Theory and methods Flashcards

1
Q

Choosing a research method: Primary data info

A

Primary Data

Primary data is information directly collected by sociologists for specific purposes, such as creating a detailed picture of a group or testing a hypothesis.

Methods of collecting primary data include:

Social surveys: Asking questions through interviews or written questionnaires.

Participant observation: Actively engaging in the activities of the studied group.

Experiments: Though rarely conducted in laboratories, sociologists may use field or comparative experiments

Strengths:

  1. Relevance: Primary data is collected directly for the research purpose, ensuring it is specific and tailored to the study’s objectives.
  2. Accuracy: It provides firsthand information, reducing the risk of distortion or reliance on outdated data.

Weaknesses:

  1. Cost and Time: Collecting primary data, such as through surveys or experiments, can be expensive and time-consuming.
  2. Researcher Bias: The design or interpretation of data collection methods, such as interviews or observations, may be influenced by the researcher’s personal biases.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Choosing a research method: Secondary data

A

Secondary data refers to information collected or created by others for their purposes but used by sociologists in their research.

Sources of Secondary Data

Official Statistics: Compiled by governments and organizations on issues such as education, crime, unemployment, and other social topics.

Documents: Includes items like letters, emails, diaries, photographs, reports, novels, newspapers, websites, and broadcasts.

Strengths of Secondary Data

  1. Cost and Time Efficiency: Secondary data is quick and inexpensive to obtain since it has already been collected and processed by others.
  2. Large-Scale Availability: Provides access to extensive data sets, such as official statistics or historical documents, that would be challenging to gather independently.

Weaknesses of Secondary Data

  1. Relevance Issues: The data may not align perfectly with the sociologist’s research questions or objectives, limiting its usefulness.
  2. Potential Bias: The data may reflect the biases or perspectives of its original creators, reducing its reliability for sociological analysis.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Choosing a research method: Quantitave data

A

Quantitative data is numerical information used to measure and analyze patterns, trends, or relationships, often collected through surveys, experiments, or official statistics.

Strengths:

  1. Reliable and objective: It allows for precise comparisons and patterns using statistical analysis.
  2. Large-scale analysis: Data can be gathered from many participants, providing generalizable findings.

Weaknesses:

  1. Lacks depth: It doesn’t capture detailed insights into emotions, motivations, or experiences.
  2. Potential bias: Data may oversimplify complex social phenomena or be influenced by poorly designed questions.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Choosing a research method: Qualitative data

A

Qualitative data is non-numerical information that explores meanings, experiences, and perspectives, often collected through interviews, observations, or case studies.

What is Qualitative Data?

Qualitative data is non-numerical information that explores social phenomena through meanings, experiences, and perspectives. It is often collected through methods like interviews, observations, and case studies, focusing on depth and detail rather than breadth.

Strengths of Qualitative Data

  1. Rich, Detailed Insights: Provides in-depth understanding of people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, offering a deeper exploration of social contexts.
  2. Flexibility: Allows researchers to adapt questions or methods based on responses, enabling discovery of unexpected findings.

Weaknesses of Qualitative Data

  1. Time-Consuming: Collecting and analyzing qualitative data, such as through interviews or observations, requires significant time and effort.
  2. Subjectivity: Data interpretation can be influenced by the researcher’s personal biases, potentially affecting reliability and validity.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods: Practical issues

A
  1. Time and Money: Research can be costly and time-consuming. Large-scale surveys require significant resources, while small-scale projects, like participant observation, may take years to complete but are cheaper. Access to funding and resources also influences a sociologist’s choices.
  2. Requirements of Funding Bodies: Funders may specify the type of data required, often preferring quantitative results for measurable outcomes. This limits the sociologist’s choice of methods.
  3. Personal Skills and Characteristics: Researchers need specific skills, such as rapport-building for interviews or observation skills for fieldwork. Not all sociologists possess the necessary qualities, which can limit their effectiveness.
  4. Subject Matter: Certain groups or topics may be more accessible through specific methods. For instance, participant observation might be better for studying close-knit groups, while illiterate populations might not suit questionnaires.
  5. Research Opportunity: Unexpected research opportunities may restrict method choice. For example, a sudden chance to study a gang required immediate participant observation, as there was no time to prepare structured methods.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods: Ethical issues

A
  1. Informed Consent
    Participants must willingly agree to take part, knowing all relevant details of the study. Consent should be obtained before starting and, if necessary, throughout the process.
  2. Confidentiality and Privacy
    Researchers must protect participants’ identities and personal information, ensuring their privacy is respected.
  3. Avoiding Harm
    Researchers should anticipate and prevent harm, such as social exclusion, psychological damage, or negative impacts on participants’ lives.
  4. Vulnerable Groups
    Special care is needed when studying vulnerable participants, such as children or those with disabilities. Consent must be obtained in an understandable manner.
  5. Covert Research
    Covert studies, where participants are unaware of the research, raise ethical concerns, as they often involve deception and lack informed consent. However, some argue they are justifiable in specific cases.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods: Theoretical issues

A

Validity: Valid methods produce truthful and accurate representations of reality. Qualitative methods like observations often offer deeper insights compared to quantitative ones like surveys.

Reliability: Reliable methods yield consistent results when repeated. Quantitative methods, such as structured questionnaires, tend to be more reliable than qualitative methods.

Representativeness: Research samples should reflect the wider population to allow generalizations. Large-scale surveys using advanced sampling techniques are more likely to produce representative data.

Methodological Perspectives: Researchers’ methods are influenced by their views on society:

Positivists prefer quantitative data, seeing sociology as a science.

Interpretivists prefer qualitative data to explore meanings and reject the scientific model.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

The Process of research- Formulating an aim or hypothesis

A

Hypothesis

A hypothesis is a testable statement predicting a relationship between variables, often structured as “if X, then Y” (e.g., “family size affects educational achievement”). It is used in research to establish cause-and-effect relationships through evidence collection and analysis.

Advantages:

  1. Provides focus and clarity:
    A hypothesis sets a clear direction for the study by defining specific variables to test. This helps researchers stay focused and ensures the research remains relevant to its goals.
  2. Tests cause-and-effect relationships:
    Hypotheses enable researchers to examine causal links between variables, such as how family size influences achievement, allowing for objective conclusions to be drawn.

Weaknesses:

  1. Restricts exploration:
    A hypothesis confines research to pre-defined ideas, which can limit the discovery of unexpected findings or alternative explanations that may emerge during the study.
  2. Potential perception of failure:
    If the hypothesis is disproven, it may be seen as a setback, even though discarding false hypotheses contributes to progress by ruling out incorrect assumptions.

Aim

An aim is a broad statement outlining the general purpose of the research, often used when exploring unfamiliar topics. It emphasizes gathering data on a phenomenon without necessarily testing a specific relationship (e.g., “exploring the way of life of a subculture”).

Advantages:

  1. Encourages open-ended exploration:
    An aim allows for more flexible research. Instead of being confined to testing one hypothesis, researchers can investigate a range of aspects related to the topic, leading to richer insights.
  2. Ideal for new or under-researched areas:
    When little is known about a subject, having an open-ended aim helps researchers gather foundational data to better understand the topic and identify potential hypotheses for future studies.

Weaknesses:

  1. Lack of specific focus:
    Since an aim is broad, it may result in less efficient data collection. Researchers might explore too many avenues, making it harder to prioritize or analyze the data meaningfully.
  2. Weaker conclusions:
    Without a clear hypothesis to test, the results of aim-driven research may be less conclusive or harder to generalize, as they focus more on description than causation.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- Operationalising concepts

A

Definition of Operationalising Concepts

Operationalising a concept involves defining an abstract sociological concept in a way that makes it measurable. This process is crucial for testing hypotheses and conducting research.

  1. Purpose of Operationalisation

Converts abstract sociological ideas (e.g., “social class”) into measurable variables.

Allows researchers to gather and analyze data systematically.

  1. Example

Hypothesis: Working-class pupils achieve lower qualifications due to lower parental income.

Operationalisation: Define “social class” using parental occupation as an indicator. Questions such as “What is your job?” help categorize pupils’ social class.

  1. Advantages

Makes abstract concepts measurable, enabling testing of hypotheses.

Facilitates comparisons between different groups or studies.

  1. Weaknesses

Subjectivity: Researchers may disagree on how to operationalise a concept (e.g., is a routine office worker “working-class” or “middle-class”?).

Comparability Issues: Differing operational definitions across studies can make it hard to compare findings.

  1. Theoretical Perspectives

Positivists:

Focus heavily on operationalisation to create measurable variables.

Believe in testing hypotheses through data collection and objective analysis.

Interpretivists:

Less concerned with operationalising concepts.

Emphasize understanding the meanings and definitions actors themselves attach to concepts like “class” or “achievement.”

  1. Challenges

Complexity arises when different sociologists operationalise the same concept differently.

Requires careful consideration to ensure reliability and validity in research.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- the pilot study

A

Sociologists using social surveys, such as questionnaires or structured interviews, often conduct a pilot study before undertaking the main survey. This involves testing a draft version of the questionnaire or interview schedule (a list of questions) on a small sample.

Purpose of a Pilot Study:

The primary goal is to identify and resolve any issues, refine or clarify the wording of questions, and allow interviewers to practice.

This ensures that the final survey proceeds smoothly without complications.

Example:
In 1962, Young and Willmott conducted over 100 pilot interviews to determine the design of their study, the types of questions to include, and how to phrase them effectively.

Advantages of a Pilot Study:

  1. Identifies Problems Early:

A pilot study helps researchers detect and correct unclear, ambiguous, or poorly worded questions before the main study.

This ensures data collection is accurate and relevant.

  1. Improves Practicality:

It allows researchers to test logistics, such as timing and the flow of questions, ensuring the final survey runs smoothly and efficiently.

Disadvantages of a Pilot Study:

  1. Time-Consuming:

Conducting a pilot study adds an extra step to the research process, increasing the time needed before the main study can begin.

  1. Limited Generalizability:

The small sample used in the pilot study may not represent the target population, leading to insights that might not apply widely.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- Samples and sampling

A

Samples and Sampling

Definition: Sampling is the process of selecting a smaller group (sample) from a larger population to represent it in a study.

Purpose:

Ensure the sample is representative of the population, allowing findings to be generalized.

Sociologists aim to make broad statements about social structures based on representative samples.

Sampling Frame

Definition: A list of all members in the target population. It is used as a basis for selecting a sample.

Example: Young and Willmott used the electoral register as their sampling frame.

Key Requirements: The frame must be complete, accurate, up-to-date, and free of duplicates to ensure representativeness.

Sampling Techniques

  1. Random Sampling:

Selection by pure chance (e.g., drawing names from a hat).

Ensures equal chance for all members of being selected.

Limitation: May not always represent all characteristics of the population.

  1. Quasi-Random/Systematic Sampling:

Selection of every nth person from the sampling frame (e.g., every 36th name).

  1. Stratified Random Sampling:

Divides the population into groups (e.g., by age, gender, class) and samples in proportion to those groups.

Example: If 20% of the population is under 18, 20% of the sample must also be under 18.

  1. Quota Sampling:

Similar to stratified sampling but involves filling pre-set quotas (e.g., 20 males and 20 females).

Interviewers continue until quotas are filled.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- Non-representative sampling

A

Definition:
Non-representative sampling does not aim to ensure that the participants in the study represent the wider research population.

Strengths of Non-Representative Sampling:

  1. Practicality:

It is often quicker and less costly to conduct, especially when resources are limited.

  1. Focus on Specific Groups:

Allows in-depth study of particular subgroups or unique cases that may not be possible in representative sampling.

Limitations of Non-Representative Sampling:

  1. Lack of Generalizability:

Findings cannot be extended to the wider population.

  1. Potential Bias:

The sample may overrepresent or underrepresent certain characteristics, leading to skewed results.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- Practical reasons for not being able to create a representative sample

A

Practical Reasons
Creating a representative sample can be challenging due to:

  1. Unknown social characteristics (e.g., age, gender, class) of the research population.
  2. Lack of a complete sampling frame, such as for unconvicted criminals.
  3. Refusal of potential respondents to participate, e.g., criminals fearing exposure.

When representation is unattainable, researchers may use:

Snowball Sampling: A chain of referrals to access hard-to-reach groups.

Opportunity Sampling: Selecting easily accessible individuals, though not always representative.

Theoretical Reasons
Some researchers, particularly interpretivists, prioritize understanding social actors’ meanings over creating representative samples. They aim for valid, in-depth data rather than generalizations.

Once the sample is chosen, data collection on the topic can begin.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Factors influencing choice of methods- Theoretical reasons for not being able to create a representive sample

A

Theoretical Reasons
Some researchers, particularly interpretivists, prioritize understanding social actors’ meanings over creating representative samples. They aim for valid, in-depth data rather than generalizations.

Once the sample is chosen, data collection on the topic can begin.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- researching pupils

A

Power and Status (Shortened)

Children and young people often have less power than adults, making it difficult for them to express their views, especially if they contradict adults. Schools amplify this dynamic due to their hierarchical nature, with teachers holding authority over pupils. This power imbalance can influence research, such as which pupils are selected to participate.

Formal methods like structured interviews or questionnaires may emphasize this power disparity since researchers control the questions and answers. Sociologists must find ways to reduce these differences, for example, by using group interviews rather than one-on-one settings. However, some power imbalances are unavoidable.

Ability and Understanding (Shortened)

Pupils typically have less developed vocabulary, self-expression, and confidence compared to adults, making it harder for them to grasp abstract ideas or articulate responses. Researchers must carefully phrase questions to ensure understanding. Limitations in understanding also complicate gaining pupils’ informed consent.

Younger children often need more time to process questions, and their memory may not recall relevant details as effectively as adults. Additionally, pupils’ differences in age, gender, class, and ethnicity impact their responses, necessitating that researchers adapt their approach to suit participants’ backgrounds.

Vulnerability and Ethical Issues (Shortened)

Due to their limited power and ability, young people are more vulnerable to harm than adults, raising ethical concerns for researchers. Sociologists must ensure participation is necessary and beneficial while obtaining informed consent from both the pupil and their guardian.

Child protection laws like the Safeguarding Vulnerable Groups Act (2006) often require additional vetting, delaying research. Gatekeepers, such as teachers and parents, also control access to pupils, adding complexity. Organizations like UNICEF and the British Sociological Association have created ethical guidelines to protect young participants in research.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- researching teachers

A
  1. Researching Teachers

Power and Status
Teachers hold more power and status within schools due to their age, experience, and legal responsibilities. Classrooms are viewed as their domain, where researchers might be seen as intruders. Teachers’ actions are also influenced by other stakeholders like heads, governors, parents, and students.

Researchers may adopt covert methods (e.g., posing as assistants) to gain access, though this can create issues as these roles are less respected, and other teachers may not treat them equally.

Impression Management
Teachers, accustomed to scrutiny (e.g., Ofsted inspections), are skilled at shaping others’ perceptions—what Erving Goffman called “impression management.” Researchers must uncover the “backstage” behaviors hidden behind this public image, often observed in private spaces like staffrooms.

Challenges
Staffrooms are tight-knit spaces, and newcomers may arouse suspicion. Teachers may avoid criticism of their schools to protect their careers, limiting honesty in interviews. Observational methods may be more effective.

Heads may select teachers to participate, skewing findings to promote a favorable school image—another form of impression management.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- researching classrooms

A

Researching Classrooms

Classroom Dynamics: Classrooms are closed, controlled spaces with strict boundaries, like time, dress, and language. This control may conceal students’ and teachers’ true thoughts or feelings, making behaviors staged or strategic.

Gatekeepers: Access to classrooms is mediated by gatekeepers like headteachers and child protection laws. The more gatekeepers, the harder it is for researchers to gain access.

Peer Groups: Students are influenced by peer pressure, which affects how they answer questions or participate in group settings. Researchers may need to supervise or anonymize responses to avoid biased results.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- researching schools

A

Researching Schools

Researching schools in the UK presents challenges due to the vast number of institutions. Observational methods are often unrepresentative, while large-scale surveys or official statistics may lack the detailed insights gained from studying individual schools. Researchers can easily identify school populations using state-published lists, which provide details like location and school type.

Schools’ Own Data

Schools generate extensive secondary data, including exam results, league tables, truancy rates, Ofsted reports, and policy documents. While these are valuable for researchers, issues like confidentiality may restrict access. Additionally, schools may falsify attendance figures or downplay issues like racism to maintain a positive image. Exam performance statistics may also be manipulated through curriculum adjustments to show false improvement.

The Law

Schools operate within a legal framework requiring them to track attendance and achievement. This data is useful to researchers but may be restricted due to schools’ duty to protect pupils.

Gatekeepers

Headteachers and governors act as gatekeepers, controlling access to schools. They may refuse research that they believe could disrupt school operations or undermine staff authority.

School Organisation

Schools are hierarchical organisations where researchers may be viewed as part of the authority. Students might see them as teachers, while staff may view them as inspectors. In schools with conflicts between students and teachers, researchers might even be seen as adversaries.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- researching parents

A

Researching Parents

Parents play a crucial role in education by influencing their children’s upbringing, engaging in school activities like parent-teacher meetings, and making choices influenced by marketisation policies. However, studying parents presents challenges due to their diversity in class, gender, and ethnicity, which affects their willingness and ability to participate in research.

Middle-class parents are more likely than working-class parents to respond to surveys about their children’s education, leading to unrepresentative findings. Parental consent is often required for research, but the likelihood of obtaining it depends on how sensitive the topic is and whether parents see benefits for their children.

Parents may also engage in “impression management,” exaggerating their involvement in their child’s education, such as claiming they attend events or read to their children more than they actually do, which can lead to invalid data.

Accessing parents is difficult because much of their interaction with their children occurs at home. Schools may assist researchers by sending letters or questionnaires to parents, but responses are not guaranteed, and data accuracy remains an issue.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Education: Research Characteristics- The researcher’s own experience of education.

A

The Researcher’s Own Experience of Education

Everyone, including researchers, has experienced education, which can shape their hypotheses and data interpretation.

However, familiarity with schools may lead sociologists to overlook how unique these environments are. Having spent years in education, they might unconsciously view schools as “natural” and must remain aware of their assumptions.

Successful researchers may struggle to relate to underachieving students or anti-school subcultures. Differences in class, gender, or ethnicity can also hinder research.

Education is politically charged, with conflicting views from political and pressure groups. Researchers must consider how their work fits into these wider debates.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Experiments- Laboratory experiments

A

In natural sciences, laboratory experiments are used to identify cause-and-effect relationships. For instance, physicists discovered that increasing a gas’s temperature causes it to expand.

To illustrate, suppose we study what influences plant growth. Identical plants are divided into:

Experimental group: Given varying nutrients, with changes in size measured.

Control group: Given constant nutrients, with size also recorded.

If the experimental group grows faster, it suggests a cause-and-effect relationship: nutrients cause growth. Here, nutrients are the independent variable (cause), and growth is the dependent variable (effect).

By altering variables and observing outcomes, this method helps predict results under specific conditions, such as how plants respond to a set nutrient level.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Experiments- Laboratory experiments: practical problems

A

Practical Problems with Laboratory Experiments

Studying society is complex due to the vast number of influencing variables. Identifying and controlling all factors, such as those affecting a child’s education or a worker’s attitude, is nearly impossible.

Laboratory experiments face further limitations:

They cannot study the past, as variables from earlier times cannot be controlled or replicated.

Typically, they involve small-scale samples, making it difficult to investigate large social phenomena like religion or voting patterns. This small-scale nature reduces their representativeness.

23
Q

Experiments- Laboratory experiments: Ethical problems

A

Here’s a more concise version:

Ethical Problems

Conducting experiments on humans raises ethical concerns, particularly regarding lack of informed consent, deception, and potential harm to participants.

Lack of Informed Consent

Researchers must obtain participants’ informed consent. However, this can be challenging for groups like children or individuals with learning difficulties, who may not fully understand the experiment’s purpose.

Deception

Deceiving participants is often unethical. In Milgram’s 1974 obedience study, participants were misled into believing they were administering electric shocks as part of a learning experiment. The actual aim was to test obedience to authority. Although no shocks were delivered, 65% believed they had administered the maximum voltage of 450 volts.

Harm

Milgram’s study also caused distress, with participants displaying physical signs of stress, such as trembling, sweating, and even seizures. Three participants experienced full-blown seizures.

Defense of Milgram’s Experiments

Supporters argue Milgram’s work was ethically justified because it highlighted the dangers of blindly obeying authority. Furthermore, 74% of participants later said they had gained valuable insights from the experience.

24
Q

Experiments- Laboratory experiments: The hawthorne effect

A

The Hawthorne Effect

Laboratories are artificial environments, so behavior observed may be unnatural and invalid. When people know they are being studied, they often alter their behavior to match perceived expectations, undermining the study. This is known as the Hawthorne Effect.

In 1927, Elton Mayo studied worker productivity at the Hawthorne plant. Five female volunteers, aware of the study, were subjected to changes in conditions like lighting and rest breaks. Surprisingly, productivity increased even when conditions worsened. Mayo concluded that the workers’ performance improved simply because they knew they were being observed and wanted to please the experimenter.

25
Q

Experiments- Laboratory experiments free will

A

Free Will

Interpretivist sociologists argue that humans are fundamentally different from plants, rocks, and other phenomena studied by natural scientists. Unlike these objects, humans have free will, consciousness, and the ability to make choices.

This means human behavior cannot be explained by simple cause and effect. Instead, it can only be understood through the choices individuals freely make. From this perspective, the experimental method, focused on finding causes, is unsuitable for studying human behavior.

Given these limitations, sociologists often use alternative approaches like field experiments and comparative methods, such as “thought experiments.”

26
Q

Experiments- Field experiments

A

Field Experiments

Field experiments differ from lab experiments in two key ways:

  1. They occur in natural surroundings, not an artificial lab.
  2. Subjects are usually unaware they are part of an experiment, avoiding the Hawthorne Effect.

Researchers manipulate variables to observe their effects. For instance, in Rosenhan’s (1973) pseudopatient study, researchers feigned hearing voices to gain admission to 12 mental hospitals. Once admitted, they behaved normally, yet staff still treated them as mentally ill. This revealed that staff responses were influenced by the “schizophrenic” label rather than the patients’ behavior.

Evaluation
Rosenhan’s study highlights the realism and validity of field experiments, which avoid artificiality. However, their reduced control over variables makes it harder to establish clear cause-and-effect relationships. Critics also find them unethical, as participants are often unaware they are being studied.

27
Q

Experiments- The comparative method

A

The Comparative Method

The comparative method is a thought experiment used by sociologists to explore cause-and-effect relationships without involving real people. It works as follows:

  1. Identify two groups of people that are similar in every way except for the one variable being studied.
  2. Compare the two groups to determine if the variable has any effect.

This method allows researchers to analyze differences without conducting actual experiments, offering insights while avoiding practical and ethical challenges.

28
Q

Experiments-The comparative method (Durkheim’s study of suicide)

A

Durkheim’s Study of Suicide

Emile Durkheim’s 1897 study is a classic example of the comparative method. He hypothesized that low levels of social integration lead to higher suicide rates. He argued that Catholicism fosters stronger social integration than Protestantism and predicted that Protestants would have higher suicide rates than Catholics.

To test this, Durkheim compared suicide rates of Catholics and Protestants who were alike in other factors, such as location and marital status. Official statistics supported his prediction, showing lower suicide rates among Catholics.

Evaluation:
The comparative method avoids artificiality, allows the study of past events, and poses no ethical concerns, such as harming subjects. However, it provides less control over variables than field experiments, making it harder to confirm cause-and-effect relationships.

29
Q

Methods in context- using experiments to investigate education ( lab experiements and teacher expectations)

A

Laboratory Experiments and Teacher Expectations

Harvey and Slatin (1976) showed 96 teachers photos of children from different social classes, genders, and ethnicities. Teachers rated lower-class children less favorably, revealing biases based on prior experiences.

Charkin et al. (1975) found that teachers with high expectations for a student used more positive body language, while those with low expectations were less encouraging. Mason (1973) showed that negative teacher expectations had a greater impact than positive ones on predicting student success.

Ethical Issues
Experiments with real students, like Charkin et al.’s, raise ethical concerns about deception and psychological harm. Studies avoiding real students, such as Harvey and Slatin’s, mitigate these risks.

Narrow Focus
Laboratory experiments isolate specific factors like body language but fail to address broader effects like labeling or self-fulfilling prophecies.

Practical Problems
Conducting experiments on teacher expectations in schools is challenging. Schools are large, complex institutions with many variables, such as class size, streaming, and school type, all of which influence teacher expectations. It is often impossible to identify, let alone control, all these factors.

Artificiality
Laboratory experiments may fail to reflect the realities of education. For instance, Charkin used university students instead of teachers, and Harvey and Slatin used photographs instead of real pupils. Such methods lack authenticity, as university students may not behave like experienced teachers, and teachers’ expectations consider factors beyond appearance, such as behavior, accents, and parental impressions.

30
Q

Methods in context- using experiments to investigate education (Field experiments and teacher expectations)

A

Laboratory Experiments: Limitations

Laboratory experiments often fail to reflect real-world education:

Charkin used university students, not teachers.

Harvey and Slatin used pupil photos instead of real students.

Teachers’ expectations depend on more than appearance, including behavior, accents, and parental impressions.

Field Experiments: Teacher Expectations

Field experiments, like Rosenthal and Jacobson’s (1968) Pygmalion in the Classroom, address these limits but have challenges.

The Study

At “Oak School,” teachers were told an IQ test identified 20% of students as “spurters” (selected randomly).

Aims:

  1. Influence teachers’ expectations.
  2. See if expectations affected performance.

“Teacher expectations” was the independent variable.

Findings

After 8 months, all pupils improved by 8 IQ points, but “spurters” gained 12.

Younger children (6–8) showed the most improvement.

After a year, the effect persisted only in 10–11-year-olds.

Here’s a shorter version:

Ethical Problems
Field experiments in education pose ethical issues. In the Oak School study, ‘spurters’ benefited, but others may have been disadvantaged due to reduced attention. Today, such experiments are unlikely due to children’s rights and schools’ legal duties. Rosenthal and Jacobson also deceived teachers, which was necessary to manipulate expectations but ethically questionable.

Reliability
The study’s design was simple and repeated over 242 times. However, differences in teaching styles and student demographics make exact replication difficult.

Validity
Rosenthal and Jacobson claimed teacher expectations influenced pupils through interactions, but their study didn’t observe classrooms, leaving no evidence for this. Later studies, like Claiborn (1969), also found no support for their claims.

Broader Focus
The study examined the full labeling process and its effects on pupils, using a longitudinal approach to identify trends over time.

31
Q

Advatages of questionaires

A

Advantages of Questionnaires

  1. Practical Advantages
    Questionnaires are quick, affordable, and efficient for collecting large-scale data from geographically diverse groups. For example, Connor and Dewson (2001) distributed 4,000 questionnaires to examine working-class students’ university decisions.

No need for interviewers; respondents complete and return questionnaires independently.

Data is easily quantifiable, especially with closed-ended questions, allowing for rapid analysis.

  1. Reliability
    Questionnaires are reliable as they can be repeated with consistent results.

Identical questionnaires ensure new respondents answer the same questions.

Postal or online methods eliminate researcher influence, ensuring unbiased responses.
Standardized questions also allow comparisons across time and societies.

  1. Hypothesis Testing
    Questionnaires are useful for identifying cause-and-effect relationships. For example, they can show links between family size and children’s achievement levels. Sociologists use them to analyze causes and predict outcomes scientifically.
  2. Detachment and Objectivity
    Questionnaires are unbiased and minimize researcher involvement. Postal methods, for instance, allow for a detached approach, which positivists favor for maintaining objectivity.
  1. Representativeness

Questionnaires allow researchers to collect data from a large number of people, increasing the likelihood of results being representative of the wider population. This is more effective than methods focusing on smaller groups.
Researchers using questionnaires often prioritize obtaining a representative sample, making the findings more generalizable to the population from which the sample was drawn.

  1. Ethical Issues

Questionnaires pose fewer ethical challenges compared to other research methods. While they may include intrusive or sensitive questions, respondents are not obligated to answer.
Researchers should still ensure informed consent, guarantee anonymity, and clarify that participants have the right to skip any questions they choose not to answer.

32
Q

Disadvatages of questionnaires

A

Sure! Here is a concise rewrite of the disadvantages of questionnaires:

Disadvantages of Questionnaires

  1. Practical Problems

Data is often limited and superficial as questionnaires must be brief to ensure responses. Long questionnaires can discourage completion.

While cost-effective, incentives (e.g., prize draws) may be needed to boost participation, increasing expenses.

Issues with postal and online questionnaires include uncertainty about whether recipients received them or if the correct person completed them.

  1. Low Response Rate

Low return rates, especially with postal surveys, can undermine the reliability of findings. For instance, Shere Hite’s 1991 study had a response rate of just 4.5% out of 100,000 questionnaires.

Follow-ups and manual collection may increase responses but add time and costs.

  1. Inflexibility

Once designed, questionnaires cannot adapt to explore unexpected findings. Unlike interviews, they lack flexibility to probe new areas of interest.

  1. Snapshots

Questionnaires provide a static view of social reality, capturing opinions or behaviors at one specific moment in time.

Disadvantages of Questionnaires

  1. Practical Problems
    Data gathered through questionnaires is often superficial because they must be concise to encourage responses. This limits the depth of information. Incentives to increase participation, such as prize draws, raise costs. Additionally, postal and online questionnaires face issues of verifying:

If the recipient received the questionnaire.

Whether the correct person completed it.

  1. Low Response Rates
    Questionnaires often yield low response rates, especially in postal surveys. For example, Shere Hite (1991) received only 4.5% of 100,000 questionnaires returned. Efforts to increase responses, like follow-ups, add time and expense. Faulty design can also deter responses.
  2. Inflexibility
    Once distributed, questionnaires are fixed, limiting exploration of new topics. This contrasts with flexible methods like unstructured interviews, where questions can evolve.
  3. Snapshots of Social Reality
    Questionnaires capture responses at a single moment, missing the evolving nature of attitudes and behaviors. This contrasts with methods like participant observation, which tracks changes over time.
  4. Detachment
    Questionnaires lack personal contact between researcher and respondent, making it hard to clarify meanings or handle misunderstandings. Both parties may interpret questions and answers differently, reducing validity.
  5. Lying, Forgetting, and “Right Answerism”

Data collection relying on respondents’ willingness and ability to provide truthful answers often faces validity issues. Respondents may lie, forget, misunderstand, or give answers they believe are expected or socially acceptable, rather than truthful ones.

  1. Imposing the Researcher’s Meanings

Questionnaires can distort respondents’ realities by reflecting the researcher’s priorities. Closed questions limit respondents to predefined answers, excluding their true perspectives. Open-ended questions allow more freedom, but researchers often simplify responses into categories, losing nuances.

As Marten Shipman (1997) argues, categorizing inevitably alters the data, imposing the researcher’s meanings and reducing validity.

33
Q

Methods in context: using questionnaires to investigate education

A

Operationalisation of Concepts:

Operationalising concepts means converting abstract ideas into measurable forms, especially challenging for young respondents like pupils. Simplifying terms like “cultural capital” may lead to either misunderstanding or loss of sociological relevance.

Samples and Sampling Frames:

Schools provide sampling frames such as lists of pupils, staff, and parents, allowing sociologists to select representative samples. However, these lists may not always reflect research needs, such as sorting by ethnicity, and schools might restrict access due to confidentiality.

Access and Distribution:

Distributing questionnaires in schools can access large respondent groups, but permission is required. Younger pupils are influenced by peers, and questionnaires may feel formal and intimidating.

Response Rates:

Schools often have low response rates due to lesson disruptions or sensitive topics like underage sexual activity. However, rates can improve with school support, as pupils and teachers may feel obliged to participate.

Practical Issues

Questionnaires are cost-effective and efficient for collecting large-scale data, as demonstrated by Rutter’s 1979 study, which gathered information from 12 inner London schools. He correlated variables like school size, attendance, and behavior. Such large-scale data would be difficult to obtain through methods like interviews or observation.

However, the data from questionnaires can be superficial, identifying correlations without explaining them. For instance, while class size might correlate with achievement, questionnaires often fail to capture the reasons behind these patterns.

Practical challenges also arise with certain groups. Young children or those with learning difficulties may struggle with written formats, making questionnaires unsuitable for them. Additionally, those who cannot read well or have limited understanding may provide incomplete or invalid responses, further limiting the data’s usefulness.

Anonymity and Detachment

Questionnaires are effective for sensitive topics like bullying, as anonymity encourages honesty and reduces fear of embarrassment or retribution. This can lead to higher response rates and more valid data compared to face-to-face methods.

However, the lack of personal contact may make it harder to reassure participants about anonymity, reducing trust and full responses. Interpretivists argue that the lack of rapport in questionnaires limits honest engagement, particularly among young people.

The formal nature of questionnaires may also discourage cooperation, especially among pupils who view them as linked to school authority. In contrast, anonymity allows teachers to give more honest answers about sensitive issues without fear of professional repercussions.

34
Q

Types of interviews

A

Types of Interviews

Sociologists use various interview types, ranging from structured to unstructured:

Structured/Formal Interviews: Conducted like questionnaires, with strict instructions to ask each question in the same order and tone, ensuring consistency.

Unstructured/Informal Interviews: These are conversational, allowing interviewers freedom to explore topics, vary questions, and ask follow-ups for deeper insights.

Semi-Structured Interviews: A mix of structured and unstructured. While core questions remain the same, interviewers can probe further, e.g., Cicourel and Kitsuse (1963) used follow-up questions like “How do you mean?” to gather more information.

Group Interviews: Involve multiple participants, such as Paul Willis’s (1977) study on “lads.” Focus groups, a subset, encourage group discussions on specific topics, offering collective insights.

35
Q

Structured interviews

A

Structured Interviews: Detailed Overview

Definition
Structured interviews use a pre-set list of standardized, closed-ended questions. The interviewer reads the questions and records responses, ensuring uniformity across participants but maintaining a degree of interaction.

  1. Practical Issues

Training:

Training interviewers is straightforward and inexpensive, as they only need to follow a set script.

However, this is still costlier and more time-consuming than distributing questionnaires, especially for large populations.

Coverage:

Structured interviews can efficiently cover large numbers of people, particularly for straightforward, factual data (e.g., age, job).

They are relatively quick and cost-effective but cannot match the reach of postal or online surveys.

Quantifiable Data:

The use of closed-ended questions with pre-coded answers makes results easy to analyze and suitable for hypothesis testing.

However, they are less effective for exploring complex or nuanced issues.

  1. Response Rate

Advantages:

Structured interviews generally achieve higher response rates than questionnaires because people are less likely to refuse a face-to-face interaction.

Respondents may feel more comfortable participating, especially when approached in a professional manner.

Improvement Strategies:

Response rates can be boosted through follow-ups, such as callbacks to participants who initially decline.

Higher response rates lead to more representative data and stronger generalizations.

Challenges:

Those who agree to interviews may not represent the general population (e.g., they may have more time or interest), creating potential bias.

  1. Reliability

Consistency and Standardization:

Structured interviews are highly reliable, as each participant is asked identical questions in the same tone and order.

This ensures that other researchers can replicate the study and achieve comparable results.

Comparability:

Uniformity makes it easy to identify patterns, similarities, and differences across responses.

Replicability:

Structured interviews act like a “recipe” for research. Following the set procedure ensures the same outcome regardless of who conducts the interviews.

  1. Validity

Criticism:

Critics argue that structured interviews often fail to capture the full complexity of participants’ experiences.

Closed-ended questions force participants to choose from limited options, which may not fully reflect their true views.

Limitations:

There is little room for interviewers to probe deeper or clarify misunderstandings.

Feminist Criticisms (Hilary Graham, 1983)

Hilary Graham critiques survey methods, including structured interviews, for reinforcing patriarchal biases. Her key points are:

  1. Control by Researcher: The researcher controls the interview process, deciding questions and direction, reflecting women’s subordination in society.
  2. Isolation of Women: Women are treated as isolated individuals rather than in the context of oppressive power relationships.
  3. Imposed Categories: Researchers’ categories limit women’s ability to express their experiences of oppression, concealing unequal power dynamics.

Graham suggests methods that uncover women’s behaviors, attitudes, and meanings, advocating for direct observation over structured interviews. Other feminists prefer unstructured interviews, emphasizing trust, empathy, and collaboration.

Evaluation of Inflexibility in Structured Interviews

Structured interviews, while offering standardization and reliability, are criticized for their inflexibility. This rigidity impacts data collection in the following ways:

  1. Pre-determined Questions: The fixed nature of questions prevents the researcher from exploring unexpected or emerging insights during the interview. This can result in a loss of valuable information.
  2. Limited Respondent Input: Interviewees are restricted to pre-set answers, which may not fully capture their thoughts or experiences, leading to a lack of depth and validity in the findings.
  3. Lack of Adaptability: Structured interviews fail to address respondents’ individual priorities and concerns. By sticking rigidly to the interview schedule, researchers may overlook important topics.
  4. Static Snapshots: These interviews provide a one-time, static view of social phenomena, failing to capture the dynamic and evolving nature of social relationships and behaviors.

Conclusion

While structured interviews are useful for hypothesis testing and ensuring comparability, their inflexibility can undermine the richness and authenticity of the data, especially when exploring complex social realities.

36
Q

Unstructured interviews

A
  1. Rapport and Sensitivity
    Unstructured interviews allow researchers to build rapport and trust with interviewees, encouraging honest and open responses. This is especially beneficial when exploring sensitive topics, as respondents feel more comfortable sharing their experiences.
  2. The Interviewee’s View
    These interviews focus on the interviewee’s perspective rather than imposing a rigid framework. They enable respondents to express their thoughts freely, leading to more authentic and detailed insights.
  3. Checking Understanding
    Researchers can clarify and probe further if an answer is unclear, ensuring a better understanding of the interviewee’s meaning. This interactive process minimizes misinterpretation and improves the accuracy of the data collected.
  4. Flexibility
    Unstructured interviews allow the interviewer to adapt questions and delve deeper into interesting or unexpected responses. This flexibility makes them particularly useful for exploring complex or nuanced topics.
  5. Exploring Unfamiliar Topics
    When entering new or poorly understood areas of research, unstructured interviews are invaluable. They provide the freedom to uncover unknown issues, themes, and patterns that might not emerge in structured formats.

Disadvantages of Unstructured Interviews

  1. Practical Problems

Time-Consuming: Conducting unstructured interviews requires significant time for both interviewing and analyzing the data.

Expensive: They are resource-intensive, requiring skilled interviewers and lengthy transcription processes.

Smaller Sample Sizes: Due to the time and cost involved, fewer people can be interviewed, limiting the scope of the study.

  1. Representativeness

Unstructured interviews typically involve smaller, non-random samples, making it difficult to generalize findings to the wider population. This undermines the representativeness of the data collected.

  1. Reliability

The lack of a standardized structure means different interviewers might ask questions in varying ways or interpret responses differently. This makes it difficult for other researchers to replicate the study and achieve the same results.

  1. Quantification

The open-ended nature of unstructured interviews makes it challenging to quantify responses. Analyzing qualitative data is subjective and can lack the precision of numerical analysis.

  1. Validity

While unstructured interviews often uncover rich insights, they may unintentionally lead interviewees, resulting in biased or inaccurate data. Furthermore, the subjective nature of interpretation can affect the validity of findings.

37
Q

The interview as a social interaction

A

The Interview as a Social Interaction

  1. Interview Bias

Personal beliefs or behaviors of the interviewer/interviewee can skew results. Leading questions or favoritism may affect accuracy.

  1. Artificiality

The formal setting may cause unnatural behaviors, limiting real-world applicability.

  1. Status and Power Inequalities

Power dynamics, such as an interviewer’s authority, can intimidate interviewees, leading to biased or dishonest responses.

  1. Cultural Differences

Cultural norms affect communication. Misunderstandings may occur if cultural contexts differ, impacting data interpretation.

  1. Social Desirability Effect

Interviewees may give socially acceptable answers rather than truthful ones, reducing validity.

  1. Ethical Issues

Issues like consent, confidentiality, and respect for participants’ rights must be managed to maintain integrity and trust.

38
Q

Improving validity of interviews

A

Improving the Validity of Interviews

To enhance the validity of interviews, several strategies can be employed:

  1. Careful Question Design

Use clear, neutral, and open-ended questions to avoid leading responses.

Pilot-test questions to identify and correct ambiguities.

  1. Standardization

Use a consistent format across interviews to reduce variability caused by the interviewer’s style.

Develop an interview guide to ensure uniformity.

  1. Training Interviewers

Train interviewers to avoid bias, manage power dynamics, and build rapport.

Teach active listening and cultural sensitivity.

  1. Mitigating Social Desirability Bias

Ensure anonymity and confidentiality to encourage honest responses.

Frame sensitive questions non-judgmentally.

  1. Cultural Awareness

Consider cultural norms and communication styles when designing and conducting interviews.

Use culturally appropriate language and interpreters if needed.

  1. Establishing Rapport

Build trust to make interviewees feel comfortable and more open.

Start with non-threatening, easy questions to ease into the discussion.

  1. Triangulation

Combine interview data with other methods (e.g., surveys or observations) to cross-check findings.

  1. Ethical Practices

Obtain informed consent and respect participants’ rights.

Create a safe, respectful environment to encourage truthful participation.

39
Q

Methods in context: using interviews to investigate education

A

Methods in Context: Using Interviews to Investigate Education

  1. Practical Issues

Time and Resources: Conducting interviews, especially with students, can be time-consuming and resource-intensive, particularly in large schools.

Age and Understanding: Younger pupils may struggle to comprehend or articulate answers, requiring simplified language and patience.

School Timetables: Interviews must fit into rigid school schedules, limiting available time.

  1. Reliability and Validity

Reliability: Structured interviews can improve reliability by standardizing questions, but variations in interviewer tone or rapport can introduce inconsistency.

Validity: Pupils may give socially desirable answers to please the interviewer, reducing authenticity. Ensuring a comfortable setting and rapport is critical for honest responses.

  1. Access and Response Rate

Access: Researchers need permission from schools, parents, and ethical boards to interview pupils, which can delay research.

Response Rate: Pupils may be absent or reluctant to participate, and parental consent may limit the sample size.

  1. The Interviewer as a ‘Teacher in Disguise’

Pupils may perceive the interviewer as a figure of authority, similar to a teacher, which can lead to:

Fear of saying the “wrong” thing.

Attempts to impress or provide “correct” answers.

Reduced openness about sensitive topics.

  1. Improving Validity of Interviews with Pupils

Building Trust: Establish a non-authoritarian relationship to make pupils feel at ease.

Clear Communication: Use age-appropriate language and avoid jargon or complex questions.

Confidentiality: Assure pupils that their answers are private and won’t be shared with teachers or parents.

Preliminary Questions: Begin with light, non-threatening topics to build comfort.

Active Listening: Show empathy and interest in pupils’ responses to encourage openness.

  1. Group Interviews with Pupils

Advantages:

Encourages peer interaction, helping shy pupils speak up.

Can reveal group dynamics and collective attitudes.

Easier to cover more participants in less time.

Challenges:

Dominant pupils may overshadow others.

Peer pressure can influence responses, reducing validity.

Improving Group Interview Validity:

Use skilled moderation to ensure balanced participation.

Create a supportive atmosphere to minimize peer pressure.

Use smaller groups to allow all pupils to contribute.

40
Q

Participants observation- Types of observation

A

Types of Observation (Shortened)

  1. Non-Participant Observation
    The researcher observes without direct involvement, e.g., using a two-way mirror to watch children play.
  2. Participant Observation
    The researcher actively engages in the group’s activities while observing.
  3. Overt Observation
    The researcher’s identity and purpose are openly shared with participants.
  4. Covert Observation
    The researcher conceals their identity and role, posing as a group member.

Example: William Whyte’s Street Corner Society combined overt and covert methods, revealing his identity to one member but not others.

41
Q

Participants observation- Conducting a participant observation study (getting in)

A

Conducting a Participant Observation Study: Getting In

  1. Acceptance

Gaining acceptance from the group is essential to conduct the study effectively.

Researchers may need to build trust and rapport with members over time to be seen as part of the group.

Challenges: Suspicion, resistance, or rejection if the group perceives the researcher as an outsider or threat.

  1. Making Contact

Initial contact often determines the researcher’s success in joining the group.

Contacts within the group (e.g., a gatekeeper) can facilitate entry.

Strategies:

Approach members through informal conversations or shared interests.

Use existing networks or introductions to gain credibility.

  1. The Observer’s Role

Researchers must adopt a role that allows them to observe without disrupting the group’s dynamics.

Types of Roles:

Peripheral roles involve minimal interaction to reduce influence on the group.

Active roles involve full participation but risk “going native” (losing objectivity).

Balancing participation and detachment is key to maintaining neutrality and ensuring data validity.

By addressing these aspects, researchers can successfully integrate into the group and conduct meaningful observations.

41
Q

Participants observation- Conducting a participant observation study (staying in)

A

Staying In: ‘Going Native’

Definition: “Going native” occurs when a researcher becomes too involved in the group they are studying, losing their objectivity and adopting the group’s values, norms, and perspective.

Causes:

Spending prolonged time with the group.

Developing close personal relationships or emotional attachments with members.

Feeling pressure to conform to group expectations to maintain acceptance.

Impact:

Data may become biased as the researcher’s role shifts from observer to participant.

Critical analysis and detachment are compromised, undermining the study’s validity.

Avoidance Strategies:

Maintain professional boundaries and regularly reflect on one’s role.

Take detailed, objective field notes.

Use triangulation with other methods to verify findings.

Regularly consult with external peers or advisors for perspective.

Staying aware of this risk ensures the balance between participation and observation is maintained.

42
Q

Participants observation- Conducting a participant observation study (getting out)

A

Getting Out: Exiting a Participant Observation Study

Challenges of Leaving:

Leaving the group can be emotionally difficult if strong relationships have been formed.

Members may feel betrayed or abandoned, particularly in covert studies where the true purpose of the research is revealed.

Strategies for Exiting:

  1. Gradual Withdrawal: Slowly reduce participation to avoid suspicion or harm to group dynamics.
  2. Debriefing: If ethical, inform the group about the study’s purpose and offer to share findings.
  3. Maintaining Professionalism: Avoid personal conflicts during departure and ensure relationships end respectfully.

Impact on Data:

Emotional attachments or ethical dilemmas during exit can bias the final analysis.

Reflections on the exit process may provide additional insights into the group’s dynamics.

Planning the exit carefully ensures ethical considerations are met and the research remains valid.

43
Q

Participants observation- Conducting a participant observation study ( overt observation)

A

Conducting a Participant Observation Study: Overt Observation

  1. Definition

In overt observation, the researcher reveals their true identity and purpose to the group being studied. Participants are aware they are being observed.

  1. Advantages

Ethical Transparency:

The researcher operates openly, avoiding deception, and obtains informed consent.

Ease of Note-Taking:

Researchers can openly record observations without secrecy, leading to more accurate data.

Access to Insights:

Members may provide explanations of behaviors or share perspectives directly with the researcher.

  1. Challenges

Hawthorne Effect:

Participants may alter their behavior because they know they are being observed, reducing authenticity.

Trust Issues:

Group members may initially be suspicious of the researcher’s presence or purpose.

Selective Access:

The group may restrict the researcher from certain activities or sensitive information.

  1. Strategies for Success

Building Rapport:

Invest time in gaining the group’s trust and acceptance.

Minimizing the Hawthorne Effect:

Observe over an extended period to allow participants to become accustomed to the researcher’s presence.

Balanced Role:

Remain involved enough to understand the group’s dynamics but avoid becoming a disruptive presence.

Overt observation allows for ethical and transparent research but requires careful management of trust and authenticity.

44
Q

Participants observation- Conducting a participant observation study (covert observation)

A

Conducting a Participant Observation Study: Covert Observation

  1. Definition

In covert observation, the researcher conceals their true identity and purpose, posing as a genuine member of the group.

  1. Practical Issues
  2. Gaining Access:

The researcher must adopt a believable identity and role to integrate into the group.

This can require significant time and effort to establish credibility.

Example: Learning the group’s norms, language, and behaviors beforehand.

  1. Maintaining Cover:

Researchers must avoid actions or questions that might reveal their purpose, which can limit the depth of inquiry.

Maintaining secrecy can be stressful and requires constant vigilance.

  1. Recording Data:

Covert researchers cannot openly take notes or record interactions, which may lead to memory-based inaccuracies.

Researchers must rely on discreet methods, such as writing notes afterward.

  1. Ethical Dilemmas:

Participants are not informed or consenting, raising significant ethical concerns.

The researcher may witness illegal or harmful activities, posing moral and legal challenges.

  1. Exiting the Group:

Leaving without exposing the research purpose can be difficult, particularly if strong ties have been formed.

  1. Advantages

Natural Behavior:

Participants are unaware they are being observed, reducing the Hawthorne Effect and increasing the authenticity of behavior.

  1. Disadvantages

Ethical issues and limited ability to seek clarification from group members without revealing the purpose of the study.

Covert observation offers deep insights into authentic behaviors but poses significant practical and ethical challenges.

45
Q

Advantages of participant observation

A

Advantages of Participant Observation

  1. Validity

Rich and Detailed Data: Allows researchers to gather in-depth qualitative data on behaviors, interactions, and emotions.

Natural Environment: Observations occur in real-life settings, capturing authentic behavior rather than artificial responses.

  1. Insight

Understanding Social Dynamics: By participating, researchers gain an insider perspective, allowing them to understand group norms, values, and experiences more deeply.

Empathy: Immersion in the group’s daily life helps researchers empathize with participants, leading to more nuanced interpretations.

  1. Flexibility

Adaptability: Researchers can adjust their focus based on emerging findings or unforeseen circumstances during the study.

Exploration of New Topics: Observing real-life scenarios may reveal previously unconsidered areas of interest.

  1. Practical Advantages

Access to Hidden Groups: Useful for studying closed or marginalized communities that are hard to access through other methods.

Cost-Effective: Participant observation often requires fewer resources than large-scale surveys or experiments.

These advantages make participant observation a valuable method for obtaining deep, authentic, and flexible insights into social behavior.

46
Q

Disadvantages of participant observation

A

Disadvantages of Participant Observation

  1. Practical Disadvantages

Time-Consuming: Building trust, integrating into the group, and collecting data over time can take months or years.

Resource Intensive: Requires significant effort, personal involvement, and adaptability.

Difficult Access: Gaining entry into closed groups or subcultures can be challenging.

  1. Ethical Problems

Deception: In covert observation, participants are not informed, violating ethical standards.

Consent: Lack of informed consent raises ethical concerns.

Harm: The researcher may witness or become complicit in illegal or harmful activities.

  1. Representativeness

Small Samples: Observations often focus on small, specific groups, making findings difficult to generalize to larger populations.

  1. Reliability

Subjectivity: Observations rely on the researcher’s interpretations, which may not be consistent or repeatable by others.

Lack of Standardization: The absence of a structured method reduces reliability.

  1. Bias and Lack of Objectivity

Personal Involvement: Researchers may form relationships with participants, leading to emotional bias or “going native.”

Confirmation Bias: Researchers may unconsciously interpret data to fit their expectations or hypotheses.

  1. Validity

Influence on Behavior: In overt studies, the Hawthorne Effect may occur, where participants alter their behavior due to being observed.

Selective Data: Researchers may focus on certain aspects of behavior while neglecting others.

  1. Lack of Concept and Structure

Unsystematic: Observations often lack a clear framework or hypothesis, making it harder to draw structured conclusions.

Data Overload: The open-ended nature can result in an overwhelming amount of unorganized data.

While participant observation provides rich, detailed insights, these disadvantages highlight the need for careful planning, reflection, and triangulation with other methods.

47
Q

Methods in Context: using observation in investigating education (Structured observation)

A

Using Structured Observation to Investigate Education

  1. Practical Issues

Ease of Use:

Structured observation involves pre-determined categories or schedules, making it straightforward to use in classrooms or educational settings.

It is time-efficient, as specific behaviors are systematically recorded.

Resource Limitations:

Observers may require training to accurately record behaviors, adding costs and time.

Classroom settings may pose challenges, such as noise, large class sizes, or restricted access to certain schools.

Limited Scope:

Structured methods may focus only on specific behaviors, missing the broader context or subtle dynamics in the classroom.

  1. Reliability

High Reliability:

Structured observation uses standardized schedules, ensuring consistency across different observers and studies.

The systematic approach minimizes subjective interpretations, allowing replication of results.

Observer Influence:

The presence of the observer may affect teacher or student behavior (Hawthorne Effect), which can slightly impact reliability.

  1. Validity

Limited Insight:

The rigid nature of structured observation may oversimplify complex classroom interactions, reducing the depth of understanding.

Pre-defined categories may fail to capture unexpected behaviors or nuanced interactions.

Improved Objectivity:

The systematic nature improves objectivity, though it may not fully reflect the lived experiences of students or teachers.

Mitigating the Hawthorne Effect:

Extended observation periods can help participants return to their natural behavior, increasing validity.

While structured observation provides reliable and systematic data, its validity may be limited by its inability to fully capture the complexities of educational settings.

48
Q

Methods in Context: using observation in investigating education ( Unstructured observation)

A

Using Unstructured Observation to Investigate Education

  1. Practical Issues

Time-Consuming:

Unstructured observation requires extended periods to observe and record complex classroom interactions.

Analyzing large amounts of qualitative data can also be labor-intensive.

Observer Training:

Requires skilled observers who can identify and interpret subtle behaviors, adding to costs and preparation time.

Classroom Dynamics:

Managing large, active classrooms can make it difficult to observe everything.

  1. Ethical Issues

Informed Consent:

Teachers and students must be fully informed about the observation’s purpose, particularly in overt observation.

Privacy Concerns:

Observing students, particularly minors, requires sensitivity to protect their privacy and anonymity.

Potential Harm:

Students or teachers may feel scrutinized, causing anxiety or stress.

  1. Validity

Rich Data:

Unstructured observation captures detailed, in-depth insights into classroom interactions and social dynamics.

Observers can adapt to emerging situations and focus on unexpected behaviors.

Subjectivity:

Observers’ personal interpretations can introduce bias, reducing objectivity and the accuracy of findings.

  1. The Hawthorne Effect

Behavioral Changes:

Awareness of being observed may cause teachers or students to alter their behavior, reducing authenticity.

Mitigation:

Prolonged observation can help participants become accustomed to the researcher’s presence, restoring natural behavior.

  1. Representativeness

Limited Generalizability:

Unstructured observation often focuses on small groups or single classrooms, making it difficult to generalize findings to the wider educational system.

  1. Reliability

Low Reliability:

Observations are unsystematic and open-ended, making it hard to replicate findings or compare results across studies.

Observer Bias:

Different observers may interpret the same behavior differently, further reducing reliability.

Unstructured observation offers rich, valid data but faces challenges with reliability, representativeness, and ethical considerations in educational research.

49
Q

Secondary sources: Official statistics

A

Official Statistics (Shortened Version)

Official statistics are quantitative data collected by governments or other bodies (e.g., births, deaths, marriages, exam results, school exclusions, crime, unemployment). Examples include the UK Census and data from agencies like Ofsted, used for policy-making and evaluating school effectiveness.

Types of Official Statistics:

Registration: Mandatory records, e.g., birth registrations.

Official Surveys: Surveys like the Census or General Household Survey.

Other Sources: Organizations such as trade unions or educational groups (e.g., National Grammar Schools Association) provide additional statistics, like school performance comparisons.

Advantages of Practical Issues in Official Statistics:

  1. Ease of Access: Official statistics are readily available and often free to access online, saving time and resources.
  2. Large-scale Data: They cover extensive populations, offering a broad and comprehensive dataset.
  3. Time-saving: Using pre-collected data eliminates the need for primary data collection, speeding up research.
  4. Cost-effectiveness: Government-funded data collection reduces the financial burden for sociologists.
  5. Longitudinal Data: Regularly collected data (e.g., censuses) allow for trend analysis over time.

Disadvantages of Practical Issues in Official Statistics:

  1. Lack of Specificity: Data may not focus on the specific research questions of the sociologist.
  2. Outdated Information: Time lags in publication can make statistics less relevant.
  3. Inflexibility: Researchers cannot alter the data collection methods or variables to suit their needs.
  4. Complexity: Large datasets may require significant expertise and time to analyze.
  5. Access Restrictions: Some data may be confidential or require special permissions to use.

Advantages and Disadvantages of Official Statistics in Key Areas

Representativeness

Advantages:

Official statistics are often highly representative as they cover large populations (e.g., Census) or complete groups (e.g., birth registrations).

They allow generalizations across wide demographics, providing a strong foundation for sociological research.

Disadvantages:

Some groups may be underrepresented (e.g., homeless people in surveys or censuses).

Exclusion of marginalized populations limits the full picture of societal trends.

Reliability

Advantages:

Data collection follows standardized methods, ensuring consistency over time and across different contexts.

Reliable data allows researchers to replicate studies and compare trends.

Disadvantages:

Errors in data recording or manipulation (e.g., misreporting by agencies) can affect reliability.

Changes in definitions or methods (e.g., unemployment criteria) over time can limit comparability.

Validity and the ‘Dark Figure’

Advantages:

Official statistics provide insights into patterns (e.g., crime rates, education outcomes).

Disadvantages:

Many statistics lack validity as they may not reflect the full reality, such as unreported crimes or hidden unemployment (referred to as the ‘dark figure’).

Official data may omit subjective experiences, limiting depth.

Positivism

Advantages:

Positivists value official statistics for their quantitative nature, enabling objective, scientific analysis.

They argue such data is useful for identifying social trends and patterns.

Disadvantages:

Critics argue that a purely positivist approach overlooks the underlying meanings or reasons behind the data.

Marxism

Advantages:

Marxists use official statistics to expose inequalities (e.g., income disparities, employment trends).

Disadvantages:

Marxists critique official statistics as ideological tools, reflecting the interests of the ruling class rather than objective truths.

Data may be skewed to serve state or capitalist agendas, masking exploitation or oppression.

50
Q

Secondary data: Documents

A

Documents in Sociological Research

Types of Documents

  1. Public Documents

Produced by organizations such as governments, schools, and businesses.

Examples: School reports, government policies, media articles, and census records.

  1. Personal Documents

Created by individuals to record personal experiences and feelings.

Examples: Diaries, letters, emails, and autobiographies.

  1. Historical Documents

Records from the past that provide insight into historical events and societies.

Examples: Old newspapers, archived government records, and personal journals.

Assessing Documents

  1. Authenticity

Is the document genuine?

Researchers must check whether the document is what it claims to be and if it has been altered.

  1. Credibility

Is the document truthful and accurate?

Researchers should evaluate bias, exaggeration, or omissions in the content.

  1. Representativeness

Does the document represent a wider group or time period?

Issues arise when only a few documents survive or when they represent specific viewpoints (e.g., elite groups).

  1. Meaning

Does the researcher correctly interpret the document?

Context is crucial to understanding the intended meaning, avoiding misinterpretation.

Advantages of Using Documents

Practical: Cheap and accessible, especially for public and historical documents.

Insightful: Provide qualitative data on personal experiences and social phenomena.

Time-saving: Historical documents offer ready-made data that would otherwise take years to gather.

Unobtrusive: Researchers can analyze data without influencing participants.

Disadvantages of Using Documents

Authenticity and Bias: Difficulties verifying the truthfulness or objectivity of personal or historical documents.

Representativeness: Surviving documents may not fully represent the population or era.

Interpretation Issues: Researchers may misinterpret the meaning due to a lack of context.

Ethical Concerns: Using personal documents without consent raises ethical questions.

51
Q

Secondary data: Content analysis

A

Content Analysis

A systematic method to analyze the content of documents and media:

  1. Quantitative Content Analysis: Counts occurrences of specific words, phrases, or themes in a document.

Example: Counting references to gender roles in newspapers.

Advantages: Provides statistical patterns.

Disadvantages: May lack depth and overlook context.

  1. Qualitative Content Analysis: Examines the meaning and underlying themes in the content.

Example: Analyzing how social media posts reflect attitudes toward mental health.

Advantages: Captures subtle meanings and interpretations.

Disadvantages: Time-consuming and subjective.

Content analysis is useful for making systematic comparisons but should be combined with other methods for a fuller understanding.

52
Q

Methods in context: using secondary sources to investigate education (Using official statistics)

A

Methods in Context: Using Official Statistics to Investigate Education

Practical Issues

Ease of Access: Educational statistics (e.g., exam results, attendance rates) are often freely available from government sources like the Department for Education or Ofsted.

Cost and Time Efficiency: Gathering such large-scale data independently would be time-consuming and expensive.

Comparisons Over Time: Official statistics allow researchers to examine trends in education (e.g., the rise in GCSE performance).

Limitations: Researchers may not have control over how the data was collected or the specific categories used (e.g., grouping of data may not align with research needs).

Representativeness

National Coverage: Official statistics typically cover large populations, such as all schools in a country, making them highly representative.

Bias Risks: Focus might be on certain groups or schools (e.g., state-funded schools), potentially excluding private schools or unregistered settings.

Social Class and Ethnicity Data: Statistics often include demographic breakdowns, providing a representative picture of inequalities in education.

Reliability

Standardized Methods: Official statistics are collected systematically, using consistent methods, making them highly reliable.

Replicability: Researchers can replicate studies using the same data, as statistics are compiled annually in the same way.

Issues with Accuracy: Schools may manipulate attendance or exam data to meet performance targets, reducing reliability.

Validity

Superficial Data: Official statistics may lack depth and fail to capture the lived experiences of students or teachers.

Example: Exam results show outcomes but not the reasons behind poor performance (e.g., bullying or teacher shortages).

‘Dark Figure’: Certain issues in education (e.g., unreported bullying, hidden exclusions) may not appear in official statistics, reducing validity.

Operational Definitions: Government definitions (e.g., what counts as truancy) may differ from how sociologists conceptualize these terms.

In Summary:
Using official statistics in educational research offers practical advantages, high reliability, and broad representativeness. However, they may lack validity due to their inability to capture the subjective, lived experiences behind the numbers. Sociologists often complement official statistics with qualitative methods to address these limitations.

53
Q

Methods in context: using secondary sources to investigate education (Using documents)

A

Methods in Context: Using Documents to Investigate Education

Practical Issues

Ease of Access: Many educational documents, such as school policies, inspection reports (e.g., Ofsted), and curriculum guides, are publicly available online or through libraries.

Cost-Effective: Using existing documents saves time and money compared to collecting primary data.

Lack of Standardization: Documents may vary widely in format and content, making comparison across schools or regions difficult.

Time-Consuming Analysis: Personal or historical documents (e.g., teacher diaries, student journals) may require extensive reading and interpretation.

Ethical Issues

Confidentiality: Some documents, such as student records or private emails, may contain sensitive information requiring permission to access.

Consent: Researchers must ensure they have ethical approval to use personal documents, particularly those involving minors or private settings.

Misrepresentation: The interpretation of documents must be fair and accurate to avoid distorting the meaning of the content.

Representativeness

Limited Scope: Documents from a single school, class, or teacher may not represent broader educational practices or trends.

Bias: Public documents often focus on success stories or meet the institution’s agenda, potentially excluding negative or diverse experiences.

Unavailability: Certain groups (e.g., private schools, marginalized students) may not produce or archive documents, reducing representativeness.

Reliability

Consistency: Public documents like Ofsted reports are produced systematically and use standardized criteria, making them reliable.

Subjectivity in Personal Documents: Diaries or letters reflect individual perspectives, which may vary greatly and lack reliability.

Changes Over Time: Historical documents may use outdated language or concepts, complicating consistent interpretation.

Validity

Rich, Qualitative Data: Personal documents (e.g., student essays, teacher journals) provide deep insights into attitudes, experiences, and motivations in education.

Distortion: Some documents (e.g., official reports) are created with specific audiences in mind, potentially skewing the truth to present institutions in a favorable light.

‘Selective Survival’: Historical documents may have been lost or selectively preserved, creating gaps in understanding.

Contextual Interpretation: Understanding the context in which the document was produced is crucial to determining its validity.

In Summary:
Using documents to study education offers valuable insights but comes with challenges related to access, ethical considerations, and the potential for bias or distortion. Researchers must critically evaluate documents for their authenticity, credibility, and context to ensure they are representative and meaningful for their investigation.