Sex and Differences || Flashcards
Hormonal modulation of sexual and aggressive behaviour
1. what are steroid hormones important for?
2. what do castrated mammals show?
3. what are changes in reproductive behaviour tightly correlated with?
4. What reinstates sexual behaviours?
5. What has much less effect?
- Steroid hormones are important in modulating aggressive and sexual behaviours.
- Castrated male mammals show reduced sexual and aggressive behaviour (e.g. stallions / geldings, bulls / heifers). Spayed female mammals show reduced sexual behaviour (e.g. cats, camels). Well known for at least several thousand years.
- There was a time where cammals were used- males cammals were placed miles away to distract the female- removing the ovaries of cammals would stop behaviour
- Changes in reproductive behaviour are tightly correlated with changes in hormonal secretion (e.g. in red deer, the rut is highly correlated with increases in testosterone in the male during autumn).
- Replacement of steroid hormones after either castration or ovariectomy reinstates sexual behaviour.
- but removal of hormones in sexually experienced adult animals may have much less effect - so a strong interaction with learning / experience over a lifetime. This is likely to be especially true in humans. When humans are sexually experienced the sexual aspect after is still rather present.
‘Organising’ and ‘activating’ effects of hormones
A classic experiment reported in 1959 first demonstrated that androgens could have effects on behavior that persisted well beyond the time that they were present (‘organizing effects’) in addition to relatively immediate effects (‘activating effects’).
Control- normal pregnancies
Experimental group- testosterone was administered during pregnancy
Ask what happens if now you take the female gp, remove their natural source of hormones and then in the first part of the experiment those gp are fed with female type steroides. What the gp do in that situation is they show a behaviour called ordosis. This is the behaviour that occurs if they are ready to mate with the male.
Then you wait and let the effect of those hormones dissapear and then you treat with testosterone. Those females who were responsive to testosterone in the process would show the appropriate behaviour (attempt to mate with the male)
Guinea pigs were exposed to testosterone prenatally, and then failed to show female sexual behavior
as adults. However, unlike control animals) they responded to adult treatment with testosterone by showing male sexual behavior
(Phoenix, et al. (1959) Endocrinology, 65, 369–382.
Effects of hormones early on in life has an affect on hormones in their later life
‘Organising’ effects of hormones:
Evidence from congenital adrenal hyperplasia
- Congenital adrenal hyperplasia is an inherited recessive condition in which a loss of an enzyme in the pathway for aldosterone synthesis (typically 21-⍺-hydroxylase) leads to excess androgen production by the adrenal gland during foetal development. It has relatively small effects on boys, but leads to physical masculinisation (‘virilisation’) in girls.
- See- substantial effect in girls which is absent in the boys
- It is also related to toy choice in young girls. Additional evidence suggests that any parental influence would reduce rather than enhance the effect.
- Hines’ short review describes the way in which a similar type of effect may be observed in vervet monkeys.
Sex differences in human aggressive behaviour
- A recent metanalysis confirms that there are large or very large (Cohen’s d values around 1) sex differences in weapon use, violent crime, homicide and violent computer-game use, and also in rape (Archer 2019).
- Darwin (1871) suggested that competition between males for access to mates leads to sex differences in size, strength and aggressive behaviour. Extended periods of maternal care for infants may be associated with avoidance of the risks associated with aggressive encounters.
- due to competing for females therefore leading to sex and behavioural differences
- The difference in aggressive behaviour emerges before puberty but there is no direct evidence to suggest that it depends on prenatal hormonal exposure.
- It’s important to note that some of the differences in aggressive behaviour are in the tail of the distributions. Even if biological factors play a part, they are likely to be (strongly) modulated by social and other environmental influences.
- organizing effects- effects emerge before puberty but there isn’t
- note you are looking at effects of the tail of the population
Organising’ and ‘activating’ effects of hormones: two human studies - 1
Study of traders in financial derivatives at a London bank (260 males– just 4 women in sample). Measures of testosterone levels at 11.00h and 16.00h – related to profit or loss for the day.
Individuals who have higher testosterone levels in the morning in two ways:
* overall they were more successful
* there was much greater variance in their performance- some did bad, some did very well
Its likely this effect is related to risk taking behaviour
High testosterone at 11.00h predicts greater profitability for the rest of the day. An ‘activating’ effect?
Cortisol levels were high when individual returns were more variable.
Organising’ and ‘activating’ effects of hormones:two human studies - 2
Used a controversial measure of organising effects
Study of traders in financial derivatives on a New York trading floor. Measures of digit ratio (2D:4D)
– related to long term profit or loss records
(Coates et al. (2009) PNAS 106:623).
A low 2D:4D ratio is associated with high levels of fetal testosterone. This ratio was able to predict profit and loss- when ratio is low high testosterone exposure
These data show it is also associated with higher success on the trading floor. An ‘organising’ effect?
Two obvious issues with these data in relation to sex / gender differences:
* Virtually no female participants
* Studies of adults completely confound biological with environmental and cultural influences
Androgens and sport
Synthetic androgens (e.g. nandrolone) have been widely (ab)used, by both men and women, for their performance enhancing effects in sport:
* Increase lean body mass (e.g. muscle tissue)
* Stimulate red blood cell production- important for stamina
* Enhances visuospatial neural activation
The relative effects of increased androgen levels in women may be greater (2% - 5% increase in performance).
So what should be the position of women with naturally high levels of androgen production?
Caster Semenya is an intersex cisgender women, assigned female at birth, with XY chromosomes and naturally elevated testosterone levels due to 5a-Reductase deficiency.
Dutee Cahnd, an Indian female athlete with naturally high androgen levels (physiology unknown) has supported Semanya
What is there evidence for in elite performance in some sports?
There is evidence for sex differences in elite performance in some sports.
800m world record 101s (112s in 1912) 113s (150s in 1922)
(100m not reliable in early records because of short timing)
The trend in female performances down wards is much stronger
There is also limited evidence that women with naturally higher androgen levels (T)
may perform better in some events but not others:
100m 11.9s (low T) 12.1s (high T) ns
800m 121.8s (low T) 120.5s (high T) *
… the difference was only significant in 4 of 21 events, though sample sizes are small (Bermon & Garnier 2017). Does this justify a high T cutoff in women’s elite sport.
- Many other differences between individuals affect sporting achievement, especially at elite levels. These include weight, height and lower body symmetry.
- Should events be subdivided using such criteria? It happens in boxing, wrestling and weightlifting. Weightlifting has 10 weight classes as well as separating the events by sex!
Sporting status of trans athletes
Are testosterone levels the appropriate marker to use?
The position of individuals who transition from male to female status has been especially controversial. Drug treatments, such as a GnRH antagonist, will greatly reduce testosterone levels. In many sports a consistently low testosterone level is taken as a marker for ‘fair’ competition by trans women.
However this does not abolish the sexual dimorphisms in
muscle size and strength that begin at puberty.
The decrease in the graph is rather small and the sex difference in still there
Biology won’t provide a neat answer to these sporting dilemmas.
Oxytocin and vasopressin also act as neurotransmitters within the central nervous system
Affect affiliative behaviour
The neurotransmitter is secreted into vesicles within the presynaptic terminal. When the presynaptic neuron becomes active the neurotransmitter is released, diffuses along synaptic cleft and activates neurotransmitter receptors on the membrane of the postsynaptic cell.
Species differences in mating systems
The most common pattern of care in mammals is care by female alone. However stable pair bonds, with care of infants by both parents, are also found in some species, though relatively rarely.
Different patterns of mating systems and affiliative behaviour
Prairie voles have stable pair bonds and both parents care for the young.
… but meadow vole (live in a more open environment) males move on from one female to another and do not take care of the young.
So… Whats the basis of this difference in mating systems
Oxytocin and vasopressin are important in the development of the pair bond in prairie voles
Extra oxytocin (OT) or vasopressin (AVP)
strengthens pair preference, but blockade
of receptors (antagonist) weakens pair
preference.
Further experiment suggest specific learning
-related effects.
If you take a female vole and place her with a male, then if you treat her with oxytosin then you can greatly increase her preference for spending time with that male. Therefore oxytosis enhances preference.
If you take a female who has already paired up with a male and now you treat her with an oxytosis antagonist then that greatly reduces her preference for spending time with the male.
If you take a male and treat him with AVP- it greatly enhances. But if treat with an antagonist- greatly reduced.
Large numbers of oxytocin receptors are present in the prairie vole (left), but not the meadow vole (right)
When looking at brains of voles for the density of oxytosis receptors, there are big differences between the species.
The black areas are those with high density of oxytocin receptors. They are concentrated in an area associated with reward-related learning.
Distribution of vasopressin receptors in prairie and meadow voles
Monogamous prairie vole: Time spent huddling was higher than in polygamous meadow voles
Adding vasopressin receptors to the meadow vole brain increases the tendency to form a pair bond
Here vasopressin receptors have been introduced into the meadow vole brain by using a viral vector.
Again the measure of pair preference is greatly increased - so confirming that the meadow vole produces vasopressin, but does not normally express the relevant receptors.
All of these studies have been carried out by Thomas Insel (reading on final slide).
Meadow voles now began to express strong preferences