to have researchers present to record observations. This might have distracted the babies or made them feel more anxious. This means it is highly likely that the participants behaved naturally while being observed. On the other hand, there are issues with asking the mothers to be the ‘observers’. They were unlikely to be objective observers. They might have been biased in terms of what they noticed when their baby was showing signs of anxiety or they may have misremembered it. This means that even if babies behaved naturally, their behaviour may not have actually been recorded.
Outline Lorenz’s research (1935)
Method:
- He took some gosling eggs and divided them into 2 groups
- One group was left with their mother while the other eggs were left in the incubator
- When the eggs hatched the first living thing they saw was Lorenz
- He then put the 2 groups back together
Findings:
- The goslings quickly divided themselves up, one group following their natural mother and the other one following Lorenz
- Lorenz noted that imprinting is restricted to a critical period
Conclusion:
- Imprinting is a form of attachment exhibited mainly in bird species that are mobile from birth
- The first 12-17 hours are the critical period
What is imprinting?
An innate readiness to develop a strong bond with the mother which takes place during a specific time in development. If it doesn’t happen in this time it probably won’t happen at all
Outline Harlow’s research (1959)
Aim:
- To demonstrate that attachment was not based on the feeding bond between mother and infant
Method:
- Harlow created 2 wire mothers each with a different head. one was wrapped in soft cloth
- He used 16 rhesus baby monkeys
- In one condition milk was dispensed by the plain wire mother and the opposite in the other
Findings:
- The monkeys cuddled the soft mother over the wire one and sought comfort from the cloth one when frightened, regardless of which one dispensed the milk
Conclusions:
- Contact comfort is more important to the monkeys when it came to attachment behaviour
Harlow continued to study his monkeys as they grew up and noted the consequences of their early attachment behaviour:
- more aggressive and less sociable
- bred less often than typical monkeys
- unskilled at mating
- neglected their young
- some attacked or even killed their children
Evaluate animal studies of attachment
A strength of Harlow’s research is it can be generalised to humans. Some psychologists argue that humans and monkeys are not all that different. Green states that, on a biological level at least, all mammals have the same brain structure as humans. Therefore, the findings of Harlow’s research may provide some insight into human behaviour, due to the similarity between humans and monkeys. However, it is also argued that it is unlikely that observations of monkeys clinging to cloth mothers reflects the emotional connections and interactions that characterise humans. Therefore, we may be unable to extrapolate the findings from animal studies to humans, limiting the application of Harlow’s research.
A limitation of animal studies is that they are often unethical. It could be argued that animals have a right not to be researched on / harmed and the monkeys raised in Harlow’s experiment all displayed dysfunctional adult behaviour. Therefore, such research is unethical which makes the purpose and integrity of animal research questionable.
A strength of Harlow’s research is that it has important practical applications. Harlow’s research has had profound implications for childcare, especially in social care. Due to the importance of early experiences in long-term development which he discovered, it is vital that children have contact comfort, and their needs are catered for; taking care of just a child’s physical health is not sufficient. Harlow’s research has helped social workers and clinical psychologists understand that a lack of bonding experience may be a risk factor in child development, allowing them to intervene to prevent poor outcomes. This means that the value of Harlow’s research is not just theoretical but also practical.
What is the learning theory of attachment?
Classical conditioning:
In the case of attachment, food serves as an unconditional stimulus. Pleasure is the unconditioned response. The caregiver/feeder is the neutral stimulus. When the same person provides food over a long period of time, they become associated with food, and the caregiver becomes the conditioned stimulus with pleasure being the conditioned response.
Operant conditioning:
Operant conditioning explains why babies cry for comfort, which is an important behaviour in building attachment. Crying leads to a response from the caregiver, for example feeding. Crying is then positively reinforced. This reinforcement is a 2 way process, and the baby stopping crying acts as a negative reinforcer for caregiver. This interplay of mutual reinforcement strengthens an attachment.
Attachment as a secondary drive:
Learning theory draws on the concept of drive reduction. Hunger is a primary drive, as it is an innate, biological motivator. We are motivated to eat to reduce the hunger drive. Sears et al suggested that, as caregivers provide food, the primary drive of hunger becomes generalised to them. Attachment is thus a secondary drive learned by an association between the caregiver and the satisfaction of a primary drive.
Evaluate the learning theory as an explanation of attachment formation
A limitation of this explanation of attachment is there is contradicting evidence from animal studies. Harlow found that the monkeys in his study tended to cling to the wire mother covered in cloth over the plain wire mother, even when the plain wire mother was the one with the bottle. The monkeys also went to the cloth mothers when scared. This shows that the vital thing in attachment is not food but contact comfort. This goes against the learning theory, which states that babies form attachments for food. Lorenz also had opposing research. Lorenz found that baby geese imprint on the first moving object they see, rather than who feeds them and maintains this attachment regardless of who feeds them. Therefore, this suggests that food isn’t the key factor in the formation of attachment.
A limitation of this explanation is Schaffer and Emerson’s study refutes it. They found that infants became attached to caregivers who spent the most time with them, were the most responsive to them, and nurtured them. This goes against the learning theory that states babies form attachments for food.
A strength of the learning theory is it can explain some aspects of attachment. Infants do learn through association and reinforcement, but food may not be the reinforcer. It may be the attention and responsiveness from a caregiver that are the rewards that assist the formation of attachments. Therefore the learning theory may still be useful in understanding the development of attachments. However, both classical and operant conditioning explanations see the baby playing a relatively passive role in attachment development, simply responding to associations with comfort or reward. In fact, research shows that babies take a very active role in the interactions that produce attachment. This means that conditioning may not be an adequate explanation of ant aspect of attachment.
What is Bowlby’s monotropic theory?
Adaptive:
- Attachments are adaptive. Humans have an innate tendency to form attachments with a caregiver
- They give our species an adaptive advantage, making our species more likely to survive
Social releasers:
- Babies have social releases, which unlock the innate tendency in adults to care for them
- Social releases can be physical: baby face features and body proportions, or behavioural: crying, cooing, smiling
Monotropy:
- Bowlby believed that infants form one very special attachment with their mother
- This special attachment is called monotropy
- If the mother isn’t available, the infant could bond with another present mother figure
- The monotropic attachment is qualitatively different from other attachments
The law of continuity -> the more constant and predictable a child’s care, the better the quality of their attachment
The law of accumulated separation -> the effects of every separation from the mother add up and ‘the safest dose is therefore a zero dose’ (Bowlby)
Critical period:
- Babies have to form the attachment with their caregiver during a critical period, which is between birth and 2.5 years old
- The sensitive period is 6 months
- Bowlby said if this didn’t happen the child would be damaged for life; socially, emotionally, intellectually, and physically
Internal working model:
- Throughout the monotropic attachment, the baby will form an internal working model
- This is a mental schema for relationships
- Your first attachment forms a template for adult relationships
- Most importantly, the internal working model affects the child’s later ability to be a parent themselves
- People tend to base their parenting behaviour on their own experiences