Lifespan development 2 - social & emotional development (chapter 13) Flashcards
What are babies born with regarding emotions?
Although infants cannot describe their feelings, research illustrates that their facial expressions, vocalisations and other behaviours provide a window into their emotional life.
We can perceive a number of emotions in young infants. For example, crying, watching their face after its been fed for contentment, when we see a baby focusing their gaze and staring at objects this gives a palpable sense that they are interested in the focus of their attention. As infants get older and turn into children, these more basic emotions branch out and divide into more fine-grained distinctions. About 6 months after birth, infants begin to show joy and surprise, and distress branches out into the separate emotions of disgust, anger, fear and sadness
When does a sense of self emerge?
A sense of self emerges but develops gradually over the first years of life, beginning with infants’ ability to perceive their own bodies and limbs. One important milestone in understanding the self occurs around 18 months of age, when infants start to respond appropriately to seeing themselves in a mirror. This growing self-awareness sets the stage for envy, embarrassment and empathy to emerge. After age 2, toddlers lean about performance standards and rules that they are supposed to follow, they begin to display pride and shame. Around the same age, they also display guilt - as evidenced by avoiding eye contact, shrugging shoulders and making facial expressions
What is emotional regulation?
The processes by which we evaluate and modify our emotional reactions
How does emotional regulation become more diverse with age?
Young infants may suck their thumb or pacifier, turn their head away from something unpleasant or cling to a parent or caregiver to soothe themselves. To reduce distress, toddlers may seek out a caretaker, cling to a doll or teddy bear, fling unpleasant objects away, and learn to smile, pout or throw a tantrum to get what they want. Once they acquire language, children can reduce distress by talking to themselves and other people.
As children age, their emotional expressiveness and ability to regulate their emotions become part of their overall emotional competence, which in turn influences their social behaviour and how well their peers and other people like them. Children who frequently display sadness or who cannot control their anger are less likely to be popular, and emotional competence remains important for well-being as children develop
Can newborns recognise emotions in others?
Research by Field, Woodson, Greenberg and Cohen (1982) has shown that newborn babies will imitate the emotional expressions they see in others, indicating that they are at least able to distinguish between these facial expressions. It is difficult to know if they feel these emotions themselves, though.
Social referencing is a behaviour in which infants or children use the emotions of another person (often the caregiver) to guide their actions. Sorce et al. (1985) undertook an experiment on social referencing using the ‘visual cliff’. In this study the infants were placed in front of the cliff, and a parents was positioned on the other side of the cliff and adopted either a happy encouraging expression or a fearful expression. If the mother adopted a happy face, the infants were fairly likely to cross over. Almost none of the infants whose mother adopted a fearful face attempted to. Their actions were guided by their parents emotions
Increased social interactions with a wider group of people influences children’s emotional development, as parents, teachers and peers serve as models and reinforce children for some types of emotional responses. But as we now explore, heredity makes a very important contribution to children’s basic emotional-behavioural style
What is social referencing?
A behaviour in which infants or children use the emotions of another person (often the caregiver) to guide their actions.
What is temperament?
A biologically based general style of reacting emotionally and behaviourally to the environment.
Some infants are calm and happy; others are irritable and fussy. Some are outgoing and active; others are shy and inactive. Within any age group, people differ in temperament
In a study by Thomas and Chess (1977), what was done and found?
They asked parents to describe their babies’ behaviour. They found that most infants could be classified into 3 groups:
- ‘Easy infants’ ate and slept on schedule, were playful and accepted new situations with little fuss
- ‘Difficult infants’ were irritable, fussy eater and sleepers, and reacted negatively to new situations
- ‘Slow-to-warm-up infants’ were the ;east active, had mildly negative responses to new situations but slowly adapted over time.
Subsequently, the difficult infants were most likely to develop emotional and behaviour problems during childhood.
This study was admired but also criticised for relying on parents’ reports of their infants’ behaviour. Other researchers directly observed infants and identified temperamental styles that differed from those described above. Moreover, researchers often found that temperament is only weakly to moderately stable during infancy. Some infants maintain a consistent temperament during their first 2 years of life, whereas others change.
To what general temperament style does shyness form part of?
Behavioural inhibition. Inhibited infants are quiet and timid; they cry and withdraw when exposed to unfamiliar people, places, objects and sounds. Uninhibited infants are more sociable, verbal and spontaneous.
Research by Kagan et al. (1988) found that about 20-25% of infants displayed this inhibited, which remained moderately stable during infancy. They also studied these infants until age age 7.5 years. For the vast majority - those who were only mildly to moderately inhibited or uninhibited between the ages of 1 and 2 years - their temperament did not predict how shy or outgoing they would be as children. But for infants who were highly uninhibited or inhibited, the findings were different. Highly uninhibited infants tended to become sociable and talkative 7-year-olds, whereas highly imhibited infants developed into quiet, cautious and shy 7-year-olds
How does childhood personality continue into adulthood?
E.g. in America and Swedne, shy, behaviourally inhibited 8-12-year-old boys are more likely than other non-shy peers to delay marriage and fatherhood when they grow up, possibly reflecting their reluctance to enter new social relationships. Shy American girls are more likely as adults to leave work after marriage and become housewives, whereas shy Swedish girls are less likely to complete university than non-shy girls.
What is personality in adulthood argued to be?
A continuation of temperament in early childhood. Newman et al (1997) found that compared to 3-year-olds with a ‘well-adjusted temperament’, those who were ‘uncontrolled’ (i.e. irritable, impulsive & inattentive) reported more antisocial behaviour in adulthood and greater conflict in family and romantic relationships, and they were more likely to have been fired from a job. In contrast, children with an ‘inhibited temperament’ (i.e. socially shy and fearful) reported less overall companionship in adulthood
What individual differences are there in personality in the first year of life? (summary)
Most young children are well adjusted and display only mild to moderately strong temperamental traits. Differences in temperament among these children only weakly predict how they will function as adults. But for the remaining children, their strong temperamental traits can provide better insight into adulthood functioning, Still, predicting how any individual child will turn out as an adult is difficult. Many factors influence development and even during childhood, strong temperaments often mellow.
What are psychosocial stages?
Psychoanalytic psychologist Erik Erikson (196) believed that personality develops through confronting a series of 8 major psychosocial stages, each of which involves a different ‘crisis’ (i.e. conflict) over how we view ourselves in relation to other people and the world. Each crisis is present throughout life but takes on special importance during a particular age period.
What are Erikson’s psychosocial stages?
- Infancy (first year): Basic trust vs basic mistrust
- Toddlerhood (1-2): Autonomy vs shame and doubt
- Early childhood (3-5): Initiative vs guilt
- Middle childhood (6-12): Industry vs inferiority
- Adolescence (12-19): Identity vs role confusion
- Early adulthood (20-30): Intimacy vs isolation
- Middle adulthood (40-64): Generativity vs stagnation
- Late adulthood (65+): Integrity vs despair
What 4 of the psychosocial stages occur in infancy and childhood?
1) Basic trust vs basic mistrust: depending on how well our needs are met and how mcuh love and attention we receive during the first year of life, we develop a basic trust or basic mistrust of the world
2) Autonomy vs shame and doubt: during the next 2 years, children become ready to exercise their individuality. If parents unduly restrict children or make harsh demands during toilet training, children develop shame and doubt about their abilities and later lack the courage to be independent
3) Initiative vs guilt: from age 3 to 5, children display great curiosity about the world. If they are allowed freedom to explore and receive answers to their questions, they develop a sense of initiative. If they are held back or punished, they develop guilt about their desires and suppress their curiosity
4) Industry vs inferiority:from age 6 until puberty, the child’s life expands into school and peer activities. Children who experience pride and encouragement in mastering tasks develop industry - a striving to achieve. Repeated failure and lack of praise for trying leads to a sense of inferiority
What can be concluded about Erikson’s model?
Although critics argue that Erikson’s model lacks detail and question its stage approach, the model captures several major issues that developing children confront. As Erikson proposed and as some research supports, successfully resolving each crisis helps prepare us to meet the next. Because each stage of life creates new opportunities, possibilities for change are ever present. Yet, like the early chapters of a novel, themes that emerge in childhood help set the stage for the unfolding story of our lives
What social skills do newborns bring into the world with them?
Some of the most obvious communicative skills already discussed are smiling and crying. these let the observer know what emotional state the infant is in. Smiling occurs in the womb. After birth, newborns smile spontaneously, without reference to any specific environmental stimulus. However, by as early as 3 weeks of age infants begin smiling in specific situations (e.g. on eye contact). Crying is a vocal communication of distress, and often happens a few moments after arriving into the outside world. Crying is an effective communicative signal and, being very difficult for adults to ignore, frequently elicits response in the caregiver
What does the preference of looking at faces over other environmental stimuli by newborns suggest?
This preferential orienting behaviour directs infants’ attention towards people and, consequently, social situations. Research by Farroni et al. (2002) has demonstrated that newborn infants also prefer to look at faces making direct eye contact with them, as opposed to faces with averted gaze. So newborn infants are not just predisposed to look at people, but also to situations in which another person is communicating with (looking at) them. Csibra and Gergely (2006) have thus argued that these kinds of preferences represent an innate predisposition to learn about the world from others. An ethologist, has also argued that as adults we are programmed to encourage this learning with universal behaviours such as the ‘eyebrow flash’
How do young infants also demonstrate the origins of conversation in their behaviour?
Young infants’ vocalisations and motor behaviours are often described as showing ‘periodicity’or ‘burst-pause’ patterns. This means that they will vocalise or move a number of times in quick succession, stop for a period and then produce another burst of activity. A particularly early example of this can be seen in the context of breast-feeding. Rather than sucking continually throughout a feeding session, the infant will typically suck a number of times in quick succession, and then pause. This kind of periodicity in behaviour gives other people the chance to communicate back to the infant during the pauses, thus producing prelinguistic social interactions like these are initiated and controlled by the infant, or whether the parent or caregiver plays a more important role in scaffolding early conversations and interactions
How can early social interaction be seen in newborns?
Imitation behaviour. Meltzoff et al have presented evidence, alongside the imitation of adult faces by newborns, that newborns will imitate a range of non-emotional facial gestures. Researchers are divided about why infants should do this. Some suggest that it helps newborns understand others’ perspectives, while some argue that imitation helps us to learn new skills from others. More recently, a number of concerns have been raised about demonstrations of early imitation. Some have found it difficult to repeat all of Meltzoff and Moore’s (1997) original findings, and others argue that findings on early imitation indicate that this ability is learned rather than innate
What social abilities do newborn babies have? (summarise)
While there is some controversy at present over the extent to which newborn infants arrive in the world with innate social behaviours, it is clear that even if this is not the case infants quickly learn a wide repertoire of social behaviours. It it likely that the earliest of these interactions will be with their parents or caregivers, at least partly because these people are in closest proximity. However, as they age, infants begin to demonstrate strong preferences for interacting with particular individuals. These social bonds which are formed between infant and adult are referred to as attachment relationships
What is imprinting?
A sudden, biologically primed form of attachment (happens in some bird species e.g. ducks)
What is attachment?
The strong emotional bond that develops between children and their primary caregivers
What did John Bowlby propose about attachment?
he was influenced by ethological observations of imprinting to propose that a similar biologically programmed process occurs in humans. He argued that infants seek proximity with a caregiver, and that this is develops into an attachment relationship. Attachment refers to the strong emotional bond that develops between children and their primary caregivers. There are some differences with imprinting in geese, however, as human infants do not automatically imprint on a caregiver, and there is not an immediate post-birth critical period during which contact is required for infant-caregiver bonding. Instead, the first few years of life seem to be a sensitive period when we can most easily form a secure bond with caregivers that enhances our adjustment later in life. Although it may be more difficult to form strong first attachments to caregivers later in childhood or later in adulthood, it is still possible