10. Moral Development Flashcards

1
Q

Moral Judgment

A
  • Judgments about how people should interact with each other
  • Made based on principles concerning the distinction between right/wrong
    –> Justice
    –> Welfare
    –> Fairness
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Piaget’s Theory of Moral Reasoning

A
  • Development of moral judgment relies on cognitive development
    –> Changes in understanding of rules and the importance of intentions
  • Premoral Stage:
    –> 0-5 years old
    –> Little understanding of rules
  • Heteronomous Stage:
    –> 5-10 years old
    –> Rules are fixed
    –> Morality = obeying the rules of authority figures, like parents
    –> Intentions don’t matter
  • Autonomous Stage:
    –> 10+ years old
    –> Rules can be changed by social agreement
    –> Consider moral principles, like fairness, when deciding what is right and wrong
    –> Intentions matter
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Kohlberg’s Theory of Mind

A
  • Pre-conventional moral reasoning
  • Conventional moral reasoning
  • Post-conventional moral reasoning
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Preconventional Moral Reasoning

A
  • 3-7 years old
  • Moral reasoning based on external consequences
    –> Avoiding punishment from authority figures
    –> Gaining rewards
  • Similar to Piaget’s heteronomous morality
  • Heinz dilemma:
    –> “Heinz should not steal the drug because stealing is illegal and he could get caught and get in trouble.”
    –> “Heinz should steal the drug, because he needs it for his wife to get better , and then he can be happy.”
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Conventional Moral Reasoning

A
  • 8-13 years old
  • Moral reasoning based on adhering to social rules and expectations
    –> Maintaining social order and norms
    –> Gaining approval from others by fulfilling duties
  • Heinz dilemma:
    –> “Heinz should steal the drug because a good husband would do anything to save his wife’s life and people would understand why he did it.”
    –> “Heinz shouldn’t steal the drug because stealing is against the law. If everyone was stealing, society would fall apart.”
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Postconventional Moral Reasoning

A
  • 13 years old +
    –> But not everyone reaches this stage
  • Moral reasoning based on personal principles and values
    –> Focus on universal principles, like fairness, equality, justice
    –> Willingness to critically evaluate rules that conflict with universal principles
  • Similar to Piaget’s autonomous morality*Heinz dilemma:
    –> “Heinz should steal the drug because human life must be preserved and life is worth more than money or personal property.”
    –> “While stealing is generally wrong, the intention to save someone makes it justifiable in this case. Laws should be flexible enough to account for such situations.”
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Piaget and Kohlberg’s Contributions

A
  • First to acknowledge that moral reasoning changes systematically as children grow older due to cognitive development
  • Recognized that children are increasingly able to take intentions into account as they age
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Theory of Mind and Morality

A
  • Children are increasingly able to appreciate others’ intentions as they get older because of developments in theory of mind
  • Children who fail false belief tasks are more likely to ignore intentions when making moral judgments
  • Consistent with Piaget and Kohlberg’s ideas that cognitive development influences moral judgment
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Weaknesses of Piaget and Kohlberg’s Theories

A
  • Underestimated children’s ability to distinguish between social conventions and morality
  • Underestimated children’s ability to infer intention
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Distinguishing Between Social Conventions and Morality

A
  • Study: 2.5-years-olds asked about the morality of different actions
    –> Social convention violation: stories about a child breaking a rule (e.g. talking during naptime)
    –> Moral transgression: Stories about a child causing harm (e.g. hitting, stealing)
  • Results: Children differentiated between rule violations and harms
    –> Viewed both breaking rules and causing harm as bad but causing harm as worse
    –> Said that rules could be changed or not apply at a different school
    –> Causing harm is always wrong no matter what the rules said
    –> Children across the globe behaved the same way
  • Suggests that very young children can distinguish between social conventions and morality, much earlier than Piaget and Kohlberg thought
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Can Infants Appreciate Intentions?

A
  • Study: Can children appreciate intentions around 2 years of age?
  • 21-month-olds participated in a lab task with 2 adults
    –> Infants stood at a table with both adults
    –> Both adults offered to give the infant a toy by placing it at the edge of the table but ultimately the child didn’t get the toy
    *Negative intention: adult pulled the toy away
    *Positive intention: adult watched in surprise as the toy rolled away from the infant
    –> Then, experimenter presents both adults with a single new toy
    *Toy falls to the floor and both adults reach for it
  • Does the infant help? If so, which adult do they help?
    –> Evidence of selective helping
    –> Contrary to Piaget’s theory, suggests that 2 year olds are able to appreciate intentions when judging others’ actions
    –> 75% of children helped adults with positive intentions
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Two-Year-Olds Can Appreciate Intentions

A
  • But what if different outcomes of actions? Will infants still base decision to help on intentions?
  • Follow-up study to test this
  • Same procedure except:
    –> Positive intention but negative outcome: adult clearly offered the toy to the infant and watched in surprise as the toy rolled away
    –> Positive intention and positive outcome: adult clearly offered the toy to the infant and child was able to examine it
  • Then, experimenter presents both adults with a single new toy
    –> Toy falls to the floor and both adults reach for it
  • 2 year olds helped the adults equally (difference is not statistically sig.) suggesting that infants care more about intentions than outcomes of actions
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Evidence for Innate Morality

A
  • Study: Is moral judgment innate?
    –> 6 month-oldswatched a “morality play” in which a red puppet is trying to climb up a hill but fails
    *Helper: Yellow puppet comes and helps red puppet
    *Hinderer: Blue puppet pushes red puppet down
  • Results:
    –> Depending on the study, 75-100% of babies prefer the helper vs. hinderer
    –> Follow-up with 3-month-olds using preferential looking paradigm shows that they also prefer helper over hinder
  • Suggests that rudimentary moral judgment is innate
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Empathy and Morality

A
  • Morality is rooted in empathy and sympathy
    –> Empathy: Understanding and sharing the emotional state of another person
    –> Sympathy: Feeling of concern for another person in response to their emotional state
  • Perspective-taking -> Empathy -> Sympathy -> Prosocial behaviour
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Prosocial Behaviour in Infancy

A
  • Before 18 months: children tend to react to others’ distress with self-focused distress rather than prosocial behaviour
    –> E.g.cry inreaction to hearing another infant cry because it is aversive
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Prosocial Behaviour in Toddlers

A
  • 18-24 months: prosocial behaviour appears and increases throughout the 2nd year of life
    –> Toddlers show a natural tendency towards prosocial action
    *Spontaneously comfort others in distress, share belongings, and help others achieve goals
    *Actually help less if they gain a reward for it
  • Due to capacity to feel empathy and sympathy
    –> Facilitated by emergence of sense of self around 18 months of age
  • Suggests that prosocial behaviour is innate
17
Q

Evolutionary Roots of Prosocial Behaviour

A
  • Behaving prosocially increased our chances of survival and helped pass on our genes
    –> Helping forage for food and repelling enemies is easier as a group
    –> Those who are likely to be helped when they werein need
    –> Helping a relative, even if costly, benefits individual by ensuring the continuity of their genes
18
Q

Limits to Toddler’s Prosocial Behaviour

A
  • Toddlers help selectively depending on:
    –> how trustworthy, friendly, and helpful the other person is
    –> the type of help required
    *struggle to engage in helping that requires self-sacrifice
    –> whether the person is similar to them
    *E.g. More likely to help and share with a peer that is of the same gender
    *Do this even if parents insist that it’s wrong to treat people differently
  • Young children tend to reason in more moral ways when asked about fictitious scenarios compared to having to behave morally themselves
19
Q

Two-Year-Olds Can Appreciate Intentions

A
  • Evidence of selective helping
  • Contrary to Piaget’s theory, suggests that 2 year olds are able to appreciate intentions when judging others’ action
20
Q

Prosocial Behaviour Beyond Toddlerhood

A
  • Prosocial behaviour increases throughout childhood, particularly emotional helping and helping requiring self-sacrifice
    –> Due to more sophisticated moral reasoning and improved perspective-taking ability
21
Q

Cultural Differences

A
  • No consistent evidence
    –> Age is more important than culture
    –> Some studies show cultural differences but the pattern of results seems to be random
22
Q

Individual Differences in Prosocial Behaviour

A
  • Genetics (nature)
  • Socialization by parents (nurture)
23
Q

Genetics

A
  • Identical twins are more similar in their level of prosocial behaviour than fraternal twins
    –> Suggests genetic basis of prosocial behaviour
  • Possible involvement of individual differences in oxytocin gene
    –> Oxytocin: neuro-hormone involved in social bonding and childbirth
24
Q

Genetics

A
  • Genetic differences manifest as differences in temperament:
    –> Proneness to distress
    *Ability to experience emotion without getting overwhelmed by it is associated with greater empathy
    *Better emotion regulation is positively associated with helping
    –> Shyness
    *High level of shyness negatively associated with helping
25
Q

Socialization by Parents

A
  1. Modelling of prosocial behaviour: Children tend to be similar to parents in terms of prosocial behaviour because they copy their behaviour
  2. Parenting style:
    - Authoritative parenting associated with more prosocial behaviour in kids
    - Sympathy-inducing reasoning that focuses on the effect on other people most likely to lead to internalization of prosocial values
    –> “Let’s donate money, because they need it more than us and it will make them happy.”
    –> vs. “because it’s a good/nice thing to do”
  3. Providing opportunities for child to engage in prosocial behaviour
    - Performing household chores
    - Community service hours in high schools
    - Increases children’s willingness to take on prosocial tasks in the future because feel competent to do it
26
Q

The Rise and Fall of Physical Aggression

A
  • Physical aggression begins around 18 months and increases until about 3 years old
    –> E.g.biting, hitting, pushing, kicking
    –> Toddlers lack perspective-taking skills and resort to aggression to get what they want
    –> Most frequent is conflict with peers and siblings over possessions
    –> Declines after 3 years old due to increases in language skills and emotion regulation
27
Q

The Emergence of Relational Aggression

A
  • Relational aggression emerges at 3 years old and continues into adolescence
    –> A type of nonphysical aggression in which harm is caused by hurting someone’s relationships or social status, such as by threatening to withdraw a friendship, ignoring, or excluding a peer
    –> Increases as a result of improvements in language and social cognition
28
Q

Aggression in Girls vs. Boys

A
  • Similar levels of aggressions just displayed differently
    –> Girls tend to be more relationally aggressive and less physically aggressive than boys
    –> Opposite for boys
  • Finding that boys are more physically aggressive is true across cultures
29
Q

Consistency of Aggressive Behaviour

A
  • Consistency in individual differences in aggression over the lifespan
    –> Those who were more physically aggressive as kids had more criminal convictions at age 30
30
Q

Genetic Origins of Aggression

A
  • Difficult temperament is associated with higher aggression
    –> Combination of impulsivity, difficulties with attention, and proneness to anger in childhood is especially predictive of aggression in adolescence
  • BUT genetic factors are not sufficient to become highly aggressive, just may put a child at risk for developing this behaviour
31
Q

Social Cognition Contributions to Aggression

A
  • Hostile attribution bias: The tendency to assume that other people’s ambiguous actions stem from malicious intent
  • Child is more likely to retaliate aggressively
  • More likely to elicit aggression or rejection from peers
32
Q

Family Origins of Aggression

A
  1. Parents model aggression to their children
    - Parental conflict
    - Spanking
  2. Authoritarian and uninvolved parenting is associated with increased risk for aggression in children
    - Very harsh physical discipline appears to lead to hostile attribution bias in children (harsh discipline -> hostile attribution bias -> aggression in children)
  3. Parental monitoring reduces likelihood that teenagers will be associating with deviant, antisocial peers
33
Q

Abuse and Hostile Attribution Bias

A
  • Physically abused children:
    –> Better able to recognize angry facial expressions
    –> React more quickly to angry facial expressions
34
Q

Comparing Parenting to Genetic Contributions

A
  • The influence between parents and children tends to be bidirectional
    –> Parents and child’s behaviour can also be caused by shared genes
  • BUT harsh parenting seems to play a larger role than genes
    –> For monozygotic twins, the twin who receives harsher parenting tends to develop higher levels of aggression than the other twin
35
Q

Peer Influences on Agression

A
  • Deviancy training: negative peer pressure wherein peers model and reinforce aggression and deviance by making these behaviours seem acceptable
36
Q

Factors Contributing to Childhood Aggression

A
  • Gender
  • Genetics
  • Parental modelling
  • Harsh discipline
  • Lack of monitoring
  • Aggressive peers
  • Hostile attribution bias
  • Age