Unit 4: Human Development Across the Lifespan Flashcards

1
Q

Mary Ainsworth

A

Research by Ainsworth and her colleagues suggests that attachment emerges out of a complex interplay between infant and mother.Ainsworth used a method called the strange situation procedure, in which infants are exposed to a series of eight separation and reunion episodes to assess the quality of their attachment. The three-minute episodes in this carefully orchestrated laboratory procedure involve events such as a stranger entering a room where an infant is playing with a parent nearby, followed by the parent leaving, returning, leaving, and returning again. The child’s reactions (distress, comfort) to the parent’s departures and returns are carefully monitored to gauge attachment quality.
Infant–mother attachments vary in quality. Ainsworth and her colleagues found that these attachments follow three patterns. Fortunately, most infants develop a secure attachment. They play and explore comfortably with their mother present, become visibly upset when she leaves, and are quickly calmed by her return. However, some children display a pattern called anxious-ambivalent attachment. They appear anxious even when their mother is near and protest excessively when she leaves, but they are not particularly comforted when she returns. Children in the third category seek little contact with their mother and often are not distressed when she leaves, a condition labelled avoidant attachment. Years later, other researchers added a fourth category called disorganized-disoriented attachment. These children appear confused about whether they should approach or avoid their mother and are especially insecure.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Jay Belsky

A

Jay Belsky’s process of parenting model, published in 1984, continues to be widely referenced within the scientific literature. The main premise of this model is that parenting is multiply determined and is influenced by characteristics of the parent, child, and social context.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

John Bowlby

A

There is an alternative explanation of attachment proposed by John Bowlby. Bowlby was impressed by the importance of contact comfort to Harlow’s monkeys and by the apparently unlearned nature of this preference. Influenced by evolutionary theories, Bowlby argued that there must be a biological basis for attachment. According to his view, infants are biologically programmed to emit behavior (smiling, cooing, clinging, and so on) that triggers an affectionate, protective response from adults.
Bowlby also asserted that adults are programmed by evolutionary forces to be captivated by this behavior and to respond with warmth, love, and protection. Obviously, these characteristics would be adaptive in terms of promoting children’s survival. Attachment theory has had an evolutionary slant from its very beginning, long before evolutionary theory became influential in psychology. While John Bowlby analyzed attachment in terms of its survival value for infants, contemporary evolutionary theorists emphasize how attachment contributes to parents’ and children’s reproductive fitness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Erik Erikson

A

Like Freud, Erikson concluded that events in early childhood leave a permanent stamp on adult personality. However, unlike Freud, Erikson theorized that personality continues to evolve over the entire life span.
Building on Freud’s earlier work, Erikson devised a stage theory of personality development. As you’ll see in reading this chapter, many theories describe development in terms of stages. A stage is a developmental period during which characteristic patterns of behaviour are exhibited and certain capacities become established. Stage theories assume that (1) individuals must progress through specified stages in a particular order because each stage builds on the previous stage, (2) progress through these stages is strongly related to age, and (3) development is marked by major discontinuities that usher in dramatic transitions in behavior.
Erikson partitioned the life span into eight stages, each of which brings a psychosocial crisis involving transitions in important social relationships. According to Erikson, personality is shaped by how individuals deal with these psychosocial crises. Each crisis involves a struggle between two opposing tendencies, such as trust versus mistrust or initiative versus guilt, both of which are experienced by the person. Erikson described the stages in terms of these antagonistic tendencies, which represent personality traits that people display in varying degrees over the remainder of their lives. Although the names for Erikson’s stages suggest either–or outcomes, he viewed each stage as a tug of war that determined the subsequent balance between opposing polarities in personality. All eight stages in Erikson’s theory are charted in Figure 11.7. We describe the first four childhood stages here and discuss the remaining stages in the upcoming sections on adolescence and adulthood.
Trust versus Mistrust.- Erikson’s first stage encompasses the first year of life, when an infant has to depend completely on adults to take care of its basic needs for necessities such as food, a warm blanket, and changed diapers. If an infant’s basic biological needs are adequately met by his or her caregivers and sound attachments are formed, the child should develop an optimistic, trusting attitude toward the world. However, if the infant’s basic needs are taken care of poorly, a more distrusting, pessimistic personality may result.
Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt.- Erikson’s second stage unfolds during the second and third years of life, when parents begin toilet training and other efforts to regulate the child’s behavior. The child must begin to take some personal responsibility for feeding, dressing, and bathing. If all goes well, he or she acquires a sense of self-sufficiency. But, if parents are never satisfied with the child’s efforts and there are constant parent–child conflicts, the child may develop a sense of personal shame and self-doubt.
Initiative versus Guilt.- In Erikson’s third stage, lasting roughly from ages three to six, children experiment and take initiatives that may sometimes conflict with their parents’ rules. Overcontrolling parents may begin to instill feelings of guilt, and self-esteem may suffer. Parents need to support their children’s emerging independence while maintaining appropriate controls. In the ideal situation, children will retain their sense of initiative while learning to respect the rights and privileges of other family members.
Industry versus Inferiority.- In the fourth stage (age six through puberty), the challenge of learning to function socially is extended beyond the family to the broader social realm of the neighborhood and school. Children who are able to function effectively in this less nurturant social sphere where productivity is highly valued should learn to value achievement and to take pride in accomplishment, resulting in a sense of competence.
The strength of Erikson’s theory is that it accounts for both continuity and transition in personality development. It accounts for transition by showing how new challenges in social relationships stimulate personality development throughout life. It accounts for continuity by drawing connections between early childhood experiences and aspects of adult personality. One measure of a theory’s value is how much research it generates, and Erikson’s theory continues to guide a fair amount of research (Thomas, 2005).
On the negative side of the ledger, Erikson’s theory has depended heavily on illustrative case studies, which are open to varied interpretations. Another weakness is that the theory provides an “idealized” description of “typical” developmental patterns. Thus, it’s not well suited for explaining the enormous personality differences that exist among people. Inadequate explanation of individual differences is a common problem with stage theories of development. This shortcoming surfaces again in the next section, where we’ll examine Jean Piaget’s stage theory of cognitive development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Harry Harlow

A

Harlow removed monkeys from their mothers at birth and raised them in the laboratory with two types of artificial “substitute mothers.” One type of artificial mother was made of terry cloth and could provide contact comfort. The other type of artificial mother was made of wire. Half of the monkeys were fed from a bottle attached to a wire mother and the other half were fed by a cloth mother. The young monkeys’ attachment to their substitute mothers was tested by introducing a frightening stimulus, such as a strange toy. If reinforcement through feeding were the key to attachment, the frightened monkeys should have scampered off to the mother that had fed them. This was not the case. The young monkeys scrambled for their cloth mothers, even if they were not fed by them. Harlow’s work made a simple reinforcement explanation of attachment unrealistic for animals, let alone for more complex human beings.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Lawrence Kohlberg

A

By presenting similar dilemmas to subjects and studying their responses, Lawrence Kohlberg devised a model of how moral reasoning develops. What is morality? That’s a complicated question that philosophers have debated for centuries. For our purposes, it will suffice to say that morality involves the ability to discern right from wrong and to behave accordingly.
Kohlberg’s model is the most influential of a number of competing theories that attempt to explain how youngsters develop a sense of right and wrong. His work was derived from much earlier work by Jean Piaget (1932), who theorized that moral development is determined by cognitive development. By this he meant that the way individuals think out moral issues depends on their level of cognitive development. This assumption provided the springboard for Kohlberg’s research.
Kohlberg’s theory focuses on moral reasoning rather than overt behaviour. This point is best illustrated by describing Kohlberg’s method of investigation. He presented his subjects with thorny moral questions such as Heinz’s dilemma. He then asked them what the actor in the dilemma should do and, more important, why. It was the why that interested Kohlberg. He examined the nature and progression of subjects’ moral reasoning.
The result of this work is the stage theory of moral reasoning outlined in Figure 11.12. Kohlberg found that individuals progress through a series of three levels of moral development, each of which can be broken into two sublevels, yielding a total of six stages. Each stage represents a different approach to thinking about right and wrong.
Younger children at the preconventional level think in terms of external authority. Acts are wrong because they are punished or right because they lead to positive consequences. Older children who have reached the conventional level of moral reasoning see rules as necessary for maintaining social order. They therefore accept these rules as their own. They “internalize” these rules not to avoid punishment but to be virtuous and win approval from others. Moral thinking at this stage is relatively inflexible. Rules are viewed as absolute guidelines that should be enforced rigidly.
During adolescence, some youngsters move on to the postconventional level, which involves working out a personal code of ethics. Acceptance of rules is less rigid, and moral thinking shows some flexibility. Subjects at the postconventional level allow for the possibility that someone might not comply with some of society’s rules if they conflict with personal ethics. For example, subjects at this level might applaud a newspaper reporter who goes to jail rather than reveal a source of information who was promised anonymity.
How has Kohlberg’s theory fared in research? The central ideas have received reasonable support. Studies have shown that youngsters generally do move through Kohlberg’s stages of moral reasoning in the order he proposed (Walker, 1989). Furthermore, relations between age and level of moral reasoning are in the predicted directions (Rest, 1986; see Figure 11.13). Although these findings support Kohlberg’s model, critics note that it is not unusual to find that a person shows signs of several adjacent levels of moral reasoning at a particular point in development (Krebs & Denton, 2005). As we noted in the critique of Piaget, this mixing of stages is a problem for virtually all stage theories. Evidence is also mounting that Kohlberg’s theory reflects an individualistic ideology characteristic of modern Western nations that is much more culture specific than Kohlberg appreciated (Miller, 2006). Finally, a consensus is building that Kohlberg’s theory has led to a constricted focus on reasoning about interpersonal conflicts, while ignoring many other important aspects of moral development (Walker, 2007). Contemporary theorists note that moral behavior depends on many factors besides reasoning, including emotional reactions, variations in temperament, and cultural background.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

James Marcia

A

Building on Erikson’s insights, James Marcia proposed that the presence or absence of a sense of commitment (to life goals and values) and a sense of crisis (active questioning and exploration) can combine to produce four different identity statuses. In order of increasing maturity, Marcia’s four identity statuses begin with identity diffusion, a state of rudderless apathy, with no commitment to an ideology. Identity foreclosure is a premature commitment to visions, values, and roles—typically those prescribed by one’s parents. Foreclosure is associated with conformity and not being very open to new experiences (Kroger, 2003). An identity moratorium involves delaying commitment for a while to experiment with alternative ideologies and careers. Identity achievement involves arriving at a sense of self and direction after some consideration of alternative possibilities.
Identity achievement is associated with higher self-esteem, conscientiousness, security, achievement motivation, and capacity for intimacy. However, research suggests that people tend to reach identity achievement at later ages than originally envisioned by Marcia. In one large-scale study, by late adolescence only 22–26 percent of the sample had reached identity achievement. Thus, the struggle for a sense of identity routinely extends into young adulthood. Indeed, some people continue to struggle with identity issues well into middle and even late adulthood.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Jean Piaget

A

The investigation of cognitive development was dominated in most of the second half of the 20th century by the theory of Jean Piaget (Kessen, 1996) and there is no doubt that questions related to cognitive development continue to predominate in developmental psychology (e.g., Klahr & Chen, 2011). Much of our discussion of cognitive development is devoted to Piaget’s theory and the research it generated, although we’ll also delve into other approaches to cognitive development.
Jean Piaget was an interdisciplinary scholar whose own cognitive development was exceptionally rapid. In his early 20s, after he had earned a doctorate in natural science and published a novel, Piaget’s interest turned to psychology. He met Theodore Simon, who had collaborated with Alfred Binet in devising the first useful intelligence tests. Working in Simon’s Paris laboratory, Piaget administered intelligence tests to many children to develop better test norms. In doing this testing, Piaget became intrigued by the reasoning underlying the children’s wrong answers. He decided that measuring children’s intelligence was less interesting than studying how children use their intelligence. In 1921, he moved to Geneva, where he spent the rest of his life studying cognitive development. Many of his ideas were based on insights gleaned from careful observations of his own three children during their infancy.
Like Erikson’s theory, Piaget’s model is a stage theory of development. Piaget proposed that youngsters progress through four major stages of cognitive development, which are characterized by fundamentally different thought processes: (1) the sensorimotor period (birth to age two), (2) the preoperational period (ages two to seven), (3) the concrete operational period (ages 7 to 11), and (4) the formal operational period (age 11 onward). Figure 11.8 provides an overview of each of these periods. Piaget regarded his age norms as approximations and acknowledged that transitional ages may vary, but he was convinced that all children progress through the stages of cognitive development in the same order.
Noting that children actively explore the world around them, Piaget asserted that interaction with the environment and maturation gradually alter the way children think. According to Piaget, children progress in their thinking through the complementary processes of assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation involves interpreting new experiences in terms of existing mental structures without changing them. In contrast, accommodation involves changing existing mental structures to explain new experiences. Accommodation and assimilation often occur interactively. For instance, a child who has learned to call four-legged pets “puppies” may apply this scheme the first time she encounters a cat (assimilation), but she will eventually discover that puppies and cats are different types of animals and make adjustments to her mental schemes (accommodation). With the companion processes of assimilation and accommodation in mind, let’s turn now to the four stages in Piaget’s theory.
Sensorimotor Period - One of Piaget’s foremost contributions was to greatly enhance our understanding of mental development in the earliest months of life. The first stage in his theory is the sensorimotor period, which lasts from birth to about age two. Piaget called this stage sensorimotor because infants are developing the ability to coordinate their sensory input with their motor actions.
The major development during the sensorimotor stage is the gradual appearance of symbolic thought. At the beginning of this stage, a child’s behaviour is dominated by innate reflexes. But by the end of the stage, the child can use mental symbols to represent objects (e.g., a mental image of a favourite toy). The key to this transition is the acquisition of the concept of object permanence.
Object permanence develops when a child recognizes that objects continue to exist even when they are no longer visible. Although you surely take the permanence of objects for granted, infants aren’t aware of this permanence at first. If you show a four-month-old an eye-catching toy and then cover the toy with a pillow, the child will not attempt to search for the toy. Piaget inferred from this observation that the child does not understand that the toy continues to exist under the pillow. The notion of object permanence does not dawn on children overnight. The first signs of this insight usually appear between four and eight months of age, when children will often pursue an object that is partially covered in their presence. Progress is gradual, and Piaget believed that children typically don’t master the concept of object permanence until they’re about 18 months old.
Preoperational Period
During the preoperational period, which extends roughly from age two to age seven, children gradually improve in their use of mental images. Although progress in symbolic thought continues, Piaget emphasized the shortcomings in preoperational thought.
Consider a simple problem that Piaget presented to youngsters. He would take two identical beakers and fill each with the same amount of water. After a child had agreed that the two beakers contained the same amount of water, he would pour the water from one of the beakers into a much taller and thinner beaker (see Figure 11.9). He would then ask the child whether the two differently shaped beakers still contained the same amount of water. Confronted with a problem like this, children in the preoperational period generally said no. They typically focused on the higher water line in the taller beaker and insisted that there was more water in the slender beaker. They had not yet mastered the principle of conservation. Conservation is Piaget’s term for the awareness that physical quantities remain constant in spite of changes in their shape or appearance. Why are preoperational children unable to solve conservation problems? According to Piaget, their inability to understand conservation is due to some basic flaws in preoperational thinking. These flaws include centration, irreversibility, and egocentrism.
Centration
is the tendency to focus on just one feature of a problem, neglecting other important aspects. When working on the conservation problem with water, preoperational children tend to concentrate on the height of the water while ignoring the width. They have difficulty focusing on several aspects of a problem at once.
Irreversibility
is the inability to envision reversing an action. Preoperational children can’t mentally “undo” something. For instance, in grappling with the conservation of water, they don’t think about what would happen if the water was poured back from the tall beaker into the original beaker.
Egocentrism in thinking is characterized by a limited ability to share another person’s viewpoint. Indeed, Piaget felt that preoperational children fail to appreciate that there are points of view other than their own. For instance, if you ask a preoperational girl whether her sister has a sister, she’ll probably say no if they are the only two girls in the family. She’s unable to view sisterhood from her sister’s perspective (this also shows irreversibility).
A notable feature of egocentrism is animism—the belief that all things are living, just like oneself. Thus, youngsters attribute lifelike, human qualities to inanimate objects, asking questions such as “When does the ocean stop to rest?” or “Why does the wind get so mad?”
As you can see, Piaget emphasized the weaknesses apparent in preoperational thought. Indeed, that is why he called this stage preoperational. The ability to perform operations—internal transformations, manipulations, and reorganizations of mental structures—emerges in the next stage.
Concrete Operational Period
The development of mental operations marks the beginning of the concrete operational period, which usually lasts from about age 7 to age 11. Piaget called this stage concrete operations because children can perform operations only on images of tangible objects and actual events.
Among the operations that children master during this stage are reversibility and decentration. Reversibility permits a child to mentally undo an action. Decentration allows the child to focus on more than one feature of a problem simultaneously. The newfound ability to coordinate several aspects of a problem helps the child appreciate that there are several ways to look at things. This ability in turn leads to a decline in egocentrism and gradual mastery of conservation as it applies to liquid, mass, number, volume, area, and length (see Figure 11.10).
As children master concrete operations, they develop a variety of new problem-solving capacities. Let’s examine another problem studied by Piaget. Give a preoperational child seven carnations and three daisies. Tell the child the names for the two types of flowers and ask the child to sort them into carnations and daisies. That should be no problem. Now ask the child whether there are more carnations or more daisies. Most children will correctly respond that there are more carnations. Now ask the child whether there are more carnations or more flowers. At this point, most preoperational children will stumble and respond incorrectly that there are more carnations than flowers. Generally, preoperational children can’t handle hierarchical classification problems that require them to focus simultaneously on two levels of classification. However, the child who has advanced to the concrete operational stage is not as limited by centration and can work successfully with hierarchical classification problems.
Formal Operational Period
The final stage in Piaget’s theory is the formal operational period, which typically begins around 11 years of age. In this stage, children begin to apply their operations to abstract concepts in addition to concrete objects. Indeed, during this stage, youngsters come to enjoy the heady contemplation of abstract concepts. Many adolescents spend hours mulling over hypothetical possibilities related to abstractions such as justice, love, and free will.
According to Piaget, youngsters graduate to relatively adult modes of thinking in the formal operations stage. He did not mean to suggest that no further cognitive development occurs once children reach this stage. However, he believed that after children achieve formal operations, further developments in thinking are changes in degree rather than fundamental changes in the nature of thinking.
Adolescents in the formal operational period become more systematic in their problem-solving efforts. Children in earlier developmental stages tend to attack problems quickly, with a trial-and-error approach. In contrast, children who have achieved formal operations are more likely to think things through. They envision possible courses of action and try to use logic to reason out the likely consequences of each possible solution before they act. Thus, thought processes in the formal operational period can be characterized as abstract, systematic, logical, and reflective.
In many aspects, Piaget appears to have underestimated young children’s cognitive development (Birney et al., 2005). For example, researchers have found evidence that children understand object permanence and are capable of some symbolic thought much earlier than Piaget thought (Birney & Sternberg, 2011). Similarly, some evidence suggests that preoperational children are not as egocentric as Piaget believed (Moll & Meltzoff, 2011).
Another problem is that children often simultaneously display patterns of thinking that are characteristic of several stages. This “mixing” of stages and the fact that the transitions between stages are gradual rather than abrupt call into question the value of organizing cognitive development in terms of stages (Bjorklund, 2012). Progress in children’s thinking appears to occur in overlapping waves rather than distinct stages with clear boundaries.
Piaget believed that his theory described universal processes that should lead children everywhere to progress through uniform stages of thinking at roughly the same ages. Subsequent research has shown that the sequence of stages is largely invariant, but the timetable that children follow in passing through these stages varies considerably across cultures (Molitor & Hsu, 2011; Rogoff, 2003). Thus, Piaget underestimated the influence of cultural factors on cognitive development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Albert Thomas and Stella Chess

A

Alexander Thomas and Stella Chess have conducted a major longitudinal study of the development of temperament. In a longitudinal design, investigators observe one group of participants repeatedly over a period of time. In a cross-sectional design, investigators compare groups of participants of differing age at a single point in time.
Thomas and Chess found that “temperamental individuality is well established by the time the infant is two to three months old” (Thomas & Chess, 1977, p. 153). They identified three basic styles of temperament that were apparent in most of the children. About 40 percent of the youngsters were easy children who tended to be happy, regular in sleeping and eating, adaptable, and not readily upset. Another 15 percent were slow-to-warm-up children who tended to be less cheery, less regular in their sleeping and eating, and slower in adapting to change. These children were wary of new experiences, and their emotional reactivity was moderate. Difficult children constituted 10 percent of the group. They tended to be glum, erratic in sleeping and eating, resistant to change, and relatively irritable. The remaining 35 percent of the children showed mixtures of these three temperaments.
According to Chess and Thomas (1996), a child’s temperament at three months was a fair predictor of the child’s temperament at age ten. Infants categorized as “difficult” developed more emotional problems requiring counseling than other children did. Although basic changes in temperament were seen in some children, temperament was generally stable over time.
Individual differences in temperament appear to be influenced to a considerable degree by heredity. Although temperament tends to be fairly stable over time, theorists emphasize that it is not unchangeable. Interestingly, there appear to be some modest cultural differences in the prevalence of specific temperamental styles. For example, an inhibited temperament is seen somewhat more frequently among Chinese children in comparison to North American children. It is not clear whether this disparity is rooted in genetic differences, cultural practices, or both.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Lev Vygotsky

A

Vygotsky was a prominent Russian psychologist whose research ended prematurely in 1934 when he died of tuberculosis at the age of 37. Western scientists had little exposure to his ideas until the 1960s, and it was only in 1986 that a complete version of his principal book, Thought and Language (Vygotsky, 1934), was published in English. His theory has become and continues to be very influential (Clarà, 2017).
Vygotsky’s and Piaget’s perspectives on cognitive development have much in common, but they also differ in several important respects (Lourenco, 2012). First, in Piaget’s theory, cognitive development is primarily fuelled by individual children’s active exploration of the world around them. The child is viewed as the agent of change. In contrast, Vygotsky places enormous emphasis on how children’s cognitive development is fuelled by social interactions with parents, teachers, and older children who can provide invaluable guidance. Second, Piaget viewed cognitive development as a universal process that should unfold in largely the same way across widely disparate cultures. Vygotsky, on the other hand, asserted that culture exerts great influence over how cognitive growth unfolds (Wertsch & Tulviste, 2005) so that cognitive development may not be universal as assumed in Piaget’s theory. For Vygotsky, culture is a critical factor in how cognitive development unfolds. For example, the cognitive skills acquired in literate cultures that rely on schools for training will differ from those skills acquired in tribal societies with no formal schooling. Third, Piaget viewed children’s gradual mastery of language as just another aspect of cognitive development, whereas Vygotsky argued that language acquisition plays a crucial, central role in fostering cognitive development (Kozulin, 2005). In fact, language and communication are seen to be crucial since cognitive development is affected by the information shared during cooperative activities with able partners.
According to Vygotsky, children acquire most of their culture’s cognitive skills and problem-solving strategies through collaborative dialogues with more experienced members of their society. Vygotsky’s emphasis on the primacy of language is reflected in his discussion of private speech. Preschool children talk aloud to themselves a lot as they go about their activities. Piaget viewed this speech as egocentric and insignificant. Vygotsky argued that children use this private speech to plan their strategies, regulate their actions, and accomplish their goals. As children grow older, this private speech is internalized and becomes the normal verbal dialogue that people have with themselves as they go about their business. Thus, language increasingly serves as the foundation for youngsters’ cognitive processes.
He saw cognitive development as more like an apprenticeship than a journey of individual discovery. His emphasis on the social origins of cognitive development is apparent in his theoretical concepts, such as the zone of proximal development and scaffolding.
The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is the gap between what a learner can accomplish alone and what he or she can achieve with guidance from more skilled partners. The ZPD for a task is the area in which new cognitive growth is likely and the area that should be the focus of instructional efforts. These efforts are more likely to be helpful when an instructor practices scaffolding: Scaffolding facilitates learning. Scaffolding occurs when the assistance provided to a child is adjusted as learning progresses. Typically, less and less help is provided as a child’s competence on a task increases.
Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory is guiding a great deal of contemporary research on cognitive development. This research has provided empirical support for many of Vygotsky’s ideas. Like Piaget’s theory, Vygotsky’s perspective promises to enrich our understanding of how children’s thinking develops and matures. Vygotsky’s theory continues to impact contemporary thinking about development and education.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Accommodation

A

Accommodation involves changing existing mental structures to explain new experiences. For instance, a child who has learned to call four-legged pets “puppies” may apply this scheme the first time she encounters a cat (assimilation), but she will eventually discover that puppies and cats are different types of animals and make adjustments to her mental schemes (accommodation).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Age of Viability

A

Sometime between 22 weeks and 26 weeks the fetus reaches the age of viability—the age at which a baby can survive in the event of a premature birth.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Assimilation

A

Assimilation involves interpreting new experiences in terms of existing mental structures without changing them. For instance, a child who has learned to call four-legged pets “puppies” may apply this scheme the first time she encounters a cat (assimilation), but she will eventually discover that puppies and cats are different types of animals and make adjustments to her mental schemes (accommodation).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Attachment

A

Attachment refers to the close, emotional bonds of affection that develop between infants and their caregivers.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Centration

A

Centration is the tendency to focus on just one feature of a problem, neglecting other important aspects.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Cognitive Development

A

Cognitive development refers to transitions in youngsters’ patterns of thinking, including reasoning, remembering, and problem solving.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Cohort Effects

A

Cohort effects occur when differences between age groups are due to the groups growing up in different time periods. For example, if you used the cross-sectional method to examine gender roles in groups aged 20, 40, and 60 years, you would be comparing people who grew up before, during, and after the women’s movement, which would probably lead to major differences as a result of historical context rather than development.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Conservation

A

Conservation is Piaget’s term for the awareness that physical quantities remain constant in spite of changes in their shape or appearance.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Cross-Sectional Design

A

In a cross-sectional design, investigators compare groups of participants of differing age at a single point in time. For example, in a cross-sectional study, an investigator tracing the growth of children’s vocabulary might compare 50 six-year-olds, 50 eight-year-olds, and 50 ten-year-olds. In contrast, an investigator using the longitudinal method would assemble one group of 50 six-year-olds and measure their vocabulary at age six, again at age eight, and once more at age ten.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Crystallized Intelligence

A

Crystallized intelligence involves the ability to deduce secondary relational abstractions by applying previously learned primary relational abstractions. Crystallized intelligence includes learned procedures and knowledge. It reflects the effects of experience and acculturation. Horn notes that crystallized ability is a “precipitate out of experience,” resulting from the prior application of fluid ability that has been combined with the intelligence of culture. Examples of tasks that measure crystallized intelligence are vocabulary, general information, abstract word analogies, and the mechanics of language.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Dementia

A

Dementia is an abnormal condition marked by multiple cognitive deficits that include memory impairment.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Development

A

Your life provides an interesting illustration of the two themes that permeate the study of human development: transition and continuity. In investigating human development, psychologists study how people evolve through transitions over time. In looking at these transitions, developmental psychologists inevitably find continuity with the past.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Developmental Norms

A

Developmental norms indicate the median age at which individuals display various behaviors and abilities. Developmental norms are useful benchmarks as long as parents don’t expect their children to progress exactly at the pace specified in the norms. Some parents become unnecessarily alarmed when their children fall behind developmental norms, but variations from the typical age of accomplishment are entirely normal.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Dishabituation

A

Dishabituation occurs if a new stimulus elicits an increase in the strength of a habituated response. Patterns of dishabituation can give researchers insights into what types of events infants can tell apart, which events surprise or interest them, and which events violate their expectations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Egocentrism

A

Egocentrism in thinking is characterized by a limited ability to share another person’s viewpoint.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

Embryonic Stage

A

The embryonic stage is the second stage of prenatal development, lasting from two weeks until the end of the second month. During this stage, most of the vital organs and bodily systems begin to form in the developing organism, which is now called an embryo.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

Family Life Cycle

A

We can divide the lifespan into four broad periods: (1) the prenatal period, between conception and birth, (2) childhood, (3) adolescence, and (4) adulthood

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS)

A

Fetal alcohol syndrome disorder (FASD) is a collection of congenital (inborn) problems associated with excessive alcohol use during pregnancy.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

Fetal Stage

A

The fetal stage is the third stage of prenatal development, lasting from two months through birth. Some highlights of fetal development are summarized in Figure 11.1. The first two months of the fetal stage bring rapid bodily growth, as muscles and bones begin to form (Moore, Persaud, & Torchia, 2013). The developing organism, now called a fetus, becomes capable of physical movements as skeletal structures harden. Organs formed in the embryonic stage continue to grow and gradually begin to function. Sex organs start to develop during the third month.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

Fluid Intelligence

A

Fluid intelligence is the ability to solve novel reasoning problems and is correlated with a number of important skills such as comprehension, problem-solving, and learning.Fluid intelligence involved basic processes of reasoning and other mental activities that depend only minimally on prior learning (such as formal and informal education) and acculturation. Horn notes that it is formless and can “flow into” a wide variety of cognitive activities. Tasks measuring fluid reasoning require the ability to solve abstract reasoning problems. Examples of tasks that measure fluid intelligence include figure classifications, figural analyses, number and letter series, matrices, and paired associates.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

Gender

A

Gender usually refers to culturally constructed distinctions between femininity and masculinity.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

Gender Differences

A

Gender differences are actual disparities between the sexes in typical behavior or average ability.

33
Q

Gender Roles

A

Gender roles are expectations about what is appropriate behavior for each sex.

34
Q

Gender Stereotypes

A

A gender stereotype is a generalized view or preconception about attributes, or characteristics that are or ought to be possessed by women and men or the roles that are or should be performed by men and women. Gender stereotypes can be both positive and negative for example, “women are nurturing” or “women are weak”.

35
Q

Germinal Stage

A

The germinal stage is the first phase of prenatal development, encompassing the first two weeks after conception. This brief stage begins when a zygote is created through fertilization. Within 36 hours, rapid cell division begins and the zygote becomes a microscopic mass of multiplying cells. This mass of cells slowly migrates along the mother’s fallopian tube to the uterine cavity. On about the seventh day, the cell mass begins to implant itself in the uterine wall. This process takes about a week and is far from automatic. Many zygotes are rejected at this point. As many as one in five pregnancies end with the woman never being aware that conception has occurred.

36
Q

Habituation

A

Habituation is a gradual reduction in the strength of a response when a stimulus event is presented repeatedly. If you show infants the same event over and over (e.g., an object dropping onto a platform), they habituate to it—their heart and respiration rates decline and they spend less time looking at the stimulus.

37
Q

Irreversibility

A

Irreversibility is the inability to envision reversing an action. Preoperational children can’t mentally “undo” something. For instance, in grappling with the conservation of water, they don’t think about what would happen if the water was poured back from the tall beaker into the original beaker.

38
Q

Longitudinal Design

A

In a longitudinal design, investigators observe one group of participants repeatedly over a period of time.

39
Q

Maturation

A

Maturation is development that reflects the gradual unfolding of one’s genetic blueprint. It is a product of genetically programmed physical changes that come with age, rather than through experience and learning.

40
Q

Menarche

A

In females, puberty is typically signaled by menarche—the first occurrence of menstruation, which reflects the culmination of a series of hormonal changes.

41
Q

Motor Development

A

Motor development refers to the progression of muscular coordination required for physical activities. Basic motor skills include grasping and reaching for objects, manipulating objects, sitting up, crawling, walking, and running.

42
Q

Object Permanence

A

Object permanence develops when a child recognizes that objects continue to exist even when they are no longer visible.

43
Q

Placenta

A

The placenta is a structure that allows oxygen and nutrients to pass into the fetus from the mother’s bloodstream, and bodily wastes to pass out to the mother. This critical exchange takes place across thin membranes that block the passage of blood cells, keeping the fetal and maternal bloodstreams separate.

44
Q

Prenatal Period

A

The prenatal period extends from conception to birth, usually encompassing nine months of pregnancy. Significant development occurs before birth. In fact, development during the prenatal period is remarkably rapid. If you were an average-sized newborn and your physical growth had continued during the first year of your life at a prenatal pace, by your first birthday you would have weighed 90 kilograms! Fortunately, you didn’t grow at that rate because in the final weeks before birth the frenzied pace of prenatal development tapers off dramatically.

45
Q

Primary Sex Characteristics

A

It is during puberty that the primary sex characteristics—the structures necessary for reproduction—develop fully. In the male, these include the testes, penis, and related internal structures. Primary sex characteristics in the female include the ovaries, vagina, uterus, and other internal structures.

46
Q

Puberty

A

Puberty is the stage during which sexual functions reach maturity, which marks the beginning of adolescence.

47
Q

Pubescence

A

Pubescence is the time when puberty begins.

48
Q

Secondary Sex Characteristics

A

The secondary sex characteristics are the physical features that distinguish one sex from the other but that are not essential for reproduction—such as facial hair and broader shoulders in males, and breast growth and wider hips in females.

49
Q

separation anxiety

A

Separation anxiety refers to the emotional distress seen in many infants when they are separated from people with whom they have formed an attachment. Separation anxiety, which may occur with fathers and other familiar caregivers as well as with mothers, typically peaks at around 14 to 18 months and then begins to decline.

50
Q

Sex

A

Sex usually refers to the biologically based categories of female and male.

51
Q

Socialization

A

Socialization is the acquisition of the norms and behaviors expected of people in a particular society. In all cultures, the socialization process includes efforts to train children about gender roles.

52
Q

Stage

A

A stage is a developmental period during which characteristic patterns of behavior are exhibited and certain capacities become established. Stage theories assume that (1) individuals must progress through specified stages in a particular order because each stage builds on the previous stage, (2) progress through these stages is strongly related to age, and (3) development is marked by major discontinuities that usher in dramatic transitions in behavior.

53
Q

Stage Theory

A

Erikson’s theory of personality development posits that people evolve through eight stages over their lifespan. Each stage is marked by a psychosocial crisis that involves confronting a fundamental question, such as “Who am I and where am I going?” The stages are described in terms of alternative traits that are potential outcomes from the crises. Development is enhanced when a crisis is resolved in favor of the healthier alternative

54
Q

Strange Situation Procedure

A

Ainsworth used a method called the strange situation procedure, in which infants are exposed to a series of eight separation and reunion episodes to assess the quality of their attachment. The three-minute episodes in this carefully orchestrated laboratory procedure involve events such as a stranger entering a room where an infant is playing with a parent nearby, followed by the parent leaving, returning, leaving, and returning again. The child’s reactions (distress, comfort) to the parent’s departures and returns are carefully monitored to gauge attachment quality.

55
Q

Temperament

A

Temperament refers to characteristic mood, activity level, and emotional reactivity.

56
Q

Teratogens

A

Teratogens are any external agents, such as drugs or viruses, that can harm an embryo or fetus.

57
Q

Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD)

A

The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is the gap between what a learner can accomplish alone and what he or she can achieve with guidance from more skilled partners. The ZPD for a task is the area in which new cognitive growth is likely and the area that should be the focus of instructional efforts.

58
Q

Zygote

A

A zygote is a fertilized ovum.

59
Q

Outline the major events of the three phases of prenatal development.

A

Germinal Stage
The germinal stage is the first phase of prenatal development, encompassing the first two weeks after conception. This brief stage begins when a zygote is created through fertilization. Within 36 hours, rapid cell division begins and the zygote becomes a microscopic mass of multiplying cells. This mass of cells slowly migrates along the mother’s fallopian tube to the uterine cavity. On about the seventh day, the cell mass begins to implant itself in the uterine wall. This process takes about a week and is far from automatic. Many zygotes are rejected at this point. As many as one in five pregnancies end with the woman never being aware that conception has occurred.
During the implantation process, the placenta begins to form. The placenta is a structure that allows oxygen and nutrients to pass into the fetus from the mother’s bloodstream, and bodily wastes to pass out to the mother. This critical exchange takes place across thin membranes that block the passage of blood cells, keeping the fetal and maternal bloodstreams separate.
Embryonic Stage
The embryonic stage is the second stage of prenatal development, lasting from two weeks until the end of the second month. During this stage, most of the vital organs and bodily systems begin to form in the developing organism, which is now called an embryo. Structures such as the heart, spine, and brain emerge gradually as cell division becomes more specialized. Although the embryo is typically only about 2.5 cm long at the end of this stage, it’s already beginning to look human. Arms, legs, hands, feet, fingers, toes, eyes, and ears are already discernible.
The embryonic stage is a period of great vulnerability because virtually all the basic physiological structures are being formed. If anything interferes with normal development during the embryonic phase, the effects can be devastating. Most miscarriages occur during this period (Simpson & Jauniaux, 2012). Most major structural birth defects also result from problems that occur during the embryonic stage (Niebyl & Simpson, 2012).
Fetal Stage
The fetal stage is the third stage of prenatal development, lasting from two months through birth. Some highlights of fetal development are summarized in Figure 11.1. The first two months of the fetal stage bring rapid bodily growth, as muscles and bones begin to form (Moore, Persaud, & Torchia, 2013). The developing organism, now called a fetus, becomes capable of physical movements as skeletal structures harden. Organs formed in the embryonic stage continue to grow and gradually begin to function. Sex organs start to develop during the third month.
During the final three months of the prenatal period, brain cells multiply at a brisk pace. A layer of fat is deposited under the skin to provide insulation, and the respiratory and digestive systems mature. All of these changes ready the fetus for life outside the cosy, supportive environment of its mother’s womb. Sometime between 22 weeks and 26 weeks the fetus reaches the age of viability—the age at which a baby can survive in the event of a premature birth (Moore & Persaud, 2008).
Sometime between 23 weeks and 25 weeks, the fetus reaches the threshold of viability—the age at which a baby can survive in the event of a premature birth. At 23 weeks, the probability of survival is still slim (about 20 percent), but it climbs rapidly to around a 67 percent survival rate at 25 weeks (Seaton et al., 2013). Unfortunately, a great many of the premature infants born near the threshold of viability go on to experience a wide range of developmental problems

60
Q

Summarize the impact of environmental factors, including maternal health care, on prenatal development.

A

Although the fetus develops in the protective buffer of the womb, events in the external environment can affect it indirectly through the mother. Because the developing organism and its mother are linked through the placenta, a mother’s eating habits, drug use, and physical health, among other things, can affect prenatal development and have long-term health consequences. Teratogens are any external agents, such as drugs or viruses, that can harm an embryo or fetus.
Maternal Drug Use
A major source of concern about fetal and infant well-being is the mother’s consumption of drugs, including such widely used substances as tobacco and alcohol as well as prescription and recreational drugs. Unfortunately, most drugs consumed by a pregnant woman can pass through the membranes of the placenta.
Virtually all “recreational” drugs (see Chapter 5) can be harmful, with sedatives, narcotics, and cocaine being particularly dangerous. Babies of heroin users are born addicted to narcotics and have an increased risk of early death due to prematurity, birth defects, respiratory difficulties, and problems associated with their addiction (Finnegan & Kandall, 2005). Prenatal exposure to cocaine is associated with increased risk of birth complications (Sokol et al., 2007) and a variety of cognitive deficits that are apparent in childhood (Singer et al., 2002, 2004).
Over-the-counter drugs. The impact of drugs on the embryo or fetus varies greatly depending on the drug, the dose, and the phase of prenatal development.
Alcohol consumption during pregnancy also carries risks. It has long been clear that heavy drinking by a mother can be hazardous to a fetus. Fetal alcohol syndrome disorder (FASD) is a collection of congenital (inborn) problems associated with excessive alcohol use during pregnancy. According to figures released by Health Canada, the estimates are that one in nine babies born in Canada each year are affected by FASD and that approximately 300 000 Canadians currently are living with its effects. Typical problems include microcephaly (a small head), heart defects, irritability, hyperactivity, and delayed mental and motor development. Fetal alcohol syndrome is the most common known cause of intellectual disability, and it is related to an increased incidence of difficulties in school, depression, suicide, drug problems, and criminal behaviour in adolescence and adulthood.
Furthermore, many children fall short of the criteria for fetal alcohol syndrome but still show serious impairments attributable to their mothers’ drinking during pregnancy. A long-running study of pregnant women’s drinking found that higher alcohol intake was associated with an elevated risk for deficits in IQ, motor skills, and attention span, and with increased impulsive, antisocial, and delinquent behavior. Clearly, even moderate drinking during pregnancy can have enduring and substantial negative effects.
Tobacco use during pregnancy is also hazardous to prenatal development. Smoking appears to increase a mother’s risk for miscarriage, stillbirth, and prematurity, and newborns’ risk for sudden infant death syndrome. Prenatal exposure to tobacco is also associated with slower than average cognitive development, attention deficits, hyperactivity, and conduct problems, although it is difficult to tease out the causal relationships that may be at work.
Maternal Nutrition
Maternal nutrition is very important because the developing fetus needs a variety of essential nutrients. Severe maternal malnutrition increases the risk of birth complications and neurological deficits for the newborn. The impact of moderate malnutrition is more difficult to gauge because it is often confounded with other risk factors associated with poverty, such as drug abuse and limited access to health care.
Still, even when pregnant women have ample access to food, it is important for them to consume a balanced diet that includes essential vitamins and minerals. Guidelines are available from Health Canada regarding eating habits designed to promote healthy maternal nutrition and for material weight gain during pregnancy. Too much or too little weight gain during gestation is associated with a variety of birth complications, and guidelines for maternal weight gain are based on pre-pregnancy body mass index. Health Canada suggestions include having expectant mothers follow Canada’s Food Guide, having at least 150 g of cooked fish each week, taking multivitamins containing 0.4 mg of folic acid each day, and monitoring weight gain in light of Health Canada’s guidelines.
Stress and Emotion
Recent studies suggest that maternal emotions in reaction to stressful events can have an impact on prenatal development. For example, elevated levels of prenatal stress have been found to be associated with increased stillbirths (Hogue et al., 2013), impaired immune response (Veru et al., 2014), heightened vulnerability to infectious disease (Nielsen et al., 2010), slowed motor development (Cao et al., 2014), below-average cognitive development (Tarabulsy et al., 2014), and social deficits (Walder et al., 2014). Why is prenatal stress so harmful? Research suggests that prospective mothers’ emotional reactions to stressful events can disrupt the delicate hormonal balance that fosters healthy prenatal development (Douglas, 2010).
Maternal Illness
The placenta screens out quite a number of infectious agents, but not all. Thus, many maternal illnesses can interfere with prenatal development. Diseases such as measles, rubella (German measles), syphilis, and chickenpox can be hazardous to the fetus (Bernstein, 2007); the nature of any damage depends, in part, on when the mother contracts the illness. The HIV virus that causes AIDS can also be transmitted by pregnant women to their offspring. The transmission of AIDS can occur prenatally through the placenta, during delivery, or through breastfeeding. Up through the mid-1990s, about 20–30 percent of HIV-positive pregnant women passed the virus on to their babies, but improved antiretroviral drugs (given to the mother) and more cautious obstetrical care have reduced this figure significantly (Cotter & Potter, 2006).
Environmental Toxins
Research also suggests that babies in the womb are exposed to a surprising variety of environmental toxins that can affect them (Houlihan et al., 2005). For example, prenatal exposure to air pollution has been linked to impairments in cognitive development at age five and increased obesity at age seven. In a similar vein, exposure to the chemicals used in flame-retardant materials correlates with slower mental and physical development up through age six.

61
Q

Describe the general trends and cultural variations in motor development.

A

Cross-cultural research has highlighted the dynamic interplay between experience and maturation in motor development. Relatively rapid motor development has been observed in some cultures that provide special practice in basic motor skills (Adolph, Karasik, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2010). For example, the Kipsigis people of Kenya begin active efforts to train their infants to sit up, stand, and walk soon after birth. Thanks to this training, Kipsigis children achieve these developmental milestones (but not others) about a month earlier than babies in North America (Super, 1976). In contrast, relatively slow motor development has been found in some cultures that discourage motor exploration. For example, among the Aché, a nomadic people living in the rain forests of Paraguay, safety concerns dictate that children under three rarely venture more than a metre from their mothers, who carry them virtually everywhere. As a result of these constraints, Aché children are delayed in acquiring a variety of motor skills and typically begin walking about a year later than other children (Kaplan & Dove, 1987).
Cultural variations in the emergence of basic motor skills demonstrate that environmental factors can accelerate or slow down early motor development. Nonetheless, the similarities across cultures in the sequence and timing of early motor development outweigh the differences. This fact suggests that early motor development depends to a considerable extent on maturation. Later motor development is another matter, however. As children in any culture grow older, they acquire more specialized motor skills, some of which may be unique to their culture.

62
Q

Summarize the findings of Thomas and Chess’s longitudinal study of infant temperament.

A

Thomas and Chess found that “temperamental individuality is well established by the time the infant is two to three months old”. They identified three basic styles of temperament that were apparent in most of the children. About 40 percent of the youngsters were easy children who tended to be happy, regular in sleeping and eating, adaptable, and not readily upset. Another 15 percent were slow-to-warm-up children who tended to be less cheery, less regular in their sleeping and eating, and slower in adapting to change. These children were wary of new experiences, and their emotional reactivity was moderate. Difficult children constituted 10 percent of the group. They tended to be glum, erratic in sleeping and eating, resistant to change, and relatively irritable. The remaining 35 percent of the children showed mixtures of these three temperaments.
According to Chess and Thomas, a child’s temperament at three months was a fair predictor of the child’s temperament at age ten. Infants categorized as “difficult” developed more emotional problems requiring counseling than other children did. Although basic changes in temperament were seen in some children, temperament was generally stable over time.

63
Q

Summarize the research on infant-caregiver attachment, including cultural variations.

A

Research by Ainsworth and her colleagues suggests that attachment emerges out of a complex interplay between infant and mother. Ainsworth used a method called the strange situation procedure, in which infants are exposed to a series of eight separation and reunion episodes to assess the quality of their attachment. The three-minute episodes in this carefully orchestrated laboratory procedure involve events such as a stranger entering a room where an infant is playing with a parent nearby, followed by the parent leaving, returning, leaving, and returning again. The child’s reactions (distress, comfort) to the parent’s departures and returns are carefully monitored to gauge attachment quality.
Infant–mother attachments vary in quality. Ainsworth and her colleagues found that these attachments follow three patterns. Fortunately, most infants develop a secure attachment. They play and explore comfortably with their mother present, become visibly upset when she leaves, and are quickly calmed by her return. However, some children display a pattern called anxious-ambivalent attachment. They appear anxious even when their mother is near and protest excessively when she leaves, but they are not particularly comforted when she returns. Children in the third category seek little contact with their mother and often are not distressed when she leaves, a condition labeled avoidant attachment. Years later, other researchers added a fourth category called disorganized-disoriented attachment. These children appear confused about whether they should approach or avoid their mother and are especially insecure
Separation anxiety emerges in children at about six to eight months and peaks at about 14 to 18 months in cultures around the world. These findings, which have been replicated in quite a variety of non-Western cultures, suggest that attachment is a universal feature of human development. However, studies have found some modest cultural variations in the proportion of infants who fall into the three attachment categories described by Ainsworth. Working mostly with white, middle-class subjects in the United States, researchers have found that 67 percent of infants display a secure attachment, 21 percent an avoidant attachment, and 12 percent an anxious-ambivalent attachment. Studies in Japan and Germany have yielded somewhat different estimates of the prevalence of various types of attachment. That said, the differences are small and secure attachment appears to be the predominant type of attachment around the world.

64
Q

Outline Erikson’s stages of childhood personality development, and critique his theory.

A

Trust versus Mistrust
Erikson’s first stage encompasses the first year of life, when an infant has to depend completely on adults to take care of its basic needs for such necessities as food, a warm blanket, and changed diapers. If an infant’s basic biological needs are adequately met by his or her caregivers and sound attachments are formed, the child should develop an optimistic, trusting attitude toward the world. However, if the infant’s basic needs are taken care of poorly, a more distrusting, pessimistic personality may result.
Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
Erikson’s second stage unfolds during the second and third years of life, when parents begin toilet training and other efforts to regulate the child’s behavior. The child must begin to take some personal responsibility for feeding, dressing, and bathing. If all goes well, he or she acquires a sense of self-sufficiency. But, if parents are never satisfied with the child’s efforts and there are constant parent–child conflicts, the child may develop a sense of personal shame and self-doubt.
Initiative versus Guilt
In Erikson’s third stage, lasting roughly from ages three to six, children experiment and take initiatives that may sometimes conflict with their parents’ rules. Overcontrolling parents may begin to instill feelings of guilt, and self-esteem may suffer. Parents need to support their children’s emerging independence while maintaining appropriate controls. In the ideal situation, children will retain their sense of initiative while learning to respect the rights and privileges of other family members.
Industry versus Inferiority
In the fourth stage (age six through puberty), the challenge of learning to function socially is extended beyond the family to the broader social realm of the neighborhood and school. Children who are able to function effectively in this less nurturant social sphere where productivity is highly valued should learn to value achievement and to take pride in accomplishment, resulting in a sense of competence.
The strength of Erikson’s theory is that it accounts for both continuity and transition in personality development. It accounts for transition by showing how new challenges in social relationships stimulate personality development throughout life. It accounts for continuity by drawing connections between early childhood experiences and aspects of adult personality. One measure of a theory’s value is how much research it generates, and Erikson’s theory continues to guide a fair amount of research (Thomas, 2005).
On the negative side of the ledger, Erikson’s theory has depended heavily on illustrative case studies, which are open to varied interpretations (Thomas, 2005). Another weakness is that the theory provides an “idealized” description of “typical” developmental patterns. Thus, it’s not well suited for explaining the enormous personality differences that exist among people. Inadequate explanation of individual differences is a common problem with stage theories of development. This shortcoming surfaces again in the next section, where we’ll examine Jean Piaget’s stage theory of cognitive development.

65
Q

Outline Piaget’s stages of cognitive development, and critique his theory.

A

Sensorimotor Period
One of Piaget’s foremost contributions was to greatly enhance our understanding of mental development in the earliest months of life. The first stage in his theory is the sensorimotor period, which lasts from birth to about age two. Piaget called this stage sensorimotor because infants are developing the ability to coordinate their sensory input with their motor actions.
The major development during the sensorimotor stage is the gradual appearance of symbolic thought. At the beginning of this stage, a child’s behaviour is dominated by innate reflexes. But by the end of the stage, the child can use mental symbols to represent objects (e.g., a mental image of a favourite toy). The key to this transition is the acquisition of the concept of object permanence.
Object permanence develops when a child recognizes that objects continue to exist even when they are no longer visible. Although you surely take the permanence of objects for granted, infants aren’t aware of this permanence at first. If you show a four-month-old an eye-catching toy and then cover the toy with a pillow, the child will not attempt to search for the toy. Piaget inferred from this observation that the child does not understand that the toy continues to exist under the pillow. The notion of object permanence does not dawn on children overnight. The first signs of this insight usually appear between four and eight months of age, when children will often pursue an object that is partially covered in their presence. Progress is gradual, and Piaget believed that children typically don’t master the concept of object permanence until they’re about 18 months old.
Preoperational Period
During the preoperational period, which extends roughly from age two to age seven, children gradually improve in their use of mental images. Although progress in symbolic thought continues, Piaget emphasized the shortcomings in preoperational thought.
Why are preoperational children unable to solve conservation problems? According to Piaget, their inability to understand conservation is due to some basic flaws in preoperational thinking. These flaws include centration, irreversibility, and egocentrism.
Centration is the tendency to focus on just one feature of a problem, neglecting other important aspects. They have difficulty focusing on several aspects of a problem at once.
Irreversibility is the inability to envision reversing an action. Preoperational children can’t mentally “undo” something. For instance, in grappling with the conservation of water, they don’t think about what would happen if the water was poured back from the tall beaker into the original beaker.
Egocentrism in thinking is characterized by a limited ability to share another person’s viewpoint. Indeed, Piaget felt that preoperational children fail to appreciate that there are points of view other than their own. For instance, if you ask a preoperational girl whether her sister has a sister, she’ll probably say no if they are the only two girls in the family. She’s unable to view sisterhood from her sister’s perspective (this also shows irreversibility).
A notable feature of egocentrism is animism—the belief that all things are living, just like oneself. Thus, youngsters attribute lifelike, human qualities to inanimate objects, asking questions such as “When does the ocean stop to rest?” or “Why does the wind get so mad?”
As you can see, Piaget emphasized the weaknesses apparent in preoperational thought. Indeed, that is why he called this stage preoperational. The ability to perform operations—internal transformations, manipulations, and reorganizations of mental structures—emerges in the next stage
Concrete Operational Period
The development of mental operations marks the beginning of the concrete operational period, which usually lasts from about age 7 to age 11. Piaget called this stage concrete operations because children can perform operations only on images of tangible objects and actual events.
Among the operations that children master during this stage are reversibility and decentration. Reversibility permits a child to mentally undo an action. Decentration allows the child to focus on more than one feature of a problem simultaneously. The newfound ability to coordinate several aspects of a problem helps the child appreciate that there are several ways to look at things. This ability in turn leads to a decline in egocentrism and gradual mastery of conservation as it applies to liquid, mass, number, volume, area, and length.
As children master concrete operations, they develop a variety of new problem-solving capacities. Generally, preoperational children can’t handle hierarchical classification problems that require them to focus simultaneously on two levels of classification. However, the child who has advanced to the concrete operational stage is not as limited by centration and can work successfully with hierarchical classification problems.
Formal Operational Period
The final stage in Piaget’s theory is the formal operational period, which typically begins around 11 years of age. In this stage, children begin to apply their operations to abstract concepts in addition to concrete objects. Indeed, during this stage, youngsters come to enjoy the heady contemplation of abstract concepts. Many adolescents spend hours mulling over hypothetical possibilities related to abstractions such as justice, love, and free will.
According to Piaget, youngsters graduate to relatively adult modes of thinking in the formal operations stage. He did not mean to suggest that no further cognitive development occurs once children reach this stage. However, he believed that after children achieve formal operations, further developments in thinking are changes in degree rather than fundamental changes in the nature of thinking.
Adolescents in the formal operational period become more systematic in their problem-solving efforts. Children in earlier developmental stages tend to attack problems quickly, with a trial-and-error approach. In contrast, children who have achieved formal operations are more likely to think things through. They envision possible courses of action and try to use logic to reason out the likely consequences of each possible solution before they act. Thus, thought processes in the formal operational period can be characterized as abstract, systematic, logical, and reflective.
Jean Piaget made a landmark contribution to psychology’s understanding of children in general and their cognitive development in particular. Piaget’s theory guided an enormous volume of productive research that continues through today. This research has supported many of Piaget’s central ideas. In such a far-reaching theory, however, there are bound to be some weak spots. Here are some criticisms of Piaget’s theory:
In many aspects, Piaget appears to have underestimated young children’s cognitive development. For example, researchers have found evidence that children understand object permanence and are capable of some symbolic thought much earlier than Piaget thought. Similarly, some evidence suggests that preoperational children are not as egocentric as Piaget believed.
Another problem is that children often simultaneously display patterns of thinking that are characteristic of several stages. This “mixing” of stages and the fact that the transitions between stages are gradual rather than abrupt call into question the value of organizing cognitive development in terms of stages. Progress in children’s thinking appears to occur in overlapping waves rather than distinct stages with clear boundaries.
Piaget believed that his theory described universal processes that should lead children everywhere to progress through uniform stages of thinking at roughly the same ages. Subsequent research has shown that the sequence of stages is largely invariant, but the timetable that children follow in passing through these stages varies considerably across cultures. Thus, Piaget underestimated the influence of cultural factors on cognitive development.

66
Q

Summarize the debate about whether some cognitive abilities are innate. What research methods are used to investigate the cognitive abilities of young children?

A

The frequent finding that Piaget underestimated infants’ cognitive abilities has led to a rash of research suggesting that infants have a surprising grasp of many complex concepts. The new findings have been made possible by some innovative research methods that permit investigators to draw inferences about the abilities of very young children. Many studies have made use of the habituation–dishabituation paradigm. Habituation is a gradual reduction in the strength of a response when a stimulus event is presented repeatedly. If you show infants the same event over and over (e.g., an object dropping onto a platform), they habituate to it—their heart and respiration rates decline and they spend less time looking at the stimulus. Dishabituation occurs if a new stimulus elicits an increase in the strength of a habituated response. Patterns of dishabituation can give researchers insights into what types of events infants can tell apart, which events surprise or interest them, and which events violate their expectations.
Working mostly with the habituation–dishabituation paradigm, researchers have discovered that infants understand basic properties of objects and some of the rules that govern them. At three to four months of age, infants understand that objects are distinct entities with boundaries, that objects move in continuous paths, that one solid object cannot pass through another, that an object cannot pass through an opening that is smaller than the object, and that objects on slopes roll down rather than up. Infants also understand that liquids are different from objects. For example, five-month-old infants expect that liquids will change shape as they move and that they can be penetrated by solid objects. In a similar vein, a recent study showed that six-month-old infants appear to understand that dried fruits derived from plants are more likely to be edible than dried fruits derived from artificial objects. In other words, they preferentially identify plants as sources of food at a surprisingly young age.
In this line of research, perhaps the most stunning discovery has been the finding that infants seem to be able to add and subtract small numbers. If five-month-old infants are shown a sequence of events in which one object is added to another behind a screen, they expect to see two objects when the screen is removed, and they exhibit surprise when their expectation is violated. According to research conducted by Yale University’s Karen Wynn, this expectation suggests that they understand that 1 + 1 = 2. Similar manipulations suggest that infants also understand that 2 − 1 = 1, that 2 + 1 = 3, and that 3 − 1 = 2. Wynn has shown in her more recent work that nine-month-old infants even have some understanding that 5 + 5 = 10 and that 10 − 5 = 5.
Again and again in recent years, research has shown that infants appear to understand surprisingly complex concepts that they have had virtually no opportunity to learn about. These findings have led some theorists to conclude that certain basic cognitive abilities are biologically built into humans’ neural architecture. The theorists who have reached this conclusion tend to fall into two camps: nativists and evolutionary theorists. The nativists simply assert that humans are prewired to readily understand certain concepts without making any assumptions about why humans are prewired in these ways. Their principal interest is to sort out the complex matter of what is prewired and what isn’t. Evolutionary theorists agree with the nativists that humans are prewired for certain cognitive abilities, but they are keenly interested in why. As you might expect, they maintain that this wiring is a product of natural selection, and they strive to understand its adaptive significance.

67
Q

Describe the milestones in the development of children’s understanding of mental states.

A

Researchers have mapped out some milestones in the development of children’s understanding of mental states (Harris, 2006; Wellman, 2002). Around age two, children begin to distinguish between mental states and overt behavior. The first mental states they understand are desires and emotions. By age three, children are talking about others’ beliefs and thoughts, as well as their desires. It is not until about age four, however, that children consistently make the connection between mental states and behavior. That is, they begin to understand how people’s beliefs, thoughts, and desires motivate and direct their behavior. Thus, they can appreciate that Harry wants to get a new watch, which would make him very happy, that he believes that it will be available at the mall, and that these mental states will motivate Harry to ask his dad to take him to the watch store. Of course, this developing theory of mind is essential if children are to competently enter their social world, where understanding others is critical.

68
Q

Outline Kohlberg’s theory of moral development, and critique his theory.

A

Kohlberg’s theory focuses on moral reasoning rather than overt behaviour. This point is best illustrated by describing Kohlberg’s method of investigation. He presented his subjects with thorny moral questions such as Heinz’s dilemma. He then asked them what the actor in the dilemma should do and, more important, why. It was the why that interested Kohlberg. He examined the nature and progression of subjects’ moral reasoning.
The result of this work is the stage theory of moral reasoning. Kohlberg found that individuals progress through a series of three levels of moral development, each of which can be broken into two sublevels, yielding a total of six stages. Each stage represents a different approach to thinking about right and wrong.
Younger children at the preconventional level think in terms of external authority. Acts are wrong because they are punished or right because they lead to positive consequences. Older children who have reached the conventional level of moral reasoning see rules as necessary for maintaining social order. They therefore accept these rules as their own. They “internalize” these rules not to avoid punishment but to be virtuous and win approval from others. Moral thinking at this stage is relatively inflexible. Rules are viewed as absolute guidelines that should be enforced rigidly.
How has Kohlberg’s theory fared in research? The central ideas have received reasonable support. Studies have shown that youngsters generally do move through Kohlberg’s stages of moral reasoning in the order he proposed. Furthermore, relations between age and level of moral reasoning are in the predicted directions. Although these findings support Kohlberg’s model, critics note that it is not unusual to find that a person shows signs of several adjacent levels of moral reasoning at a particular point in development. As we noted in the critique of Piaget, this mixing of stages is a problem for virtually all stage theories. Evidence is also mounting that Kohlberg’s theory reflects an individualistic ideology characteristic of modern Western nations that is much more culture specific than Kohlberg appreciated. Finally, a consensus is building that Kohlberg’s theory has led to a constricted focus on reasoning about interpersonal conflicts, while ignoring many other important aspects of moral development. Contemporary theorists note that moral behavior depends on many factors besides reasoning, including emotional reactions, variations in temperament, and cultural background.

69
Q

Describe the major events of puberty.

A

Soon, youngsters reach puberty—the stage during which sexual functions reach maturity, which marks the beginning of adolescence. It is during puberty that the primary sex characteristics—the structures necessary for reproduction—develop fully. In the male, these include the testes, penis, and related internal structures. Primary sex characteristics in the female include the ovaries, vagina, uterus, and other internal structures.
In females, puberty is typically signaled by menarche—the first occurrence of menstruation, which reflects the culmination of a series of hormonal changes. North American girls typically reach menarche at age 12–13, with further sexual maturation continuing until approximately age 16. North American boys typically experience spermarche—the first occurrence of ejaculation—at age 13–14, with further sexual maturation continuing until approximately age 18.
Generational changes have occurred in the timing of puberty over the last 150 years. Today’s adolescents begin puberty at a younger age, and complete it more rapidly, than their counterparts in earlier generations. This trend appears to be occurring in both genders. The reasons for this trend are the subject of debate. It seems likely that multiple factors have contributed. The most obvious potential causes are widespread improvements in nutrition and medical care, which would probably explain why the trend toward younger puberty has mostly been seen in modern, “developed” countries. Some theorists also believe that a variety of environmental pollutants serve as “endocrine disrupters” that hasten the onset of puberty.
The timing of puberty varies from one adolescent to the next. Generally, girls who mature early and boys who mature late seem to experience more subjective distress with the transition to adolescence. This experience of subjective stress may contribute to the elevated prevalence of psychological disorders seen in both groups, but especially females. However, in both males and females, early maturation is associated with greater use of tobacco, alcohol, and other drugs; more high-risk behavior; greater aggression; and more trouble with the law. Among females, early maturation is also correlated with a greater risk for eating problems. Thus, we might speculate that early maturation often thrusts both genders (but especially females) toward the adult world too soon.

70
Q

Based on the research evidence on adolescent suicide, would you conclude that adolescence is a time of turmoil?

A

Back around the turn of the last century, G. Stanley Hall, one of psychology’s great pioneers, proposed that the adolescent years are characterized by convulsive instability and disturbing inner turmoil. Hall attributed this turmoil to adolescents’ erratic physical changes and resultant confusion about self-image. While there is continuity with other life stages, there is no doubt that adolescence is a time of change and transition.
Statistics on adolescent depression and suicide would seem to support the idea that adolescence can be a time marked by turmoil and disturbance for some. We know that adolescence is a period of increased risk for a variety of problems. According to Constance Hammen at the University of California at Los Angeles, a prominent researcher in depression, depression rates in adolescents can be as high as 20 percent. As we discuss in Chapter 15, depression is a risk factor for suicide.
Turning to our original question, does the weight of evidence support the idea that adolescence is usually a period of turmoil and turbulence? Overall, recent consensus of the experts has been that adolescence is not an exceptionally difficult period. Although turbulence and turmoil are not universal features of adolescence, challenging adaptations do have to be made during this period. In particular, most adolescents struggle to some extent in their effort to achieve a sound sense of identity.

71
Q

Using research evidence on adolescent moods, behaviors, and conflicts, would you reach the same conclusion about adolescence being a time of turmoil for adolescents among Canada’s Indigenous peoples?

A

Recent analyses of suicide rates have found that ethnic and racial groups differ both in suicide rates and their precipitants. First Nations Canadians are especially vulnerable. The Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care reports that the suicide rates in this segment of Canadian society are twice the sex-specific rates and three times the age-specific rates reported for the general Canadian population. More recent data suggests the problem is even more serious, with suicide rates for First Nations youth reported to be seven times the rate of non-Aboriginal youth. The particularly high suicide rate among First Nations teens is of special concern and recently it has been getting much-needed publicity.
Canadian psychologists Michael Chandler and Christopher Lalonde have been conducting research in this area for many years. They suggest that the increased rate of suicide among First Nations Canadians is specific to certain contexts, rather than being true across that population. In some communities, the rate is as high as 800 times the national average, while in other communities, suicide is virtually unknown. Chandler and Lalonde believe that cultural continuity factors differentiate the settings where suicide rates are high, and argue that a sense of personal and cultural continuity is necessary, especially in times of change, to serve as a critical foundation for personal and cultural identity. While acknowledging the inevitability and importance of change, Chandler and Lalonde argue that a sense of personal continuity or personal persistence is fundamental to psychological health.
Chandler and Lalonde feel that two important sets of circumstances may combine to cause the high suicide levels among First Nations teens. One is the set of changes that most adolescents experience. The other arises “whenever one’s culture, out of which the particulars of one’s identity are necessarily composed, is also thrown into serious disarray. In either case, the grounds upon which a sense of self is ordinarily made to rest are cut away, life is made cheap, and the prospect of one’s own death becomes a matter of indifference”. The Featured Study for this chapter links suicide rates of First Nations youth to contexts in which cultural continuity has or has not been emphasized.

72
Q

Explain why the struggle for a sense of identity is particularly intense during adolescence. Discuss common patterns of identity formation (identity statuses).

A

Erik Erikson was especially interested in personality development during adolescence, which is the fifth of the eight major life stages he described. According to Erikson, the premier challenge of adolescence is the struggle to form a clear sense of identity. This struggle involves working out a stable concept of oneself as a unique individual and embracing an ideology or system of values that provides a sense of direction. In Erikson’s view, adolescents grapple with questions such as “Who am I, and where am I going in life?”
Recent research has focused on the consequences of identity confusion. Studies have found that identity confusion is associated with an increased risk for substance abuse, unprotected sexual activity, anxiety, low self-worth, and eating disorders.
Adolescents deal with identity formation in a variety of ways. Building on Erikson’s insights, James Marcia proposed that the presence or absence of a sense of commitment (to life goals and values) and a sense of crisis (active questioning and exploration) can combine to produce four different identity statuses. In order of increasing maturity, Marcia’s four identity statuses begin with identity diffusion, a state of rudderless apathy, with no commitment to an ideology. is a premature commitment to visions, values, and roles—typically those prescribed by one’s parents. Identity foreclosureForeclosure is associated with conformity and not being very open to new experiences. An identity moratorium involves delaying commitment for a while to experiment with alternative ideologies and careers. Identity achievement involves arriving at a sense of self and direction after some consideration of alternative possibilities.
Identity achievement is associated with higher self-esteem, conscientiousness, security, achievement motivation, and capacity for intimacy. However, research suggests that people tend to reach identity achievement at later ages than originally envisioned by Marcia. In one large-scale study, by late adolescence only 22–26 percent of the sample had reached identity achievement. Thus, the struggle for a sense of identity routinely extends into young adulthood. Indeed, some people continue to struggle with identity issues well into middle and even late adulthood.

73
Q

Summarize the evidence regarding the stability of personality and the prevalence of the midlife crisis. Outline Erikson’s three stages of adulthood.

A

Recall from Chapter 9 that psychological test scores are relative measures. They show how one scores relative to other people. Raw scores are converted into percentile scores that indicate the precise degree to which one is above or below average on a particular trait. The data indicate that these percentile scores tend to be remarkably stable over lengthy spans of time. People’s relative standing doesn’t tend to change much.
However, if we examine participants’ raw scores on fundamental personality traits, we can see meaningful developmental trends. Although adults’ mean raw scores on extraversion remain pretty stable, neuroticism scores tend to decline moderately with increasing age, while agreeableness, openness to experience, and conscientiousness tend to increase gradually. Moreover, studies show that (1) there are variations among people in the extent to which they experience personality change, (2) the biggest changes in raw scores tend to occur between the ages of 20 and 40, (3) significant changes can even occur in old age, and (4) the typical developmental trends represent “positive” changes that move people toward great social maturity. In sum, it appears that personality in adulthood is characterized by both stability and change.
Insofar as personality changes during the adult years, Erik Erikson’s (1963) theory offers some clues about the nature of changes people can expect. In his eight-stage model of development over the lifespan, Erikson divided adulthood into three stages. In the early adulthood stage, called intimacy versus isolation, the key concern is whether one can develop the capacity to share intimacy with others. Successful resolution of the challenges in this stage should promote empathy and openness. In middle adulthood, the psychosocial crisis pits generativity versus self-absorption. The key challenge is to acquire a genuine concern for the welfare of future generations, which results in providing unselfish guidance to younger people and concern with one’s legacy. During the late adulthood stage, called integrity versus despair, the challenge is to avoid the tendency to dwell on the mistakes of the past and on one’s imminent death. People need to find meaning and satisfaction in their lives, rather than wallow in bitterness and resentment. Empirical research on the adult stages in Erikson’s theory has been sparse, but generally supportive of the theory. For example, researchers have found that generativity increases between young adulthood and middle age, as Erikson’s theory predicts.

74
Q

Describe the typical transitions in family relations during the adult years.

A

Marriage
Conjugal status is a term used by Statistics Canada when reporting the nature of a relationship between cohabiting couples. The most recent data available from Statistics Canada (Milan, 2013) indicates that most Canadian couples are still opting for marriage (80.1 percent of couples) as their preferred form of cohabitation, with about one-quarter living in a common-law relationships (19.9 percent). The percentage of common-law relationships has increased from 6.3 percent in 1981 to the current rate of about 20 percent. No matter the form of the relationship, committed new couples have adjustments to make. While not all married couples are happy—the divorce rate in Canada increased to 11.5 percent in 2011 from 5.1 percent in 1981 —most new couples are pretty happy. Still, 8–14 percent of newlyweds score in the distressed range on measures of marital satisfaction, with the most commonly reported problems being difficulties balancing work and marriage and financial concerns. Optimism can help, but it depends on the nature of one’s optimism. One recent study found that the personality trait of optimism, which involves a general tendency to expect good outcomes, fosters constructive problem solving and marital well-being. However, this study found that relationship-specific optimism, which involves idealistic expectations about marriage (my partner will always be affectionate, always communicate well, never intentionally hurt me, and so on), was associated with less constructive problem solving and steep declines in marital well-being during the first year of marriage. So, it may help to have realistic expectations about marriage.
You might guess that partners who cohabit prior to getting married would have an easier transition and greater marital success. However, until relatively recently, research demonstrated just the opposite. Studies found an association between premarital cohabitation and increased divorce rates. Theorists speculated that people inclined to cohabit were less traditional, more individualistic, and had a weaker commitment to the institution of marriage. However, the findings on the effects of cohabitation have shifted. One reason may be that cohabitation prior to marriage has gradually become the norm rather than the exception. In the 1970s, only about 10 percent of couples lived together before marriage, but that figure has risen to 66 percent. A large-scale study in Australia that looked at trends over decades (from 1945 to 2000) found that cohabitants had higher rates of marital dissolution up through 1988, but then the trend started to gradually reverse itself, with cohabitants showing lower rates of divorce. In the United States, studies focusing on more recent marriages also failed to find cohabitation associated with marital instability.
One major source of conflict in many new marriages is the negotiation of marital roles in relation to career commitments. More and more women are aspiring to demanding careers. However, research shows that husbands’ careers continue to take priority over their wives’ career ambitions. Moreover, many husbands maintain traditional role expectations about housework, child care, and decision making. Men’s contribution to house-work/child care has increased noticeably since the 1960s. But studies of couples with children indicate that wives are still doing about twice as much housework/child care as their husbands. This is true even among highly paid and highly stressed female executives. Miller Burke and Attridge interviewed 106 successful men and women from the world of business who were mostly worth over a million dollars. Among the women, 44 percent reported they did most of the housework/child care, whereas only 4 percent of the men reported shouldering the bulk of housework/child care. All that said, husbands still put in more hours of paid work than wives on average, and most wives do not view their larger share of housework as unfair because most women don’t expect a 50–50 split.
Parenthood
The arrival of the first child represents a major transition (Senior, 2010), and can lead to negative emotions (Held & Rutherford, 2011), and the disruption of old routines can be extremely stressful (Carter, 1999). Dual roles for the mother also increase the level of stress and the tendency to experience marital dissatisfaction. More Canadian women than ever are returning to the workplace after having children (Statistics Canada, 2007d). A review of decades of research on parenthood and marital satisfaction found that (1) parents exhibit lower marital satisfaction than comparable nonparents, (2) mothers of infants report the steepest decline in marital satisfaction, and (3) the more children couples have, the lower their marital satisfaction tends to be (Twenge, Campbell, & Foster, 2003). Consistent with these trends, a recent longitudinal study found that the transition to parenthood was associated with a sudden deterioration in relationship quality (Doss et al., 2009). The decline in marital satisfaction tended to be small to medium in size. Ironically, the more satisfied couples were prior to birth of their first child, the more their marital satisfaction declined.

75
Q

Describe the physical and cognitive changes associated with aging. For each physical and cognitive change, describe how that change affects everyday life.

A

Physiological Changes
Most Canadians remain relatively healthy as they age, but there are physical and psychological changes. With increasing age comes increasing attention to the physical changes associated with age. People obviously experience many physical changes as they progress through adulthood. In both sexes, hair tends to thin out and become grey, and many males confront receding hairlines and baldness. To the dismay of many, the proportion of body fat tends to increase with age, while the amount of muscle tissue decreases. Overall, weight tends to increase in most adults through the mid-50s, when a gradual decline may begin. These changes have little functional significance, but in our youth-oriented society, they often have an impact on self-concept, leading many people to view themselves as unattractive.
In the sensory domain, the key developmental changes occur in vision and hearing. The proportion of people with 20/20 visual acuity declines with age, while farsightedness and difficulty seeing in low illumination become more common. Sensitivity to colour and contrast also decline. Hearing sensitivity begins declining gradually in early adulthood but usually isn’t noticeable until after age 50. Hearing loss tends to be greater in men than in women, and for high-frequency sounds more than low-frequency sounds. Even mild hearing loss can undermine speech perception. Such loss puts an added burden on cognitive processing.
Age-related changes also occur in hormonal functioning during adulthood. Among women, these changes lead to menopause. This ending of menstrual periods, accompanied by a loss of fertility, typically occurs at around age 50. Most women experience at least some unpleasant symptoms (e.g., hot flashes, headaches, night sweats, and mood changes), but the amount of discomfort varies considerably. Menopause is also accompanied by an elevated vulnerability to depression. Not long ago, menopause was thought to be almost universally accompanied by severe emotional strain. However, it is now clear that most women experience relatively modest psychological distress.
Overall, the physiological changes brought on by aging tend to decrease functional capabilities, reduce biological resilience in the face of stress, and increase susceptibility to acute and chronic diseases
Neural Changes
The amount of brain tissue and brain weight decline gradually in late adulthood, mostly after age 60. These trends appear to reflect both a decrease in the number of active neurons in some areas of the brain and shrinkage of still-active neurons, with neuron loss perhaps being less important than once believed. Although this gradual loss of brain tissue sounds alarming, it is a normal part of the aging process. Its functional significance is the subject of some debate, but it doesn’t appear to be a key factor in any of the age-related dementias. Dementia is an abnormal condition marked by multiple cognitive deficits that include memory impairment.
Cognitive Changes
In the cognitive domain, aging seems to take its toll on speed first. Many studies indicate that speed in learning, solving problems, and processing information tends to decline with age. The evidence suggests that the erosion of processing speed may be a gradual, lengthy trend beginning in middle adulthood. Although mental speed declines with age, problem-solving ability remains largely unimpaired if older people are given adequate time to compensate for their reduced speed.

76
Q

Explain how Chapter 11 highlights the theme of the joint influence of heredity and environment.

A

We saw how heredity and environment jointly mold behavior. We’ve encountered the dual influence of heredity and environment before, but this theme is rich in complexity, and each chapter draws out different aspects and implications. Our discussion of development amplified the point that genetics and experience work interactively to shape behavior. In the language of science, an interaction means that the effects of one variable depend on the effects of another. In other words, heredity and environment do not operate independently. Children with “difficult” temperaments will elicit different reactions from different parents, depending on the parents’ personalities and expectations. Likewise, a particular pair of parents will affect children in different ways, depending on the inborn characteristics of the children. An interplay, or feedback loop, exists between biological and environmental factors. For instance, a temperamentally difficult child may elicit negative reactions from parents, which serve to make the child more difficult, which evokes more negative reactions. If this child develops into an ornery 11-year-old, which do we blame—genetics or experience? Clearly, this outcome is due to the reciprocal effects of both.
All aspects of development are shaped jointly by heredity and experience. We often estimate their relative weight or influence, as if we could cleanly divide behavior into genetic and environmental components. Although we can’t really carve up behavior that neatly, such comparisons can be of great theoretical interest, as you’ll see in our upcoming Personal Application, which discusses the nature and origins of gender differences in behavior.

77
Q

Summarize the evidence regarding gender differences in behavior. Discuss the significance of these differences.

A

Cognitive Abilities
In the cognitive domain, it appears that there are three genuine—albeit very small—gender differences. First, on the average, females tend to exhibit slightly better verbal skills than males (Leaper, 2013). For example, girls score higher in reading achievement around the world (Stoet & Geary, 2013). The size of females’ advantage varies depending on the nature of the task, but the gender gaps generally are quite small (Halpern, 2012). Second, starting during high school, males show a slight advantage on tests of mathematical ability. When all students are compared, males’ advantage is quite small. Indeed, it appears that the gender gap in math has disappeared in the general North American population (Hyde, 2014). Around the world, though, small to modest gender disparities are still seen in many countries, and these differences usually favor males (Else-Quest, Hyde, & Linn, 2010; Stoet & Geary, 2013). Also, at the high end of the ability distribution, a gender gap is still found in North America. About three to four times as many males as females manifest exceptional math skills (Wai, Putallaz, & Makel, 2012). Third, starting in the grade-school years, males tend to score higher than females on most measures of visual-spatial ability. Tasks requiring mental rotations in space and perception of movement in space tend to generate the biggest gender disparities.
In regard to social behavior, research findings support the existence of some additional gender differences. First, studies indicate that males tend to be much more physically aggressive than females (Archer, 2005; Card et al., 2008). This disparity shows up early in childhood. Its continuation into adulthood is supported by the fact that men account for a grossly disproportionate number of the violent crimes in our society (Kenrick, Trost, & Sundie, 2004). Contrary to popular belief, females and males seem pretty similar in their level of verbal or relational aggression (snide remarks and so forth), but this is the type of aggression females use most commonly, resulting in the “mean girls” stereotype (Leaper, 2013). Second, there are gender differences in nonverbal communication. The evidence indicates that females are more sensitive than males to subtle nonverbal cues (Hampson, van Anders, & Mullin, 2006; Schmid et al., 2011) and that they pay more attention to interpersonal information (Hall & Mast, 2008). Third, males are more sexually active than females in a variety of ways. For example, males are more likely to engage in casual and premarital sex, masturbation, and the use of pornography
Biological Differences
The gender gap in aggression is also explained in terms of reproductive fitness. Because females are more selective about mating than males, males have to engage in more competition for sexual partners than females do. Greater aggressiveness is thought to be adaptive for males in this competition for sexual access because it should foster social dominance over other males and facilitate the acquisition of the material resources emphasized by females when they evaluate potential partners (Campbell, 2005; Cummins, 2005). Evolutionary theorists assert that gender differences in spatial ability reflect the division of labour in ancestral hunting-and-gathering societies in which males typically handled the hunting and females the gathering. Males’ superiority on most spatial tasks has been attributed to the adaptive demands of hunting (Newcombe, 2007; Silverman & Choi, 2005; see Chapter 1).
Evolutionary analyses of gender differences are interesting, but controversial. On the one hand, it seems eminently plausible that evolutionary forces could have led to some divergence between males and females in typical behavior. On the other hand, evolutionary hypotheses are highly speculative and difficult to test empirically (Eagly & Wood, 1999, 2013). The crucial problem for some critics is that evolutionary analyses are so “flexible” that they can be used to explain almost anything. For example, if the situation regarding spatial ability were reversed—if females scored higher than males—evolutionary theorists might attribute females’ superiority to the adaptive demands of gathering food, weaving baskets, and making clothes—and it would be difficult to prove otherwise.
Differences in Brain Organization
Many theorists believe that gender differences in behavior are rooted in male–female disparities in brain structure and organization. For example, some theorists have tried to link gender differences to the specialization of the cerebral hemispheres in the brain. In most people, the left hemisphere is more actively involved in verbal processing, whereas the right hemisphere is more active in visual–spatial processing. After these findings surfaced, theorists began to wonder whether this division of labor in the brain might be related to gender differences in verbal and spatial skills. Consequently, they began looking for sex-related disparities in brain organization.
Some thought-provoking findings have been reported. For instance, some studies have found that males tend to exhibit more cerebral specialization than females. Other studies suggest that females tend to have a larger corpus callosum, the band of fibers that connects the two hemispheres of the brain. Thus, some theorists have concluded that differences between the genders in brain organization are responsible for gender differences in verbal and spatial ability.

78
Q

Are fathers essential to children’s well-being? What three critical thinking skills could you use to evaluate the argument?

A

Fathers surely are important, and it seems likely that father absence contributes to a variety of social maladies. So, why do Blankenhorn and Popenoe argue for the much stronger conclusion—that fathers are essential? They appear to prefer the stronger conclusion because it raises much more serious questions about the viability of nontraditional family forms. Thus, they seem to want to advance a political agenda that champions traditional family values. They are certainly entitled to do so, but when research findings are used to advance a political agenda—whether conservative or liberal—a special caution alert should go off in your head. When a political agenda is at stake, it pays to scrutinize arguments with extra care, because research findings are more likely to be presented in a slanted fashion.
Skills:
Understanding the limitations of correlational evidence
The critical thinker understands that a correlation between two variables does not demonstrate that there is a causal link between the variables.
Looking for alternative explanations for findings and events
In evaluating explanations, the critical thinker explores whether there are other explanations that could also account for the findings or events under scrutiny.
Recognizing and avoiding common fallacies, such as irrelevant reasons, circular reasoning, slippery slope reasoning, weak analogies, and false dichotomies
The critical thinker is vigilant about conclusions based on unrelated premises, conclusions that are rewordings of premises, unwarranted predictions that things will spin out of control, superficial analogies, and contrived dichotomies.