Social Cognition and Perception Flashcards
What is meant by the term ‘social cognition’ and ‘social perception’?
what is social cognition? It is essentially the cognitive processes and structures that affect and are affected by social context and behaviour.Social cognition is an approach in social psychology that focuses on how our thinking processes and thoughts are affected by wider and more immediate social contexts, and how thinking and thoughts affect our social behaviour. The focus here is on what goes on in the head of the individual person and how that impacts and is impacted by human interaction.
social perception… that’s like social cognition, right? Well, partly. Social perception is highly related to social cognition, but rather than focusing on how we think about our social world more generally, social perception is interested in our tendency to engage in attributions. Attribution is the process of assigning a cause to your own or another’s behaviour.
Outline the major assumptions of the following broad approaches to social cognition, as found in social psychology: (a) cognitive consistency; (b) naive scientist; (c) cognitive miser; and (d) motivated tactician.
1940-50s
Cognitive consistency A model of social cognition in which people try to reduce inconsistency among their cognitions, because they find inconsistency unpleasant.
1960s
Naive scientist (or psychologist) Model of social cognition that characterises people as using rational, scientific-like,
cause–effect analyses to understand their world.
1970s
A model of social cognition that
characterises people as using the least complex and demanding cognitions that are able to produce generally adaptive behaviours.
(we are often economic rather than accurate when jumping to a conclusion).
1985
Motivated tactician A model of social cognition that
characterises people as having multiple cognitive strategies available, which they choose among on the basis of personal goals, motives and needs.
-a fully engaged thinker who has multiple cognitive strategies available and chooses among them based on goals, motives, and needs. Sometimes the moti vated tactician chooses wisely, in the interests of adaptability and accuracy, and sometimes . . . defensively, in the interests of speed or selfesteem. (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 13)
What are central traits? Provide an example.
What are peripheral traits? Provide an example.
Traits that have a disproportionate influence on the configuration of final impressions, in Asch’s configural model of impression formation. (warm/cold)
Peripheral traits Traits that have an insignificant influence on the configuration of final impressions, in Asch’s configural model of impression formation. (polite/blunt)
A central trait is an attribute in someone’s personality that is considered particularly meaningful, in that its presence or absence signals the presence or absence of other traits. For example, if a person has a warm personality, it usually means that he or she is also friendly, courteous, cheerful, and outgoing—among many other possible traits. A peripheral trait is one whose presence or absence does not imply many other characteristics. For example, if a person is sarcastic, it might imply that he or she is cynical about the world or has a dark sense of humor—but not much else.
What did Asch do to test his ideas and what were his findings?
page 32.
However, the impressions we form are influenced by some bits of information more than others. Very early on, Solomon Asch (1946) argued that some attributes are strongly related in our minds to a large number of other attributes – knowing someone has one of these attributes allows one to infer a great deal about a person and readily form an integrated impression of that person. These attributes he called central traits, to distinguish them from less diagnostic attributes that he called peripheral traits. To investigate this idea, Asch had students read one of two lists of seven adjec tives (traits) describing a hypothetical person. The lists differed only slightly – embedded in one was the word warm and in the other the word cold. The stu dents then evaluated the target person on a number of other dimensions, such as generous/ungenerous, happy/unhappy, reliable/unreliable. Students who read the list containing warm formed a much more favourable impression of the target than did those exposed to the list containing the trait cold (see Figure 2.1). When the words warm and cold were replaced by polite and blunt, the difference in impres sion was far less marked. Asch argued that warm/cold is a central trait dimension that has more influence on impression formation than polite/blunt, which is a peripheral trait dimension.
Perhaps you are now wondering how ordinary people, or social psychologists
for that matter, decide which traits are central and which peripheral. Asch believed that central traits are ones that are intrinsically highly correlated with other traits.
What criticisms have been made regarding the concept of central trait? (Note the role of personal constructs and implicit personality theories).
However, others such as Mark Zanna and David Hamilton (1972) argued that what makes a trait central is influenced by context. In Figure 2.1, a trait that is distinctive (e.g. warm) and semantically linked to the other judgement dimensions (e.g. good-natured) will be more central than one that is nondistinctive or not obviously related to the other dimensions. Yet others have suggested that people have their own idiosyn cratic and enduring beliefs, which the personality psychologist George Kelly (1955) called personal constructs, about which attributes are most important in making judge ments of people – for example, you might organise your impressions around humour while your partner anchors it on intelligence. Arising from his research in person per ception, David Schneider (1973) suggested that people may also have more integrated implicit personality theories, or philosophies of human nature, which are enduring general principles about what sorts of characteristics go together to form certain types of personality. Implicit personality theories are widely shared within cultures but differ between cultures, according to Hazel Markus and her colleagues (Markus, Kitayama & Heiman, 1996), and can sometimes be quite idiosyncratic.
What is the primacy effect? Provide an example.
What is the recency effect? Provide an example.
Impressions are also influenced by the order in which bits of information about the person are encountered. There is a primacy effect in which the first things you learn about a person disproportionately affect your overall impression. For example, Asch (1946) found that people had a more favourable impression of a hypotheti cal person described as being intelligent, industrious, impulsive, critical, stubborn, envious (i.e. positive traits first, negative traits last) than when the order of the traits was reversed. He speculated that early information functions in the same way as central traits. There is also evidence for a recency effect where later information has more impact than earlier information – this is most likely to occur if you are distracted (e.g. overworked, bombarded with stimuli, tired) or you have little moti vation to attend to someone. Overall, however, primacy is more common (Jones & Goethals, 1972) – first impressions really do count!
How do physical cues feed into our impression formations? Provide a summary of a research finding in this area.
Given that in forming impressions of strangers often the first bit of information we have is what they look like, maybe appearance has a primacy effect. Although we would like to believe that we are way too sophisticated to be swayed in our impressions by mere physical appearance, research suggests otherwise – physical appearance has a huge influence on impressions. According to Leslie Zebrowitz and Mary Ann Collins (1997), people do tend to ‘judge a book by its cover’. This may not necessarily always be a bad thing, as appearancebased impressions can be surprisingly accurate. Indeed, as you will find in Chapter 10, impressions based on physical appearance play a critical role in romantic attraction. Now try answering the first focus question. However, forming impressions based on appearance can also have undesirable implications. For example, Mark Knapp (1978) found that professional men taller than 1.88 m had 10 per cent higher starting salaries than men under 1.83 m. In her research conducted in work settings, Madeline Heilman found that attractive male executives were considered more able than less attractive male executives. She also found that this effect was reversed for female executives; participants suspected that attractive female executives had been promoted because of their appearance, not their ability (Heilman & Stopeck, 1985; also see Chapter 7).
What are the positivity and negativity biases? Identify a reason why it might be more difficult to change a negative impression, once it is formed, than a positive impression.
Another problem with appearancebased first impressions is that because racial, ethnic and gender cues are highly visible, people rapidly categorise others and generate impressions based on these cues, effectively stereotyping them, sometimes in negative ways (again, see Chapter 7). Negative impressions formed in this way are difficult to change. Indeed a review of research shows that we usually give more weight to nega tive information when we form impressions than we do to positive information (Skowronski & Carlston, 1989). Even a positive view of a stranger that we have just formed can be dramatically reversed by just a small negative ‘fact’, such as appearing to avoid eye contact a couple of times. Unfortunately, positive infor mation seems to have little impact on a negative impression. Negative information has this effect because it is unusual and distinctive; it may also have survival value because it signals potential danger.
why do we classify people into categories?
The classification of a person or situation as fitting a particular category enables us to then apply schematic knowledge to social perception (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). This means, for example, that:
“Once you recognize someone filling a particular role (e.g., gas station attendant) on the basis of particular attributes (probably male, maybe wearing a jumpsuit, maybe with an oily rag in a pocket, maybe approaching your car, perhaps helping others get gas), then you can apply your knowledge about the role to guide the subsequent interaction (this person can help you with your car).” - Fiske & Taylor (1991, p. 105)
In other words, it is only after categorising a person or a situation that we can apply a particular type of schema.
Define the term ‘schema’ and identify the type of cognitive processing that a schema facilitates once it is invoked. (Note that the plural is either ‘schemas’ or ‘schemata’.)
A schema is a circumscribed and coherent set of interrelated cognitions (e.g. thoughts, beliefs, attitudes) that allows us quickly to make sense of a person, situation, event or place on the basis of limited information. Typically, certain cues activate a schema and the schema then ‘fills in’ missing details to provide a rich set of perceptions, interpretations and expectations.
Once activated, schemas facilitate what is called top-down, conceptdriven or
theorydriven processing – that is, they rapidly generate an overall impression based on preconceptions and prior knowledge. The converse is bottom-up or datadriven processing in which an impression is painstakingly put together from separate bits of information gleaned directly from the immediate context.
Define and identify examples of each of the following types of schemas: (a) person schemas; (b) role schemas; (c) event schemas (scripts); (d) content-free schemas; (e) self-schemas.
Person schemas are idiosyncratic schemas we have about specific people: for example, a close friend (she is kind and intelligent but is shy and would rather frequent cafes than go mountain climbing).
• Role schemas are knowledge structures about role occupants: for example, air line pilots (they fly the plane and should not be seen swigging whisky in the cabin) and doctors (although often complete strangers, they are allowed to ask intimate questions and get you to undress). Role schemas can sometimes be better understood as schemas about social groups, in which case if such schemas are shared, they are social stereotypes.
• Scripts are schemas about events (Abelson, 1981): for example, attending a lecture, having a party, giving a presentation or eating out in a restaurant.
• Self-schemas are schemas about your self – they are often more complex and varied than schemas about other people. They form part of a person’s concept of who they are, the selfconcept, and are discussed in Chapter 3 when we deal with self and identity.
Family resemblance Defining property of category membership.
Fuzzy sets Categories are considered to be fuzzy sets of features organised around a prototype.
Prototype
Cognitive representation of the typical/ideal defining features of a category.
• Content-free schemas do not describe specific people or categories, but are ‘rules’ about how to process information: for example, a contentfree schema might specify how to attribute causes to people’s behaviour (see discussion of attribution theories below); or that if you like John and John likes Tom, then in order to maintain balance you should also like Tom (Heider, 1958).
What is meant by the term ‘family resemblance’?
Family resemblance Defining property of category membership.
What are ‘prototypes’? (Note that they are used to assess family resemblance.)
Prototype
Cognitive representation of the typical/ideal defining features of a category.
What sort of relationship between ‘categories’ is thought to exist?
What determines whether we represent a category as a prototype or an exemplar? As people become more familiar with a category, they shift from using prototypes to exemplars. This shift is most clearcut when people represent out groups (Klein, Loftus, Trafton & Fuhrman, 1992).
Once a person, event or situation is categorised, the relevant schema is invoked. Schemas and prototypes are similar and indeed are often used interchangeably by social psychologists. One way to distinguish them is that prototypes are more nebulous and fuzzy whereas schemas are much more organised (Wyer & Gordon, 1984).
Distinguish between prototypes and exemplars and ensure you understand the difference between prototypes and schemas (go back to earlier definition if necessary).
prototype is a typical idea of something in a category.
exampler is something specific in that category
A schema is a circumscribed and coherent set of interrelated cognitions (e.g. thoughts, beliefs, attitudes) that allows us quickly to make sense of a person, situation, event or place on the basis of limited information.
Note the definition of stereotypes. Briefly overview five things research has told us about stereotypes.
Stereotypes are simplified images of members of a group; they are often deroga tory when applied to outgroups; and they are often based on, or create, clearly visible differences between groups (e.g.
in terms of physical appearance;
Zebrowitz, 1996). They are usually shared by group members characterising members of another group; and can also be shared images of one’s own ingroup.
• People readily describe vast human groups using a few fairly crude shared features. Stereotyping is an adaptive cognitive short cut that allows one to form quick impressions of people. Stereotypes are not inaccurate or wrong, and they may or may not have a kernel of truth; but the key point is that they serve to make sense of particular intergroup relations.
• Because stereotypes are cognitively adaptive they are slow to change. When they do, it is generally in response to wider social, political or economic changes. However, stereotypes of the same group can vary from context to context – they are selected to fit situational demands and our own goals and motives. Stereotypes will usually persist if we can readily access them in memory, because we use them a great deal and they are important to who we are. Changes in accessibility or fit will change the stereotype.
• Some stereotypes are acquired at an early age, often before the child has any knowledge about the groups that are being stereotyped, while others crystallise later in childhood, after age 10 (e.g. Rutland, 1999).
• Stereotypes become more pronounced and hostile when social tensions and con flict arise between groups, and then they are extremely difficult to modify.