Selective social learning Flashcards
How do children learn from social constructivism?
- proposed by Vygotsky
- explained that children are curious explorers, but the best way children learn is through the collaboration between the child and more knowledgeable members of society
- more knowledgeable members of society offer scaffolding-offer tailored by modelling activities ad providing verbal instructions
Piaget saw the child as an “autodidact”, what does this mean?
They are self taught. Children learn primarily from their own exploration and active interpretation of the data that they make themselves gather
what is testimony?
information communicated via assertions (in contrast to information we gain by sense experience)
what do we rely on testimony for?
- for general knowledge (science, history, politics)
- for specific information (train times, weather outlook)
- for culture norms and rules (keep clothes on in public)
- for personal information (our date of birth)
what are the 3 types of learning from others?
- formal: explicit teaching
- informal: everyday dialogue with adults, siblings and peers, asking questions, imitation, overhearing
- indirectly: through books, TV and the internet
How do children learn from others?
- who and what to approach/avoid
- what things are called
- what timings are for
- how to categorise correclty
what are the limitations of learning from others?
- testimony nor always reliable
- some sources more credible than others
what is Epistemic vigilance? (Sperber et al., 2010)
- evaluating the credibility of the information source and the plausibility of the claims, and calibrate trust in testimony accordingly
- it is needed to achieve effective social learning
how do we evaluate testimony using competence and benevolence
whether someone is competent to provide info and a good intentions as opposed to wanting to deceive us
Historical perspective o whether children trust everything we tell them?
- “..a disposition to confide in the veracity of others and to believe what they tell us…It is unlimited in children” (Reid, 1764)
- Bertrand Russell claimed that: “Doubt, suspense of judgment and disbelief all seem later and more complex than a wholly unreflecting assent” (Russell, 1921).
- “A child learns there are reliable and unreliable informants much later than it learns the facts which are told it” (Wittgenstein, 1969).
recent perspective o whether children trust everything we tell them?
- “Children are especially credulous, especially gullible, especially prone toward acceptance and belief – as if they accepted as effortlessly as they comprehended but had yet to master the intricacies of doubt.” (Gilbert, 1991)
- According to Dawkins, credulity is adaptive: “It is easy to see why natural selection – the survival of the fittest – might penalize an experimental and skeptical turn of mind and favour simple credulity in children.” (Dawkins, 1995)
eye scepticism in children
- Rejecting blatantly false claims
- From 16 months infants reject false labels (Koenig & Echols, 2003; Pea, 1980), for example a ball and a hat and the experimenter labelling them the wrong way around
- 3-4 year-olds reject claims that are inconsistent with their own perceptual judgement (Clement et al., 2004), experiementer looks in box and says there is a blue and red pom pom when it’s actually 2 yellow ones. Children disagree with the experimenter
Trusting your own eyes vs. testimony, Tamis-Lemonda et al., (2008)
- pitted perceptual cues and social cues
- mums encouraged children to walk down risky slopes and discouraged them to walk down safe slopes
- 18mo’s were found to ignore advice and relied on perceptual information
- children relied on mums advice when they could not assess risk
are children biased to believe things?
- 2 and 3 yr-olds accept labels conflicting with own perceptions (Jaswal & Markman, 2007)
- 4 yr-olds more likely to accept conflicting labels if given additional information suggesting it’s an unfamiliar subtype of that category: “This is a Moroccan bird” (Jaswal, 2004)
- 6 and 8 yr-olds more likely to accept conflicting labels when stimuli is ambiguous (Chan & Tardiff, 2013)
Role of prior knowledge in assessing unexpected testimony
some studies demonstrate role of intuition/prior knowledge when children making trust judgements:
- Children compare other people’s testimony to their existing knowledge
- When own knowledge base is strong (in unambiguous situations) they are more likely to stick with own beliefs
- When own knowledge base is weak (in ambiguous situations) they are more likely to accept implausible claims