Aggression Flashcards
Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression
The limbic system
- is the subcortical structure in the brain which is associated with the regulation of emotion and emotional behavior including aggression
Maclean identified that it is comprised of the fornix, hypothalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, cingulate gyrus
- the amygdala is thought to be particularly important in regulating emotional behavior as demand Gospic et al in her study of ultimatum game
- the researcher found that when participants rejected unfair monetary reward there was a sudden increase in in amygdala activity = measured using FMRI
They also found that a Benzodiazepine drug ( which reduced arousal of the autonomic nervous system) taken before the game had 2 effects in responses to unfair offers
It halves the number of rejections ( reduced aggression) and decrease the activity of the amygdala.= strong evidence of an association between relative aggression and amygdala activity
Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression
Serotonin
- is a neurotransmitter involved in communication of impulses between neurons
It has widespread inhibitory effect on the brain= slows down neuronal activity
Normal level of serotonin in the orbitofrontal cortex are linked with reduced firing of the neuron and is associated with greater degree of behavioral self control
Decrease serotonin may well disturb this mechanism, reducing self control and leading to an increase impulsive behaviour including aggression ( Denson et al)
Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression
Hormonal mechanism in aggression
Testosterone
- it is a reliable observation that males are generally more aggressive than females due to testosterone
Definition : it is a hormone from the androgen group that is produced mainly in the male testes and is associated with aggressiveness
Animal studies such as Giammanco demonstrated experimental increase in testosterone are related to greater aggressive behavior in several species
Human studies ; Dolan et al found a positive correlation between testosterone level and aggressive behaviors in a sample of 60 male offenders in UK maximum security hospitals
These men mostly suffered from personality disorders and had histories of impulsively violent behavior
Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression
Evaluation
Role of other brain structures
- recent studies indicate that amygdala does not operate in isolation in determine aggression
Appears orbitofrontal cortex is thought to be involved in self control, impulsive regulation and inhibition of aggressive behaviour
Coccaro et al found that in patient with psychiatric disorders that prominently feature aggression = activity in OFC is reduced, disrupting it impulsive control function= lead to increase aggression
- effects on drugs on setting
- drugs that increase serotonin activity also reduce levels of aggressive behaviour
BERMAN et al - gave ps either a placebo or a dose of paroxetine= drug that enhance serotonin activity
Ps then took part in a lab based game in which electric shocks of varying intensity were given and received - the paroxetine ps consistently gave fewer and less intense shocks than those in the placebo group
This is only true of the ps who had prior history of aggressive behavior - study is useful evidence of link between setting and aggression
Other hormones
- evidence of the link between testosterone and aggression in human is mixed
Carre and Mehta dual hormone hypothesis attempts explains why
They claim high level of testosterone = lead to aggression only when level of cortisol is low
When it is high = testosterone influence in aggression is blocked
Cortisol is. Hormone they plays central role in stress response
- therefore combined activity of testosterone and cortisol may be a better predictor of human aggression than either hormone alone
Most research in both hormonal and neural influence on aggression is correlational
- there’s good ethical reason as opportunities to experimentally manipulate brain structures and hormones is limited
But is impossible to establish which one is the cause of the other
Genetic factors in aggression
Twin studies
Several twin studies have suggested that heritability accounts for about 50% of the variance in aggressive behaviour. E.g Coccaro et al. studied adult male monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins.
Because MZ twins share 100% of their genes but DZ twins share only 50% we would expect to find greater similarities in aggressive behaviour between MZ twins if aggression is mostly influenced by genetic factors= both MZ and DZ twins are raised together in the same environment, but MZ twins have a greater degree of genetic similarity than DZs.
For aggressive behaviour defined as direct physical assault, the researchers found concordance rates of 50% for MZ twins and 19% for DZs. The corresponding figures for verbal aggression were 28% (MZs) and 7% (DZs).
Genetic factors in aggression
Adoption studies
Similarities in aggressive behaviour between an adopted child and their biological parents suggest that genetic influences are operating. Similarities with the adopted parents suggest that environmental influences are operating.
So Rhee and Irwin Waldman (2002) carried out a meta-analysis of adoption studies of direct aggression and antisocial behaviour, a prominent feature of which is aggressive behaviour.
They found that genetic influences accounted for 41% of the variance in aggression
Genetic factors in aggression
the MAOA gene
The gene responsible for the activity of the enzyme monoamine oxidase A in the brain
MAOA is an enzyme
- it is done by breaking down the neurotransmitter especially serotonin into constituent chemical to be recycled.
- dysfunction in the operation of this gene may lead to abnormal activity of the MAOA enzyme= affected level of serotonin in brain
The low activity variant of the gene is closely associated with aggressive behaviour
- Brinner et al studies 28 male member of a large Dutch family who were repeatedly involved in impulsive aggressive violent criminal behaviors as rape, attempted murder
Researcher found that these men had abnormally low level of MAOA in their brains and low activity version of the MAOA gene
Genetic factors in aggression
Gene - environment interaction
Genes are crucial influences on aggressive behaviour but they do not function in isolation. It appears to be the case that low MAOA gene activity is only related to adult aggression when combined with early traumatic life events.
Giovanni Frazzetto et al. found an association between higher levels of antisocial aggression and the low-activity MAOA gene variant in adult males, But this was only the case in those who had experienced significant trauma (such as sexual or physical abuse) during the first 15 years of life
.
Those who had not experienced such childhoo trauma did not have particularly high levels of aggression as adults, even if they possessed the low-activity MAOA variant.
This is strong evidence of a gene-environment interaction (sometimes described as diathesis-stress).
Genetic factor in aggression
Evaluation
Evaluation
Isolating genetic factors
challenging to establish how influential genes are in aggressive behaviour. One major reason for this is the difficulty researchers have in separating genetic and environmental factors, as we saw at GxE interactions. An individual may possess a gene associated with aggression, but that behaviour is only expressed if the environmental conditions are favourable.
McDermott et al. (2009) showed that participants with the low-activity MAOA gene behaved aggressively in a laboratory-based money-allocation game, but only when they were provoked. Otherwise, they were no more or less aggressive than other participants.
Methods of measuring aggression differ significantly between studies, and include self-reports, parent and teacher reports, and direct observations.
In Rhee and Waldman’s meta-analysis of 51 twin and adoption studies, genetic factors had a greater influence on aggression in studies using self-reports rather than parent or teacher reports.
If research findings vary depending upon how aggression is measured, then it becomes very difficult to draw valid conclusions about the role of genetic factors
Research support for role of MAOA gene
- mertins et al studied ps with low activity and high activity of the MAOA gene in a money distributing game
Ps had to make decisions about whether or not to contribute money for the good of the group
Found that males with high activity variant were more co- operative and made fewer aggressive moves than low activity ps
- other better explanation
The ethical explanation of aggression
Ethological explanations suggest that the main function of aggression is adaptive. That is, aggression is beneficial to survival because a ‘defeated’ animal is rarely killed , but rather forced to establish territory elsewhere
. This means that members of a species spread out over a wid area and have to discover resources in a different place, which reduces competition pressure and th possibility of starvation.
Another adaptive function of aggression is to establish dominance hierarchies. Male chimpanzes use aggression to climb their troop’s social hierarchy.
Their dominance gives them special status (for example, mating rights over females).
This happens in humans too. Gregory Pettit et al. (1988) who found that young children use aggressive tactics in playground to asset their authority, lead the others and have their way.
This would be adaptive as dominance over other brings benefits such as power to get your own way
The ethical explanation of aggression
Ritualistic aggression
A ritual is a series of behaviours carried out in a set order. One of Lorenz’s intriguing early observations of fights between animals of the same species was how little actual physical damage was done.
Most aggressive encounters consisted mainly of a period of ritualistic signalling (e.g. displaying claws an teeth, facial expressions of threat) and rarely reached the point of becoming physical.
Furthermore, Lorenz pointed out that intra-species aggressive confrontations end with ritual appeasement displays= indicate acceptance of defeat and inhibit aggressive behaviour in the victor, preventing any damage to the loser.
The ethical explanation of aggression
Innate releasing mechanism and fixed action patterns
An innate releasing mechanism (IRM) is a biological structure or process which is activated by an external stimulus that in turn triggers a FAP
An environmental stimulus (such as a certain facial expression) triggers the iRM which then ‘releases’ a specific sequence of behaviours. This behavioural sequence is called a fixed action pattern (FAP). According to Stephen Lea (1984), FAPs have six main features. They are:
• Stereotyped, or relatively unchanging sequences of behaviours.
• Universal, because the same behaviour is found in every individual of a species.
• Unaffected by learning, the same for every individual regardless of experience.
• ‘Ballistic, once the behaviour is triggered it follows an inevitable course and cannot be altered before it is completed.
• Single-purpose, the behaviour only occurs in a specific situation and not in any other.
• A response to an identifiable specific sign stimulus
FAP- a sequence of stereotyped pre programmed behavior triggered by an innate releasing mechanism
The ethnological explanation of aggression
Key study: research into IRM and FAP
Procedures :Male sticklebacks are highly territorial during the spring mating season, when they also levelop a red spot on their underbelly.
If another male enters their territory, a sequence of highly-tereotyped aggressive behaviours is initiated (a FAP).
The sign stimulus that triggers the innate eleasing mechanism is the sight of the red spot. Niko Tinbergen (1951) presented sticklebacks with a eries of wooden models of different shapes.
Findings
Regardless of shape, if the model had a red spot the stickleback would aggressively display and even attack it. But if there was no red spot, there was no aggression, even if the model looked realistically like a stickleback. Tinbergen also found that these aggressive FAPs were unchanging from one encounter to another.
The ethnological explanation of aggression
Evaluation
Evidence against ritualistic aggression
challenged by observations made by Jane Goodall (2010) of chimpanzees at the Gombe Stream National Park in Tanzania. male chimps from one community set about systematically slaughtering all the members of another group. They did this in a co-ordinated and premeditated fashion.
others hit and bit it in an attack lasting up to 20 minutes. The violence continued like this despite the fact that the victims were offering signals of appeasement and defencelessness. These signals did not inhibit the aggressive behaviour of the attacking chimps as predicted by the ethological explanation.
Unjustified generalization to humans
Lorenz and Tibergen both made generalization about aggressive behaviour in humans including warfare
Lorenz also extrapolated from the behaviour of individual animals to the behaviour of the entire countries and states
= not valid
+ There is also evidence supporting the biological, innate basis of IRM and FAP systems. For example, researchers have pointed to Bremner’s work (1993) on the link between the MAOA activity and levels of aggression, as evidence for the heritability of IRM and FAP systems. This is due to aggressive behaviour being triggered by increased levels of testosterone which must have been preceded by exposure to a releaser or signal, which had triggered the IRM.
However, a more accurate description of FAPs may be ‘modal’ rather than fixed’, as , as suggested by Hunt (1973). The researcher provided evidence that the duration of each behaviour hin each FAP may vary between individuals as well as the specific other animal towärds which s targeted. Hence, environmental and social factors may have significant influences on the urse of the FAP, resulting in lower validity of the universal nature of FAPs as part of an planation for aggression.
Evolutionary explanation human aggression
Evaluation
+ Evolutionary theories are useful because they can provide an explanation for gender differences in aggression.
For example, Campbell (1999) suggested that females are more likely to engage in acts of verbal, as opposed to physical, aggression as this ensures that their own survival, as well as the survival of their offspring, is not endangered.
Such tactics also prevents females from being involved in life-threatening physical confrontations with their partners,
and so further increases their chance of survival through the use of non-aggressive methods of resolving conflicts =increases the validity of the evolutionary explanation of aggression.
- methodological issues associated with the use of evolutionary theories to explain current examples of aggression.
, most studies are correlational, meaning that there is only a correlation between aggression and the use of male retention strategies.
means that the research may suffer from the ‘third factor problem’,= may be a third contributory factor which has not been studied.= jump to make causal conclusions, when really correlations can never demonstrate a ‘cause and effect’ relationship
+ However, there is research supporting the link between sexual jealousy and aggression.
The main example of this would be Shackleford’s 2005 study which found that male retention strategies are a method of expressing sexual jealousy, which leads to aggressive behaviour both towards females and other partners.
This increases the reliability of evolutionary theories as a method of explaining aggression, due to this supporting evidence.
Cultural differences
The !Kung San people of the Kalahari have very negative attitudes towards the use of aggression. Aggressive behaviour is discouraged from childhood and is therefore rare.
Those who do use it find that their status and reputation within the community are diminished. The Yanomamo of Venezuela and Brazil have been described as ‘the fierce people’= it appears that aggression is an accepted and required behaviour in order to gain status in their highly structured society.
Evolutionary explanation of human aggression
Evolutionary explanation of sexual jealousy
There are characteristics that enhance survival and reproduction are naturally selected
- sexual jealously is a major motivator of aggressive behavior in males compared to females due to paternity uncertainty, which may lead to cuckoldry I.e. male raised a son which is not his own
This is an evolutionary disadvantage as the male wasting his resource which he could have otherwise used in raising his own children
Therefore anti cuckoldry behaviour In The form of male retentive strategies are adaptive because it reduce the risk of cuckoldry. For instance sexual jealously = drives the form aggressive strategy men employ to retain their partners and prevent them from straying