Neuroimaging Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

MRI scanner

A
  • Incredibly strong magnet: usually 1.5 - 7 tesla
  • 1.5 tesla = 15,000 gauss
  • Earth’s magnetic field strength = 0.5 gauss
  • Exeter MRI scanner = 30,000 X earth’s magnetic field strength
  • NO METAL IN THE SCANNER!!!
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

what does BOLD stand for?

A

Blood Oxygen Level Dependent

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

BOLD signal

A
  • When neurons become active, blood flows to the part of the brain to provide oxygen to fuel the cells.
  • Haemoglobin (the iron-containing oxygen transporting protein present in blood) differs in how it responds to magnetic fields, depending on whether it has a bound oxygen molecule
  • The MRI scanner, which is basically a giant magnet, detects these small changes in the magnetic field.
  • With fMRI we are not directly measuring brain activation.
  • We are measuring blood flow (more precisely, the magnetic properties of oxygenated vs deoxygenated blood)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

from raw data to functional ‘activation’ maps

A
  1. Design a task to be used in the scanner
  2. Collect some data
  3. Preprocess the data
  4. Analyse the data
  5. Interpret your results
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

fMRI experimental design

A
  • BOLD signal is arbitrary: It has no stable baseline - means that the baseline BOLD signal may be something in one session for one subject, and completely different for the same subject on the next day
  • Therefore, the most important aspect of any fMRI experiment is that we have to provide both a) an experimental condition and b) a baseline condition.
  • Resulting functional brain map reflects the difference between these two conditions.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

what is a good baseline?

A
  • One that differs from the experimental condition only by the process of interest
  • In general, having the subject rest, i.e. do nothing, is not a good baseline, because there will be so many things differing between that and your experimental condition that you never know what process your brain activation reflects.
  • Ideally you want a baseline that differs from the experimental condition only according to the cognitive process you’re interested in.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

block designs

A
  • BOLD signal is slow - peaks 4-5 seconds after stimulus onset and takes around 16 seconds to return to baseline
  • All fMRI experiments originally employed block designs - long periods of alternating task/baseline performance
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

disadvantages of block designs

A
  • Block designs often group together lots of trials
  • Highly predictable occurrence of stimuli: subjects know what is coming and may alter strategies accordingly (not always a pro)
  • Inflexible for more complex tasks: impact of oddball stimuli? or stimuli or events that occur uncontrollably?
  • Ecological validity. Does blocking trials change the psychological process you are interested in?
  • Can’t separate trials by performance - e.g. to look at activation associated with correct vs incorrect response
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

event related design

A

Trials of different conditions are randomly intermixed and occur close together in time

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

advantages of event-related design

A
  • Flexibility and randomisation - eliminate predictability of block designs - avoid practice effects
  • Post hoc sorting - (e.g., correct vs. incorrect, aware vs. unaware, remembered vs. forgotten items, fast vs. slow RTs)
  • Can look at novelty and priming - rare or unpredictable events can be measured - e.g., P300
  • Can look at temporal dynamics of response - Dissociation of motion artefacts from activation - Dissociate components of delay tasks - Mental chronometry
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

collect some data

A
  • 2 to 3 seconds to collect a single ‘volume’
  • To reference each point in the brain in 3D space we divide image into cubes or ‘voxels’ - Typical voxel size = 3x3x3 mm - Refer to each voxel with coordinates (x, y, z)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

preprocess the data

A
  • Correcting for non-task-related variability in experimental data - Getting rid of the ‘noise’
    1. High pass filtering
    2. Motion correction
    3. Slice time correction
    4. Co-registration
    5. Normalisation
    6. Spatial smoothing
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

high pass filtering

A
  • Remove low frequency oscillations such as scanner drift that introduce noise into your data
  • Standard low pass filter is approx 120 secs
  • The consequence of this is that we can’t contrast events more than a couple of minutes apart because they will be wiped out by our filter.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

slice time correction

A
  • Because it takes a few seconds to collect a whole brain volume, different slices may be acquired up to a couple of seconds apart
  • This means that our estimate of the haemodynamic response will be incorrect for slices acquired later
  • Slice timing simply corrects this by moving them earlier in time
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

coregistration

A

Each functional image is aligned with the subject’s structural image so that functional maps can subsequently be overlaid and anatomical landmarks identified

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

normalisation

A
  • Ultimately we want to do statistics on group activation maps, so we have to get all the brains into a standard space
  • To do this we use complex algorithms to warp each subject’s brain into the shape of a template brain
17
Q

normalisation - standardises space

A
  • Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space - Combination of 352 MRI scans on normal controls
  • Can compare activation across subjects and do group analyses.
18
Q

smoothing

A

Application of Gaussian kernel - allows closer neurons to influence the value more strongly than distal ones.

19
Q

why smooth?

A
  • Because neurons do not fire in isolation.
  • If a neuron fires, neurons close to it will tend to fire as well.
  • Smoothing attempts to model the data according to this property of neurons.
20
Q

analysis

A
  • Multiple regression is used to determine the effect of a number of independent variables (our conditions) on a single dependent variable (brain activation)
  • For each voxel, we use multiple regression to estimate how closely the BOLD signal correlates with the timecourse of each condition.
  • Finally perform a contrast - simply a t-test comparing beta condition 1 beta values to condition 2 beta values
  • Calculating the t-statistic for each of the 130,000 voxels in the brain is equivalent to asking “in which regions is activation greater in condition 2 than in condition 1?”
  • Raw t-map indicates the magnitude of the t-statistic using a colour scale - Yellow = bigger t values
  • The threshold we apply defines what we accept as a significant difference in activation between our 2 conditions
  • Essentially arbitrary (as it is in any statistical procedure)
  • Standard alpha level in psychological research is p < 0.05, i.e. we accept a 5% rate of false positives
21
Q

multiple regression

A

Simply a way to find out the explanatory effect of a number of independent variables on a dependent variable.

22
Q

correction for multiple comparisons

A
  • Brain images divided into up to 130,000 voxels = 130,000 individual t-tests
  • Chance of a type 1 error (false positive) increases with every test performed
  • With an alpha level of p < 0.05 (standard used in psychological research) with one t-test we have a 5% chance of type 1 error.
  • With 100 independent t-tests, we have a 99.4 % chance of type 1 error
  • In other words, with 130,000 t-tests and an alpha level of 0.05 we are guaranteed to find some significant voxels simply by chance.
  • Very important to correct for multiple comparisons - adjust alpha level (p-value)
23
Q

different approaches to data analysis

A
  1. Whole brain analysis

2. Region of Interest (ROI) analysis

24
Q

advantages of WBA

A
  • Requires no prior hypotheses about areas involved

- Includes entire brain

25
Q

disadvantages of WBA

A
  • Can lose spatial resolution with inter-subject averaging
  • Can produce meaningless “laundry lists of areas” that are difficult to interpret
  • Depends highly on statistics and threshold selected
  • Multiple comparisons problem
26
Q

WBA

A

examine effects on a voxel by voxel basis across the whole brain

27
Q

ROI analysis

A

Restrict our analysis to a particular brain region

28
Q

advantages of ROI analysis

A
  • Hypothesis driven - avoids meaningless ‘laundry lists’ of activated regions.
  • Avoids multiple comparisons problem - data summarised in a single number per subject reflecting mean activation across voxels in ROI.
  • Simple - data can be exported and treated as any other type of data, requiring no special software for further analysis
  • Generalisable - data can easily be compared across studies, e.g. for meta analysis.
29
Q

disadvantages of ROI

A
  • Easy to miss things going on elsewhere in the brain.

- Not always simple how to define ROIs

30
Q

limitations of fMRI

A
  • fMRI data is correlative, therefore we can’t say that a region activated during a task/function is essential for that task/function.
  • Need converging evidence from TMS/neuropsychology to make stronger inferences about causation.
  • Temporal resolution of fMRI is low
  • Need converging evidence from EEG/TMS to provide finer grained temporal information.