Reading and Evaluating Scientific Research (LO) Flashcards
Identify the qualities of good scientific research
- Objectivity: Unbiased and impartial results.
2.Reliability: Consistency in findings when research is repeated. - Validity: The extent to which the research measures what it claims to.
- Replicability: The ability for others to replicate the study with similar results.
- Transparency: Clear reporting of methods and findings.
- Ethics: Research conducted in accordance with ethical guidelines.
Identify examples of over-generalization
- Drawing broad conclusions from a small, specific sample.
- Example: Conducting a study on a group of college students and claiming the findings apply to all age groups.
- Another example: Observing one person from a particular culture and assuming everyone in that culture behaves the same way.
Differentiate different forms of reliability
- Test-Retest Reliability: Consistency of results when the same test is repeated on the same sample after some time.
- Inter-Rater Reliability: The degree to which different observers or raters agree on their observations.
- Internal Consistency: The consistency of results across items within a test (e.g., using Cronbach’s alpha).
- Parallel-Forms Reliability: Consistency between different versions of the same test designed to measure the same construct.
Differentiate reliability from validity
- Reliability: Consistency of a measure—whether it produces stable results over time or across observers.
- Validity: Accuracy of a measure—whether it actually measures what it is intended to measure.
- Example: A bathroom scale might consistently give the same weight (reliable), but if it’s calibrated incorrectly, it won’t give the true weight (invalid).
Identify and differentiate examples of bias in research
- Sampling Bias: When the sample is not representative of the population (e.g., only studying wealthy individuals to represent a general population).
- Confirmation Bias: When researchers favor information that supports their preconceptions.
- Observer Bias: When the researcher’s expectations influence their interpretation of the data.
- Publication Bias: When studies with positive or significant findings are more likely to be published than studies with null results.
Identify and differentiate techniques that reduce bias
- Anonymity
- Confidentiality
- Inform Participants
- Single-blind Study
- Double-blind study
Identify how the publication process facilitates good science
- Peer Review: Ensures research is critically evaluated by experts before publication.
- Replication Requirements: Publishing clear methods allows others to replicate and verify results.
Differentiate between strong and weak forms of evidence
- Strong Evidence: Based on well-designed experiments, large sample sizes, and is replicable (e.g., randomized controlled trials, meta-analyses).
- Weak Evidence: Based on anecdotal accounts, small sample sizes, or poorly controlled studies.
- Example: A well-designed clinical trial is stronger evidence than a single case report.
Identify and differentiate types of descriptive research
- Case Studies: In-depth analysis of a single individual or group.
- Naturalistic Observation: Observing behavior in its natural setting without interference.
- Surveys/Questionnaires: Collecting data from a large group of people to describe attitudes or behaviors.
- Archival Research: Analyzing existing data sets or records.
- Example: Survey research can describe political attitudes in a population, but doesn’t explain why people hold those attitudes.
Differentiate between strong and weak correlations
- Strong Correlation: When two variables have a high degree of association (closer to +1 or -1).
- Weak Correlation: When the association between two variables is low (closer to 0).
- Example: A correlation of +0.8 (strong positive) vs. a correlation of +0.2 (weak positive).
Differentiate between positive and negative correlations
- Positive Correlation: As one variable increases, the other also increases (e.g., height and weight).
- Negative Correlation: As one variable increases, the other decreases (e.g., stress and immune function).
- Example: Hours studied and exam scores are positively correlated, while exercise frequency and body weight may show a negative correlation.
Identify the problems with inferring causation from correlation
- Correlation does not imply causation: Just because two variables are correlated doesn’t mean one causes the other.
- Third Variable Problem: A third factor may influence both correlated variables (e.g., ice cream sales and drowning incidents both increase in summer due to hot weather).
- Directionality Problem: It can be unclear which variable is influencing the other.
Generalize and apply understanding of correlations to one’s life
- Recognize that correlations in daily life (e.g., exercise and mood improvement) don’t always mean one causes the other.
- Example: If you notice that getting more sleep is correlated with better concentration, you can’t be certain that sleep causes better concentration—other factors, like overall health, may be involved. However, improving sleep may still be a good strategy to test and potentially enhance focus.
Identify the qualities of experimental research
- Manipulation: The researcher manipulates one or more independent variables.
- Control: Control over extraneous variables to avoid confounding factors.
- Random Assignment: Participants are randomly assigned to different groups to avoid selection bias.
- Replication: The experiment can be repeated to verify results.
- Causal Inference: The ability to infer cause-and-effect relationships due to the controlled conditions.
Identify and differentiate among independent, dependent and confounding variables
- Independent Variable (IV): The variable that is manipulated by the researcher to observe its effect (e.g., the type of treatment).
- Dependent Variable (DV): The variable that is measured to see if it is affected by changes in the IV (e.g., response to treatment).
- Confounding Variable: An uncontrolled variable that could influence the DV, potentially skewing results (e.g., participants’ prior knowledge or experience).