3.3: The factors affecting attraction - Physical attractiveness Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks)

A

Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
Why is this?

A

This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also

A

People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, why?

A

People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

A

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.

A

The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists

A

Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis

A

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We

A

We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.

A

Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

First AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
Example

A

For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
What did Towhey find?

A

Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
What does this show?

A

This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

Second AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
Example

A

For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise

17
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
Example

A

For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters

18
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.

A

The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise

19
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

Third AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis

20
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
Example

A

For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction

21
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
What does this do?

A

This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid

22
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
What is a weakness here however?

A

A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like

23
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore,

A

Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’

24
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’.

Fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that despite this,

there is research to suggest that what is considered physically attractive is consistent across other cultures

25
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that despite this,
there is research to suggest that what is considered physically attractive is consistent across other cultures.
Example

A

For example, Cunningham et al. (1995) found that female features of large eyes, prominent cheekbones, a small nose and high eyebrows were rated as highly attractive by white, hispanic and Asian males

26
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that despite this,
there is research to suggest that what is considered physically attractive is consistent across other cultures.
For example, Cunningham et al. (1995) found that female features of large eyes, prominent cheekbones, a small nose and high eyebrows were rated as highly attractive by white, hispanic and Asian males.

A

The physical attractiveness stereotype is also culturally pervasive, as Wheeler and Kim (1997) found that Korean and American students judged physically attractive people to be more trustworthy, concerned for others, mature and friendly

27
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that despite this,
there is research to suggest that what is considered physically attractive is consistent across other cultures.
For example, Cunningham et al. (1995) found that female features of large eyes, prominent cheekbones, a small nose and high eyebrows were rated as highly attractive by white, hispanic and Asian males.
The physical attractiveness stereotype is also culturally pervasive, as Wheeler and Kim (1997) found that Korean and American students judged physically attractive people to be more trustworthy, concerned for others, mature and friendly.
What does this show?

A

This shows that the stereotype is just as strong in collectivist cultures as it is in individualist cultures

28
Q

Discuss physical attractiveness as a factor affecting attraction in romantic relationships (16 marks).
Physical attractiveness refers to how appealing we find someone and one explanation for this is an evolutionary theory by Shackleford and Larsen (1997), which is that we find symmetrical faces more attractive.
This is because it may be an honest signal of genetic fitness (it’s difficult to fake facial symmetry).
People are also attracted to faces with neotenous (baby face) features such as widely separated and large eyes, a delicate chin and a small nose, because these trigger a protective or caring instinct, a valuable resource for females wanting to reproduce.

Dion et al. found that physically attractive people are consistently rated as kind, strong, sociable and successful compared to unattractive people.
The belief that good-looking people probably have these characteristics makes them even more attractive to us, so we behave positively towards them, which becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Psychologists use the term halo effect to describe this, how one distinguishing feature (in this case physical attractiveness) tends to have a disproportionate influence on our judgements of a person’s other attributes, for example, their personality.

The matching hypothesis is the belief that we do not select the most attractive person as a prospective partner, but instead are attracted to people who approximately ‘match’ us in physical (facial) attractiveness.
We assess our own attractiveness and choose someone with similar levels to avoid rejection.
Walster et al. called this ‘realistic choices,’ because individuals are influenced by the chances of having their feelings reciprocated back.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there are individual differences, as some people do not attach much importance to physical attractiveness.
For example, Towhey (1979) asked male and female participants to rate how much they would like a target individual based on their photograph and some biographical information.
The participants also completed a questionnaire, the MACHO scale, designed to measure sexist attitudes and behaviours.
Towhey found that the participants who were the most sexist were more influenced by the physical attractiveness of the target when making their judgement of likeability, whereas those who were less sexist were less sensitive to this influence.
This shows that the effects of physical attractiveness can be moderated by other factors and so challenges the matching hypothesis, which claims that physical attractiveness is always a significant consideration in relationship formation.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the halo effect.
For example, Palmer and Peterson (2012) found that physically attractive people were rated as more physically knowledgeable and competent than unattractive people.
This halo effect was so powerful that it persisted even when participants knew that these ‘knowledgeable’ people had no particular expertise.
For example, this has clear implications for the political process, because perhaps there are dangers for democracy if politicians are judged as suitable for office, merely because there are considered physically attractive by enough voters.
The existence of the halo effect has been found to apply in many other areas of everyday life, confirming that physical attractiveness is an important factor in the initial formation of relationships, romantic or otherwise.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that there is research support for the matching hypothesis.
For example, Walster (1969) paired students up for an upcoming dance telling them they had been paired dependent on their ideal partner, when in truth it was assigned randomly.
Students met up before the dance and those who had been paired with partners of similar levels of attraction to themselves reported to like their partner more than those paired at dissimilar levels of physical attraction.
This supports the matching hypothesis and so suggests it is valid.
A weakness here however is the subjective nature of how attraction is rated in Walster’s study, as this is likely to be based on Western ideals of what someone attractive looks like.
Therefore, this study could be argued to be culturally biased and the results invalid, because of the subjective nature of deciding which two people are ‘similar in attraction’.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that despite this,
there is research to suggest that what is considered physically attractive is consistent across other cultures.
For example, Cunningham et al. (1995) found that female features of large eyes, prominent cheekbones, a small nose and high eyebrows were rated as highly attractive by white, hispanic and Asian males.
The physical attractiveness stereotype is also culturally pervasive, as Wheeler and Kim (1997) found that Korean and American students judged physically attractive people to be more trustworthy, concerned for others, mature and friendly.
This shows that the stereotype is just as strong in collectivist cultures as it is in individualist cultures, suggesting what?

A

This shows that the stereotype is just as strong in collectivist cultures as it is in individualist cultures, suggesting it is universal and a valid element of relationship formation