normative development of fears Flashcards
childhood fears
- Assessed directly (and often retrospectively) using child interviews, questionnaires, parent/teacher report.
- Normal childhood fears are:
- Commonly experienced/universal?
- Relatively mild.
- Appear and disappear spontaneously.
- Follow a predictable pattern?
- Decrease with age?
· Gullone (2000) reviewed interview studies with 4-19 year olds: - Average number of fears is 2-5 per child.
- Tend to elicit general themes e.g. animals, death/injury, the unknown, social concern
fear survey schedule for children - revised
· Ollendick (1983):
- 80 item measure of children’s fear in response to a range of specific stimuli/situations (none, some, a lot).
· Measures number, severity and type of normal fears children experience.
· Five reliable factors:
- Fear of danger and death (e.g. being hit by a car or truck).
- Fear of failure and criticism (e.g. looking foolish).
- Fear of the unknown (e.g. going to bed in the dark).
- Fear of animals (e.g. snakes).
- Stress and medical fears (e.g. getting an injection from the doctor).
methodological issues
· Some of the items are quite outdated and contemporary threats (e.g. climate change) not included (Fishkin, Rohrbach & Anderson-Johnson, 1997; Lengua, Long, Smith & Meltzoff, 2005).
· May not adequately capture cultural variation in childhood fears.
· Can only measure what is included, not an exhaustive list (see Muris, Merckelbach, Meesters & Van Lier, 1997).
· Might not index actual frequency of fears (McCathie & Spence, 1991).
Ollendick, King and Fray, 1989
· 1185 children and adolescents (395 aged 7-10, 449 aged 11-13, 341 aged 14-16).
· Recruited in USA and Australia.
· Average of 14 fears reported (see also Ollendick, Yang, King, Dong & Akande, 1996).
· Top fears relate to dangerous situations and physical harm.
· Children who identified as girls report more fears than children who identified as boys
· Fears highest for death/danger items
moderation of childhood fears
- Gender
- Cultural variation
- Socioeconomic effects
gender effects
- Gullone and King (1993) - items that most strongly discriminated between boys and girls, e.g., rats, spiders, snakes, mice, creepy houses, being alone, bad dreams
- Gender effects may have be explained by biological sex differences and/or gender role orientation
- High femininity and low masculinity associated with greater anxiety and avoidance (Ginsburg and Silverman, 2000; Muris, Meesters and Knoops, 2005)
- Gender role orientation stronger predictor of fear than child’s biological sex (Brody, Hay and Vandewater, 1990)
cultural variation
· Across “Western” countries lots of consistency:
- Number of fears decreases with age
- Girls more fearful than boys
- Content of fears appears to show similar developmental pattern (but also some idiosyncrasies e.g., sharks in Australia)
· Fear levels may vary as a function of cultural group membership
· Cross-cultural differences have been found within-countries - e.g., in the USA, hispanic youth display higher fear and anxiety than white youth
cultural variation 2
· Ollendick et al. (1996)
- 1200 participants aged 7-17 years in Nigeria, China, USA and Australia
· Differences in intensity and patterns of fears:
- Nigeria > China > America = Australia.
- Girls > boys apart from Nigeria.
- Fears decreased with age only in USA and Australian samples. No age differences in Nigerian sample and peak in anxiety in late childhood (10-14yrs) in Chinese sample.
- Common fears primarily death/danger related but more social-evaluative and safety-related fear in Nigerian and Chinese samples.
- Idiosyncratic fears – ghosts in China, looking foolish in USA, ocean in Nigeria, guns in Australia.
· Socialisation practices?
- Collectivist cultures emphasise self-control, social inhibition and compliance with social norms which might fuel greater fear and anxiety.
socioeconomic effects
· Lower SES children report more fears (Croake, 1969; Croake & Knox, 1973; Sidana, 1975).
· Differences in content of fears:
- Low SES: animals, strange people, abandonment by parents, death, violence, knives.
- Middle/Upper SES: heights, ill health, rollercoasters, pet’s safety.
· Children in low SES environments are exposed to more specific threats and enhanced general feelings of fear and anxiety.
developmental patterns
Often widely argued that childhood fears show a predictable developmental pattern
· Predictable developmental pattern?
- Infants: environmental stimuli (loud noises, separation, unusual stimuli).
- 4-8 years: ghosts, imaginary creatures, and animals.
- 10-12 years: social fears, self-injury.
· Some evidence that this patterns maps on to the age of onset of phobias (Field & Davey, 2001; Muris & Field, 2011):
- Height/water phobia begins in infancy.
- Animal phobias start between 7-9.
- Social fears in pre-adolescence.
· BUT
· Some researchers argue that some fears may be innate and present from very early in development e.g. snakes, spiders.
developmental patterns 2
· Some research consistent with a predictable pattern but results are mixed and sometimes hard to interpret across studies.
· Bauer (1976):
- Fear of monsters and ghosts decreases with age.
- Fear of bodily injury and physical danger increases with age.
· Muris, Merckelbach, Gadet & Moulaert (2000):
- Fears and scary dreams more common in 7-9 year olds compared to 10-12 year olds.
- Worry more common in 10-12 year olds than 4 – 6 year olds.
developmental patterns 3
· Westenberg, Drewes, Goedhart, Siebelink & Treffers, 2004:
- 882 participants aged 8-18 years.
- Assessed social and physical fears using the FSSC-R.
theoretical approaches
· Suggests some fears can be learned through conditioning experiences i.e., verbal information, vicarious learning.
· Some fears may be innate or we might be prepared to learn them very rapidly and at an early age
evolutionary approach
· Natural selection favours individuals who rapidly learn about threats that pose a danger to self because this facilitates survival.
· Fear system evolved to focus on threats at ages at which those threats would have been greatest risk to our ancestors (Ohman, Dimberg & Ost, 1985).
· Some fears may be innate and may not need to be learned at all (Poulton & Menzies, 2002).
· We may be prepared to rapidly acquire some fears with little or no prior learning.
cognitive development
· Fear and anxiety originates from conceptualisation of threat.
· Conceptualisation of threat depends on a child’s cognitive and physical abilities (Vasey, 1993).
· As cognitive abilities (e.g. biological regulation, memory, self-control, theory of mind, counterfactual reasoning) develop, fear and anxiety become more sophisticated.
· Range of fear-provoking stimuli broadens and cognitive features of anxiety (e.g. worry) become more prevalent.