Social Memory Flashcards
McNally et al., 2004
- Study that measured psychophysiological responses (e.g. heart rate) to listened to and imagined scripts of alien abduction in individuals who claimed to have been abducted by aliens and individuals who did not (control).
- Results showed greater emotional and physiological reactivity to abduction and stressful scripts than to positive and neutral scripts in “abductees” than controls.
- Findings showed memories of implausible experiences (e.g. alien encounters) elicit physiological responses similar to those elicited by verifiable traumatic experiences (those of PTSD patients) → Physiological reactions indicating emotions during memory recollection cannot be taken as evidence of those memories’ authenticity.
Clancy et al., 2002
- Individuals reporting recovered memories of alien abduction showed more false recall/recognition responses than controls in a semantic associate word task.
- Consistent with the hypothesis that individuals who are prone to false memories in the lab are also prone to false memories in real life.
- The source of the memories (e.g. a fiction film vs a real experience) can be confused leading to distorted memories. → source monitoring errors
Memory suggestibility
- Memory distortion by linguistic presuppositions: the way questions are asked after an event can cause a reconstruction of one’s memory for that event (Loftus & Palmer, 1974)
- Leading questions: questions that suggest an answer or lead the respondent to an answer by the way they are phrased or their content.
Misinformation effect
- Change in the reporting of an event that occurs after the receipt of misleading, postevent information (Loftus, 2005).
- Participants watch materials e.g. video of an accident
- Read a summary of the event, half of the participants with incorrect details (post-event misinformation
- Memory task: worse memory performance of participants exposed to the post-event misinformation than controls (difference of ~30%-40%, Loftus & Pickrell, 1995)
Loftus and Palmer, 1974 - Experiment 1
- Car accident video
- Participants asked: How fast were the cars going when they [hit/smashed] each other?
- Mean speed estimate was 20% higher in ‘smashed’ question participants than ‘hit’ →
- A change in a single word can bias a witness‘ answer to a question
- Possible interpretations:
- The verb “smashed” makes the participants choose the higher speed if they are undecided between two responses such as 30 or 40 mph (response bias).
- The verb “smashed” changes the participant’s memory so that the accident appears as more serious than it was (memory distortion).
Loftus and Palmer, 1974 - Experiment 2
- Based on experiment 1, researchers wanted to know whether the estimation was higher due to response bias (the verb “smashed” makes the participants choose the higher speed if they are undecided between two responses such as 30 or 40 mph) or memory distortion (the verb “smashed” changes the participant’s memory so that the accident appears as more serious than it was).
- Car accident video
- Participants asked: How fast were the cars going when they [hit/smashed] each other?
- One week later they were asked 10 additional questions among which: “Did you see any broken glass?”
- ‘Smashed’ participants were significantly more likely to report broken glass than were ‘hit’ participants → memory distortion (rather than response bias)
- Participants were more likely to remember elements compatible with a more serious accident, but that were not there when the question included a verb suggesting that the accident was serious.
Source monitoring errors
Information perceived in the original films and the external, post-event one may have been integrated in one memory leading to confusion about the information source.
Scoboria et al., 2002
Inducing temporary states make people less likely to notice the discrepancy between the misinformation and their original memory of the event e.g. hypnosis.
Schooler et al., 1986
Descriptions of “unreal” memories are:
- longer
- more verbal hedges (e.g., “I think I saw… ”)
- more cognitive (but not sensory) information (e.g., “My answer was more of an impression… ”)
Rich false memories
- Loftus & Pickrell, 1995
- ‘Lost-in-the-mall’ method: inducing people to believe they had been lost in a mall as a child even if that never happened.
- Findings:
- Over time, the boy remembered increasing details about being lost in the mall (e.g., the rescuer was “really cool”).
- A few weeks later, he gave this memory the second-highest rating in terms of clarity and gave details of where he was lost, what he thought at the time, and the rescuer.
- When told that one of the memories was false, he guessed it was one of the real ones.
Impossible false memories
- Braun et al., 2002
- Participants were given a fake ad showing Bugs Bunny at Disneyland to evaluate on several characteristics.
- The fake ad led 16% of participants to falsely remember having shaken hands with Bugs Bunny at Disneyland (although he is a Warner Bros character).
Flashbulb memories
- Memories of the circumstances in which people learnt of an impactful, and emotionally charged event (Brown & Kulik, 1977; Hirst & Phelps, 2016) e.g. what were you doing when you learn of the terrorist attack of September 11?
- The source events are inherently public because there is an informant (e.g., a journalist, a family member).
- Flashbulb memories are memories of the reception event as opposed to event memories (those about the facts concerning the event): “Do you recall the circumstances in which you first heard that… ?” (Brown & Kulik, 1977, p. 78).
- Content of memories typically involves canonical categories: place (where the news was learnt), aftermath, ongoing activity, own affect, other affect, informant
Brown and Kulik, 1977
Study that showed that assassination of Malcom X and Martin Luther King Junior registered as a flashbulb memory more in Black participants’ mind than in White participants
Are flashbulb memories ‘special’?
- YES - Brown and Kulik, 1977: they are different to other autobiographical memories and underlain by a ‘Print now!’ mechanism, thus indelible, they remain unchanged
- NO - Hirst et al., 2009, 2015: Test-retest methodology showed that flashbulb memories do not remain unchanged. Inconsistencies between the two recollections start to emerge usually within a year and they tend to be repeated. Although initially they may be better recalled, consistency declines as in other autobiographical memories. However, confidence in flashbulb memories remains high.
Flashbulb memories application
- Study of traumatic memories:
- Brown and Kulik (1977) argued for specific neural correlates underlying flashbulb memories.
- Sharot et al. (2007) found a selective activation of amygdala (involved in memory and emotion processing) in participants who were close, but not at the World Trade Center.
- However, learning of a traumatic event is different to directly experiencing it.