Week 10-Empathy & Coping with Chronic Pain Flashcards

1
Q

What are Definitions of empathy? (Vignemont & Singer, 2006)

A

Empathy is the ability to share the feelings of another’s emotional or sensory state. To be empathetic:
(i) one is in an affective state
(ii) this is congruent to another person’s affective state- isomorphism
(iii) this state is elicited by the observation or imagination of another person’s affective state - causal relationship (you feel sad because you thought or observed it, NOT spontaneous)
(iv) one knows that the other person is the source of one’s own affective state - awareness

Not to be confused with:
Theory of mind: understanding other person’s mental processes, intentions, thoughts. Not necessarily affective or isomorphic.

Sympathy: feel negative response but no isomorphism between the mental state of observer and other person (i.e., without actually feeling the isomorphic state i.e., other’s emotions)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Why are we empathic? (Decety et al., 2016)

A

Empathy likely has an evolutionary basis because it exists:
– The affective experience of empathy utilises a neuropsychological system evident across species.

– It responds (aversively) to generate discomfort while witnessing others in pain or distress which facilitates the urge to help which helps relieves stress (although empathy can be associated with positive experiences).

– Promotes care-giving for young which benefits survival

– Promotes prosocial and altruistic behaviour – benefit based on increased fitness of helper based in reciprocity

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What is the perception-action model of empathy?

A

Preston and de Waal (2002) formulated an influential theory of empathy (typically based on animal research):
“attended perception of the object’s state automatically activates the subjects’ representations of the state… and that activation of these representations automatically primes or generates the associated autonomic or somatic responses, unless inhibited” (Preston and de Waal, 2002, p.4).

Features of empathy:
“automatically”: empathy occurs spontaneously without conscious effort

“representations”: brain activation patterns during viewing someone else in pain which are similar to pain experience

“somatic responses”: predicts bodily response to observed pain similar to experience of pain e.g., increased heart rate or skin conductance responses (i.e., physical reactions)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

What supporting evidence is there for the P-A-M?

A

-Viewing motor actions activates the motor
mirror neuron system

-Mirror neurons (e.g., Rizzolatti, 1996) are visuomotor neurons first identified in monkeys which discharged during both the execution and observation of a particular action (Researcher ate a banana and the neurons in the monkey went off during observation not just in the execution i.e., being rewarded with a banana). Therefore mirror neurons underpin empathy.

-Vogt et al. (2007): guitarists and non-guitarists observed video clips of guitar chords being played in fMRI scanned. Observation of someone playing the guitar activates both the visual areas but also the motor areas even if they cannot play the guitar. This demonstrates similar neural representations to (attempted) execution of the chords (i.e. you are putting yourself in the place even if you can’t). The same pattern is seen when trying and observing

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What supporting evidence is there for the P-A-M relating to pain?

A

Viewing images with implicit pain activates the pain matrix (Jackson et al., Neuroimage, 2005).
* fMRI research to investigate neural activations associated with empathy for pain showing them videos and sounds related to pain.

  • Compared brain activations associated with pain images to graphically similar non-painful images (active and controlled and take these away from each other to see differences in brain activations to know what is specific to the pain content).
  • Enhanced BOLD activation in anterior
    cingulate/anterior mid-cingulate cortex
    (ACC/aMCC) and anterior insula (AI) cortices
  • Positively correlated with subjective pain ratings (i.e., how painful they thought it was)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

What supporting evidence is there for the P-A-M relating to pain? Meta-analysis to look for consistencies across studies (Lamm et al., 2011)

A

-Meta-analysis of brain activations related to pain empathy

-Anterior mid-cingulate cortex and
bilateral anterior insula consistently
activated in 9 pain empathy studies –
e.g. similar to the Jackson paradigm.

Both ‘pain matrix’ regions.

-Other regions are likely but these are
most consistent from evidence.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What Evidence is there for a biological basis for empathy - Pain empathy in mice (Langford et al., 2006)

A
  • Empathy was classically considered to be unique to higher primates or humans alone.
  • Mice were given identical levels of noxious stimulus in isolation or in pairs and pain behaviours were quantified (if empathy seen in lower order animals, then it suggests empathy is a basic biological thing everything has)
  • They display increased pain behaviour (behaviourally) in the presence of another mouse (even with the same level of noxious stimuli)
  • Presence of social modulation of pain behaviour is evidence for biological basis, or automaticity, of empathy (i.e., mouse A thinks mouse B can help hence shows social signs of distress)
  • Dyads (relationships) comprising cagemates and siblings enhanced the effect, i.e., produced greater increases in pain responses in familiar pairs and then even greater with sibling pairs than pairs comprising strangers – modulation by familiarity/familial bond suggests something beyond mere biology? (if it was biology it would be steady across all levels)

-Was published in a journal indicating these findings are important (hard for animal research to get published)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

SLIDE 11 CHECK SLIDES

A

-Evidence of bottom-up processes for empathy (i.e., biological basis)

-The free rat shared the treats with the trapped rat once freed

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What is empathy like in Lower-order mammals when looking at in-group and out-group bias (Bartal….Mason et al., 2015)

A

-Empathic behaviour in rats – is modulated by social experience. Will rats help others who are different to themselves? E.g., a different strain?

  • In mammals, helping is preferentially provided to members of one’s own group.
  • Rats helped trapped strangers by releasing them from a restraining device, just as they did cagemates.
  • However, rats did not help strangers of a different strain, unless previously housed with the trapped rat.
  • To test if genetic relatedness alone can motivate helping, rats were fostered from birth with another strain and were not exposed to their own strain. As adults, fostered rats helped strangers of the fostering strain but not rats of their own strain.
  • Thus, familiarity is required for the expression of pro-social behaviour.

-It is not the genetic alignment but rather the learned social strain to whether they would help or not.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What is the Top-down modulation of empathy (Singer and Lamm, 2009)?

A

Theoretical contribution (Singer and Lamm, 2009) suggested that brain imaging from empathy research differentiates distinct neural processes:
1. bottom-up processes : perception = representation of pain (automaticity/mirror neurons?)
2. top-down processes: includes appraisal of context, attention, preparations for action etc.

Evidence for top-down modulation of empathy come from studies of the following:
– affection for object of empathy
– social proximity
– attention
– group membership
– expertise

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

What evidence is there suggesting Positive affect (emotions) for the person in pain modulates Empathy (Cheng, 2010)

A
  • Participants where instructed to imaging the image depicting pain was either a loved one or a stranger.
  • Activations in ACC and Insula were enhanced in ‘Love’ condition.
  • Activation in tempero-parietal junction is also modulated in the ‘other’ condition – self other distinction?

-The closeness of the relationship seems to matter (seen in rats too)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What evidence is there suggesting Jealousy (intrasexual competition) modulates
empathic responses (Zheng et al., 2016)?

A

-Participants were introduced via video to a couple who would later be the targets to be exposed to pain.

-Either ‘lucky guy’ much more attractive partner (punching above his weight)

-Or ‘not so lucky’ with matched partner (similar level of attractiveness)

-Participants attributed lower empathy scores for the ‘lucky guy’ condition.

-Something as basic as being jealous (why does he have that partner) impacts and manipulates the level of empathy given with pain.

-Neural responses in ACC and AI were reduced compared to those seen for control condition

-Sample was men

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What evidence is there for In-group bias and pain empathy (Azevedo et al., 2012)?

A
  • Caucasian and black participants viewed
    video clips of a hypodermic needle (pain) or
    earbuds (touch) with black or Caucasian
    hands.
  • Anterior insula showed increased BOLD
    activation during viewing pain in own race
    group compared to other race – increased
    in-group empathy?

-Preferential anterior insula activation during pain empathy for ones own group
correlated with subjective measures of
racial bias (implicit association task) i.e., Those who showed the highest levels of racial bias, showed the highest differences in brain activation between the groups

-Has been replicated in other countries and cultures

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

What evidence is there for Intentionality in empathy for pain (Decety et al., 2009)?

A

-A study in children.

-Pain images were associated
with typical activations, e.g., aMCC or AI.

-Study manipulated context as to whether the pain was caused intentionally by other person

-The temporo-parietal junction (involved in context) was preferentially activated when pain was perceived to be caused intentionally by another person (i.e., they saw it as unfair)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What evidence is there for Focus of attention modulation of empathic response (Lamm et al., 2007)?

A

P’s saw Numbed vs. non-numbed target hand and Sensory vs. unpleasantness focus

  • Activity in the somatosensory cortex was enhanced when participants evaluated the
    sensory aspects of pain.
  • Viewing pain in the numbed hand still activate pain brain regions suggesting some
    degree of automatic activation in accordance with Preston and de Waal (2002) i.e., Even if told that they cannot feel pain because they are numbed, they still felt pain showing perception action to a degree automatic and cannot be blocked
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Beyond ‘up and down’ – What are more complex models of empathy? (de Waal, 2008) (needing a model which combines both bottom-up and top-down)

A

The Russian-Doll model of empathy posits a tiered system with progressive levels of
empathy from basic affective (e.g. emotional contagion) to higher-order processes such as sympathetic concern and
emotional perspective taking.

  1. Core of the doll is P-A-M (i.e., evolutionary aspect) called emotional contagion
  2. White part of doll is sympathetic concern (i.e., context)
  3. The outside of the doll is empathetic perspective-taking (considering what they may be thinking and formulating a response).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Beyond ‘up and down’ – What are more complex models of empathy? (Lamm, 2016) (needing a model which combines both bottom-up and top-down)

A

Lamm (2016) suggested a need for multifaceted neuroscientific theoretical
models of empathy which necessitate shared representations between self
and other, plus the ability to distinguish between the two, as distinct and essential
building blocks for empathic experience

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Beyond ‘up and down’ – What are more complex models of empathy? (de Waal, 2017) (needing a model which combines both bottom-up and top-down)

A

-Somewhat similar to Lamm

-The most recent iteration of the PAM describes a dynamic and graded system with flexibility for learning and experience
which is likely to recruit different brain regions for distinct components of
empathic processing (de Waal & Preston, 2017).

-Laam and de Waal both were theoretical without the data

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Beyond ‘up and down’: What was found in the Meta-Analysis of fMRI: Empathy + Pain overlap (Fallon et al., 2020)

A
  • We performed meta- analysis of fMRI
    studies of empathy and compared the
    activation profile against meta-analysis of
    pain experience.
  • The conjunction (overlapping) analysis of
    ALE maps representing empathy for pain
    and direct pain experience pooled 219
    studies with a total of 3717 participants and
    3307 reported foci.
  • The results highlighted an overlap of
    activation likelihood between empathy and pain coordinates in seven clusters encompassing
  • bilateral AI and ACC/aMCC (expected)
  • bilateral IFG which bordered (and in
    the case of the right hemisphere
    extended to) middle frontal gyrus, and
    bilateral supramarginal regions
  • The location of significant clusters from
    conjunction analysis of ALE maps for empathy for pain and directly perceived pain.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

What did Fallon (2020) find when contrasting empathy vs pain?

A
  • Contrast analyses: empathy > pain (looked at separate areas when witnessing pain and experiencing pain)
  • Contrast analysis pointed to significantly greater likelihood of activation during empathy for pain, relative to directly perceived pain, in 6 clusters encompassing bilateral supramarginal, IFG and occipitotemporal regions (specific to empathy but not pain)
  • Contrast analyses: pain> empathy
  • The reverse contrast revealed six clusters
    indicative of increased activation likelihood
    estimates for directly perceived pain relative to empathy for observed pain. These encompassed two large bilateral clusters over parietal opercular cortices (S2), posterior insula and S1. Right putamen and right frontal cluster encompassing right prefrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal and two clusters in aMCC (specific to experiencing pain but not witnessing pain)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

What are the implications of Fallon’s (2020) study?

A
  • Meta-analysis demonstrates a rich functional brain network of empathy
    for pain, extending beyond AI and aMCC, and including activations areas that demonstrate activation to empathy but not pain.
  • This is important for theoretical understanding of the phenomenon which
    is likely to complex and multi-faceted.

From a theoretical perspective, the patterns of ALE seen in conjunction and contrast analyses show alignment with a tiered theoretical understanding of empathic processing such as:
* Russian-Doll model (de Waal, 2008; de Waal and Preston, 2017)

  • Independent components of shared representation and self-other distinction (Lamm et al., 2016).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Implications of empathy for pain: What evidence is there for the empathy response for pain in ASD?

A
  • Fan, 2014: ASD participants exhibited
    significantly reduced activation in aMCC and
    anterior insula but sensory activation profile
    intact.
  • Fan infers that ASD exhibit heightened
    empathic arousal but impaired social
    understanding when perceiving others’
    distress (lacking in social understanding but understands the sensory aspects)
    – This is still a matter for research and debate!
  • Alternatively Gu and colleagues (2015)
    reported enhanced neural activation in AI for ASD relative to controls (although behavioral empathetic pain discriminability was reduced). Skin conductance responses were also elevated but the ratings of pain was reduced.

-ASD is a wide spectrum so this still needs to be investigated as it is likely the sample always varies.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Implications of empathy for pain - Clinical disorders and pain empathy: What evidence is there for an impaired empathy response for pain in Psychopathy (Deming et al., 2020)?

A
  • Adult male incarcerated offenders (N = 94) viewed images of two people interacting, with one individual’s face obscured by a shape.
  • Participants attempted to identify the target emotions
  • In line with predictions, psychopathy was negatively related to task accuracy during affective perspective-taking for fear, happiness, and sadness.
  • Psychopathy was related to reduced brain activity exclusively during fear perspective condition in left anterior insula posterior orbitofrontal cortex, right precuneus, left superior parietal lobule, and left superior occipital cortex.
  • These findings highlight brain regions that are hypoactive in psychopathy when explicitly processing another’s fear.
24
Q

Implications of empathy for pain: What evidence is there for Altered neural responses for observed pain in fibromyalgia syndrome (Fallon, et al., 2015)?

A

-FMS patients demonstrate augmented electrophysiological brain responses to both types of image (but particularly painful
scenes):
* Increased empathy
or
* Hypervigilance to pain? (more sensitive to cue environments due to chronic pain experience)
* Or both?

25
Q

What are Real world implications of empathy: What is the link between clinicians and empathy for pain?

A
  • Hodgkin et al. (Pain, 23, 272-277, 1985) Pain reports in 21 patients undergoing painful fluid aspiration procedure. Estimates were collected from their doctors as well.
  • No significant difference in pain ratings. Is this a good study? Confounds and Bias?
  • Forrest et al. (Anaesthesiol. Scand., 1989) asked 52 patients with abdominal pain and their 8 doctors to rate patient pain using visual analogue scales.
  • The median of pain ratings was significantly smaller in doctors (3.1) than in patients (6.1).
  • There was a positive correlations between the doctors’ and patients’ ratings ( r = 0.64, P < 0.05) (the higher the pain in a patient, the higher the pain rated by the doctor)
26
Q

How does expertise modulate the empathetic brain response?

A
  • Cheng et al. (Curr. Biol., 2007) analysed fMRI in control subjects and physicians observing painful needle insertion
  • Physicians gave reduced subjective pain ratings and exhibited reduced neural empathic response than control subjects.
  • Since shown that this reduction develops across ‘time served’ in job – protection against burnout? (the more time working i healthcare, the lower the empathy modulation and pain ratings)
  • Cheng (Front Behav Neurosci., 2017) demonstrated modulation of pain ratings and brain activations in medical practitioners perceiving’ pain in a hospital vs home context (saw a photo of a ward or at home). The deficit to brain responses to pain was specific to the work environment (so doctors aren’t incapable of empathy but rather downregulates at work due to constant exposure to avoid burnout)
  • Situational contexts supports burnout hypothesis – interesting additional finding: perceiving reward from patient care protects them from burnout.
27
Q

What evidence is there for Empathic behaviour in chronic pain couples
correlating with marital satisfaction?

A
  • Cano et al. (J. Pain, 2008) analysed 91 couples in which one of the partners was a chronic pain patient
  • Interactions between the partners during an interview about impacts of pain on their lives was recorded and analysed by experienced observers.
  • Empathic behaviour of the spouse correlated with greater marital satisfaction – for BOTH (i.e., greater empathy correlates with greater relationship strength)
28
Q

What is the importance of coping with chronic pain? (Fayaz et al., 2016)

A

-40% of adults in the UK had some form of chronic pain which equates to 8,000,000 adults requiring care

29
Q

What did Lazarus and Folkman (1962) find on stress and coping?

A

-Pain (chronic or acute) can be considered as a classical ‘stressor’

If you stress an individual they will go through a certain process (they created a transactional model):
We will appraise the stress come up with a style of coping that works and then re-appraise based off it it works

Stressor = primary appraisal (does it matter, how stressful and if yes is it a cost or threat) = second appraisal (what can I do about this) = emotion-focused or problem-focused coping

30
Q

What are the Two basic types of coping? (Lazarus & Folkman, 1962)

A
  1. Emotion-focused coping
  2. Problem-focused coping
31
Q

What is Emotion-focused coping?

A
  • used when we feel like we have little control over situation.
  • Aims to alleviate emotional distress

– Avoiding – ‘I’m not going to go there because….. ’

– Distancing yourself from the emotion

– Acceptance – ‘it’s a problem, but I have a lot of good things too’ (seen as negative back then)

– Seeking emotional support from your partner

32
Q

What is Problem-focused coping?

A
  • Used when we feel some control of situation to manage the source of stress.

– Define the problem,
– Generate alternative solutions
– Learning new skills to manage stressor
– Reappraising

33
Q

What was Lazarus & Folkman (1962) right about regarding their model?

A

Reappraisal: new information from environment can ‘feedback’ to effect primary and secondary appraisals
* Can increase or decreases the stress pressure

Coping is a fluid, dynamic process involving the ongoing interaction between appraisals and reappraisals

34
Q

How did Rosentiel & Keefe (1983) create an assessment of pain coping?

A

Coping Strategies Questionnaire (CSQ, Rosentiel and Keefe, 1983)
Self-report instrument 50 items using 7-point Likert scales

Subscales: 6 coping dimensions and 2 behavioural dimensions

Coping strategies:
* Diverting attention
* Reinterpreting pain sensations (e.g., seeing it as warm)
* Coping self-statements
* Ignoring pain sensations
* Praying or hoping
* Catastrophising

Behavioural responses:
* Increase or decrease activity (FAM)
* Increased pain behaviour

35
Q

What are some other assessments of pain coping?

A

The Chronic Pain Coping Inventory (Jensen and Turner, Pain, 1995) 65-item questionnaire measuring 11 coping dimensions; e.g., resting and asking for assistance

+ it has a version for patients and for significant others

  • some of items refer to physical interventions (e.g., ‘opioid use’) rather
    than to psychological coping strategies

Vanderbilt Pain Management Inventory (Brown and Nicassio, 1987) Evaluates active and passive coping using 18 scales grouped into:
* Active coping,
– e.g., taking medicine/ busying yourself

  • Passive coping (Social support seeking, Catastrophising), e.g.,
    – Telling others that it hurts /believing that you can’t do anything to relieve pain.
    -This assessment doesn’t quite have the depth that the CSQ has
36
Q

What is found in a typical correlation study of pain coping?

A

Robinson et al. (1997) performed a correlation study in 965 chronic pain
patients by taking CSQ scales and various pain and pain outcome measures
(MPI=multidimensional pain inventory, depression, MPQ)

-One of the coping strategies is catastrophise which correlates negatively with measures such as activities and positively with pain severity

37
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: I. Attentional distraction? (do not mention all of the evidence in the exam as there are 6: be selective!)

A

Example: “If possible, I would try and read a book or magazine to take my
mind off the pain” (Boothby, 1999)

Mixed findings for effects of attentional refocusing on chronic pain :
* van Lankveld et al. (1994): use of distraction correlated with well-being

  • Roelofs et al. (2006): used electronic diary to evaluate the effects of attentional distraction on pain – no effects seen
  • Affleck et al. (1992) found negative correlation between pain levels and attentional distraction in patients with comparatively low pain; BUT those with strong pain showed a positive correlation.
  • Robinson et al. (1997): distraction predicted greater pain and interference

Evidence for success of distraction for chronic pain IS POOR, this contrasts with acute pain, which shows a strong effect.

38
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: II. Hope and prayer?

A

Prayer and hope can affect pain (link to resilience and acceptance later):
1. as a distraction
2. inducing positive emotion/outlook
3. by strengthening social contacts and support
4. by inducing a relaxed, meditative state

“Although most patients with chronic diseases do pray for relief from their
physical and mental suffering, the intention of their prayers is not only for healing. Rather, prayer can be a resource that allows patients to positively transform the experience of their illness” (Review of prayer for chronic pain coping: Jors et al., 2015)

39
Q

What is the conflicting data on the effectiveness on hope and prayer as a coping strategy?

A

Negative effects of H&P:
Hill et al. (1995): more disability and pain severity
Robinson et al. (1997): greater life interference

Positive effects of H&P:
Anderson et al. (2008): religion predicts greater satisfaction with life in spinal cord injury (so works in circumstances but not perhaps through all aspects of chronic pain)

Complex effects of H&P:
Wachholz et al. (2007): Impact of negative spirituality (fear of God, anger toward God, feels abandoned by God).
Ribbentrop at al. (2005): positive spirituality in pain patients was related to improved mental health negative spirituality was related to poor health outcomes and greater mortality

Mixed support for H&P as a coping strategy, some evidence that there can actually be negative consequences.

40
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: III. Reinterpreting pain?

A

Example: “I do not think of it as pain but rather as a dull or warm feeling” (Boothby, 1999)

Positive effects of reinterpretation:
ter Kuile et al.(1995 : patients after a hypnosis therapy program showed less pain if they used reinterpretation

No effects:
Dozois et al. (1995): no correlation with disability
Hill et al. et al.(1995): or with pain severity
Robinson et al. (1997): no correlations with pain outcomes either

Unless part of a fuller therapeutic program, reinterpreting pain does not seem to be an effective coping strategy.

41
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: IV. Positive self-statements?

A

Example: “I see it is a challenge and don’t let bother me” (Boothby, 1999)

Studies showing positive effects of positive self-statements:
Hill (1993): negative correlation with pain severity
van Lankveld et al. (1994): negative correlation with depression

Lack of correlations:
Dozois et al. (1996): Robinson et al. (1997): no correlation with different pain outcomes.

Mixed findings. Some limited evidence for positive self-statements on pain and pain outcomes, but a number of studies have not found any associations.

42
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: V. Ignoring Pain?

A

Example: “I tell myself it does not hurt” (Boothby, 1999)

No correlations between pain outcome measures and ignoring pain:
Dozois et al. (1996) physical disability
Hill (1993) psychosocial dysfunction
Hill et al. (1995) pain severity

Little evidence for effects of ignoring pain on pain severity and pain-related outcomes

43
Q

What are certain Coping strategies: VI. Catastrophising?

A

Pain catastrophising relates to greater pain, depression, anxiety, and life interference (See previous lecture in week 5)

Catastrophising (helplessness, anxiety) is a passive pain coping mechanisms which correlates with pain, depression, and disability in chronic pain patients (Samwel et al., 2006).

Catastrophising as a ‘coping’ mechanism is likely to contribute to more negative outcomes.

44
Q

What are methodological problems in pain coping research?

A

Most of the studies are cross-sectional, typically correlating one coping questionnaire with a set of outcome measures (pain severity, depression, disability).

This leads to a problem with circularity: was a positive correlation e.g. between hoping and praying and clinical pain severity, due to pain occurring after the coping, or was it due to particularly strong pain urging a person to pray?

Lack of longitudinal studies to compare samples of coping mechanisms and pain outcomes over years (more longitudinal research in this area is needed but drop out rates tend to be high and it is hard to get the funding for the entire research duration)

A compromise: day-by-day variations in the level of coping, pain, depression and other measures (Affleck et al., 1996; Keefe et
al., 1987)

45
Q

Define Pain acceptance

A

Acceptance of chronic pain entails that an individual reduce selfs unsuccessful attempts to avoid or control pain and focus instead on participation in valued activities and the pursuit of personally relevant goals (McCracken et al., Pain., 2004)

Acceptance:
1) willingness to experience pain,
2) to continue healthy life activities anyway!

Note: Acceptance is intrinsic in Mindfulness Meditation training (Week 7) and some forms of religious styles of coping. This could relate to positive effects for both!

46
Q

What is the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (McCracken et al., 2004)?

A

-20-item questionnaire to measure levels of acceptance and initiative to go and be active

Two factors of pain acceptance (Vowles et al., Pain, 2008) (these overlap with the theoretical basis of pain acceptance meaning we are onto something)
Factor 1: Pain willingness
Factor 2: Activity engagement

47
Q

What evidence is there to suggest Acceptance of pain predicts pain-related outcomes independently of coping measures (McCracken and Eccleston, Pain, 2003)?

A

-Hierarchical multivariate regression
analysis was performed in 230 chronic
pain patients using CPAQ, CSQ and
other measures.

-Acceptance predicted lower pain,
disability, depression and pain-related
anxiety.

-Interestingly, none of the coping
strategies showed association with pain when acceptance was included as a factor except for praying and hoping which
showed a positive relationship with pain
(i.e., >H&P = >pain) and low adjustment (however this may be because hope and prayer tends to be with the most desperate people who tend to have the worse clinical outcomes)

48
Q

What was found in the Meta-analysis of effects of acceptance on chronic pain
(Veehof et al., 2011)?

A

-Meta-analysis of 19 studies aiming at effects of acceptance in chronic pain patients

Moderate positive effects for acceptance were observed in:
Pain : size effect = 0.47, P < 0.01
Depression: size effect = 0.69, P < 0.01
Anxiety: size effect = 0.69, P < 0.01
Physical wellbeing: size effect = 0.48, P < 0.01
Quality of life: size effect = 0.63, P < 0.01

These effects were comparable to effect sizes for cognitive behavioural therapy which may also be useful for improving acceptance…

49
Q

What evidence is there to suggest Cognitive behavioural therapy increases pain acceptance and decreases pain catastrophising (Vowel et al., 2007)?

A

Outcome measures (e.g. depression, disability) were equally predicted by acceptance (+) and pain catastrophising (-)
- two halves of the same coin?

50
Q

What did Fallon et al. (2020) find when investigating coping with pain in a pandemic?

A

-Administered online questionnaires regarding things such as social factors, catastrophising and the level of pain and if it was worse during lockdown

-Chronic Pain patients reported that their pain intensity was worse during lockdown.

-They were more adversely affected by
lockdown conditions than people without
chronic pain.

> Loneliness, anxiety, depressed mood, reduced exercise in CP group compared to HC.

-Their levels of pain catastrophizing were the best predictor of changes in perceived pain during lockdown (i.e., they were worse off). Reductions in exercise were also important. Both relate to ACCEPTANCE.

-Catastrophizing plays an important role in
coping with chronic pain (Vowell., 2007 study which demonstrated efficacy of CBT for >acceptance and <PCS)

51
Q

Define Resilience: a novel concept in evaluation of coping with chronic pain

A

Resilience = continuing functioning and facilitated recovery in the presence of pain (Sturgeon and Zautra, 2010)

Resilience manifests in 3 outcomes:
* Facilitated recovery – resilient individuals regain equilibrium faster (i.e., recovers better)
* Sustainability – perseverance in desired actions, goal pursuits, and social engagement (can complete normal life)
* Growth – realisation and understanding own capacities, ability to follow life goals under adverse conditions

52
Q

What are Mechanisms of Resilience (Sturgeon and Zautra, 2010)?

A
  • Optimism – positive expectations for the future
  • Purpose in life – striving for goals
  • Pain acceptance – see previous
  • Emphasis on positive emotions
  • Preference of active coping - function in spite of presence of pain, engage in physical activity
  • Resilience through social support –utilise support network
53
Q

What is the impact of Vulnerability on Resilience?

A
  • Vulnerability contributes to worsened physical and psychological functioning in response to stress.
  • Separate, but related, to resilience.
  • Can be related to diminished coping resources

Can be influenced by various factors:
- fitness and (mental and physical) health
- social support
- material resources

Different levels of resilience/vulnerability could be an important confounding factor in the varied findings for coping strategy studies

54
Q

What is Resilience as a moderator of coping resources? (Sturgeon and Zautra, 2010) FINISH SLIDE

A

Pain episode:
-There are pre-resources to include such as resilience resources (trait positive affect, strong social ties) and vulnerability traits (recurrent depression, childhood trauma)
-These intertwine with mechanisms resilience (state positive affect positive social interactions) + vulnerability (

-Resilience modifies the relationship between pain and pain outcomes through coping mechanisms and acceptance
i.e., resilience links all of the concepts we have discussed.

55
Q

What Evidence is there for implications for Resilience: Optimism and Purpose of Life?

A

Habituation to repeated heat pain stimuli (acute pain in healthy people: Smith et al., 2009)

Optimism is related to lower level of pain and depression in early stages of rheumatoid arthritis in elderly (Ferreira et al., 2007)

Purpose in Life – faster recovery after a knee surgery (Smith and Zautra, 2004)

Resilience Factors (particularly optimism) are protective against development of chronic pain in children (Cousins, 2015)