Lecture 7 Flashcards
Describe which features lead people to confer agency to objects
Contingent responding: objects that respond appropriately to external stimuli, especially in a contingent manner, a often perceived as more agent-like
Goal-directed behaviour: objects exhibiting purposeful, goal-oriented actions are more likely to be attributed agency
Predictability: objects that behave predictably, following a pattern of actions that align with a goal, are seen as more intentional
Adaptability: the ability of an object to adapt its actions based on changes in the environment of stimuli enhances the perception of agency
Consistency: consistent and coherent behaviour over time contributes to the perception of agency
Describe which features are most central (e.g., contingent responidng)
Central feature:
Contingent responding: the feature is often considered the most central. Objects responding appropriately to external cues strongly influence the perception of agency
Describe which features are less informative (e.g., irregular path)
Less informative feature
Irregular path of travel: movements that lack a clear purpose or goal, such as irregular paths of travel, may be less informative and contribute less to the perception of agency
How easily do children identify objects as agents?
Developmental progression: children ability to identify objects as agents undergoes developmental progression
Early sensitivity: infants show early sensitivity to features like contingent responding and goal-directed behaviour
Gradual improvement: as children age, there is a gradual improvement in recognizing more complex signs of agency
Emergence of theory of mind: the development of theory of mind contributes to a more sophisticated understanding of objects agency
How easily do adults identify objects as agents
Efficient recognition: adults generally identify objects as agents efficiently
Integration of cues: adults integrate various cues, including contingent responding
Experience-based: past experience play a role in the quick and accurate recognition of agency
Higher level inferences: adults can make higher-level inferences about an object’s intentions and adaptability based on observed behaviour
Describe the implications of perceiving an object as agentic
Creating connections: treating objects as agentic helps us feel more connected to them. It’s like turning a regular objects into a friend or a playmate
Emotional bond: we start to attach emotions to objects. They can make us happy, comfort us when we’re sad, or even feel like they understand us
Enhanced play: objects become more than just things. They become part of our games and adventures, making playtime more enjoyable and imaginative
Influence on feelings: seeing objects as agentic can affect our emotions. We might feel different things depending on how we perceive the objects in that moment
Learning engagement: The idea of objects having agency can make learning more interesting. It’s like turning them into teachers or helpers in our learning journey
Interactive actions: we might interact with objects as if they can respond, like giving them high fives or imagining them participating in actions with us
Shared decision making: when objects have agency, we might consider what they would like or prefer. Its like involving them in decision, especially during play
Boosting courage: objects with agency can provide comfort or bravery, especially in moments of fear. It’s like having a supportive companion
Storytelling magic: our stories and imaginative play become more vibrant. Objects take on roles and contribute to exciting narratives when seen as agentic
Adventure companions: everyday objects transform into companions on our adventures. They add an extra layers of fun and excitement to our play experiences
In a nutshell, perceiving objects as agentic brings a touch of magic to our interactions. Its like unlocking a world where even ordinary things can become extraordinary friends in our imaginative journey
In what ways do we children make specical assumptions about social stimuli?
Face preference: from a very early age infants display a preference for faces. They often gaze longer at faces than non-face stimuli, suggesting an innate attraction to facial features
Social referencing: Children, even in infancy, tend to look to their caregivers for cues about how to interpret ambiguous situations. This social referencing helps them make sense of their environment by relying on the reactions of familiar individuals
Joint Attention: Infants engage in joint attention, where they share attention with others toward a common object or event. This is an early sign of social interaction and communication
Emotion recognition: Infants begin to recognize and respond to basic emotions in others, such as happiness, sadness, and fear. This ability contributes to early social bonding
Attachment behaviour: Attachment behaviours, like seeking proximity to caregivers when distressed, demonstrate the child’s reliance on specific individuals for comfort and security
Explain why the Woodward (1998) reaching experiment provides evidence that infants are encoding the underlying intentions of other’s actions
Main experiment: infants as young as 6 months showed a preference for looking at the object that the experimenter reached for when it was consistent with their prior preferences. This suggests that infants were sensitive to the underlying intentions guiding the experimenter’s actions
Why was the “new side” trial well-designed as a control condition?
“New side” Trial as control → control condition: The “new side” trial involved introducing a new object to one side of the actor, and the infants did not show a consistent preference. This trial served as a control condition because it ruled out the possibility that infants were simply attracted to the side where the action occurred. The lack of a consistent response in the “new side” trial strengthened the interpretation that infants were indeed responding to the actor’s intentions
What happened when infants were instead shown the same actions performed by a mechanical claw?
Mechanical claw control condition → mechanical claw experiment: when the same actions were performed by a mechanical claw instead of a human actor, infants did not show the same preference. This result suggested that the ability to attribute intentions was specific to human agents, highlighting the role of social cues in intentions understanding
Why was the mechanical claw a well-designed control condition?
Well-designed control → significance of mechanical control: the use of the mechanical claw as a control condition was crucial because it controlled for the visual and motor features of the actions while removing the social and intentional cues. The contrast between the infant’s responses to human actions and mechanical actions supported the idea that infants were specifically attributing intentions to human actors
Connecting to Lecture 10, what kinds of experiences facilitate children “passing?”
Facilitating “passing” → connection to lecture 10: the ability to “pass” such tasks and attribute intentions develops over time. Experiences involving social interactions, exposure to intentional actions, and social cues likely contribute to the development of infant’s ability to understand and attribute intentions. As infants gain more experience with observing and engaging in social interactions, they become more proficient in decoding other’s intentions
Explain the significance of pointing in ambiguous situations
Pointing: significant communicative gesture, especially in ambiguous situations where individuals seek clarification or share attention. Serves as a non-verbal cue to direct someone’s focus toward a specific object or event
Explain the significance of social referencing in ambiguous situations
Social referencing: involves seeking information from others in uncertain or ambiguous situations. individuals look to social cues, often from trusted others, to interpret and respond appropriately to a novel or ambiguous stimulus
How does pointing and social referencing connect to joint attention
Joint attention → example: a mother and her infant are playing with a toy. The mother looks at the toy, then looks at the infant, and back to the toy. The infant follows the mother’s gaze, establishing joint attention on the toy. This early from of joint attention is crucial for the development of shared attentional gesture
Pointing → example: a toddler sees a bird outside the window and points to it while looking at their caregiver. By pointing, the child directs the caregiver’s attention to the bird, establishing joint attention on the external stimulus
Social referencing → example: a child encounters a new and potentially frightening object in the environment. The child looks at their parents facial expression for cues on how to react. If the parent smiles and appears calm, the child may interpret the object as safe, facilitating social referencing in an ambiguous situation
How does pointing and social referencing facilitate children’s learning in at least two different situations?
Language acquisition → pointing: children often point to objects of interest, and caregivers, in turn, label those objects. This reciprocal interaction contributes to language acquisition as children associate words with the objects they are pointing to
Emotional regulation → social referencing: in situations of ambiguity or novelty, social referencing helps children regulate their emotions. By observing a caregiver’s emotional expression, a child may decide how to interpret and respond to a situation. This is a form of learning emotional cues from others
What was the difference between Repacholi & Gopnik (1997) and Kushnir et al. (2010)? What did each study find regarding children’s understanding of others’ desire?
Repacholi & Gopnik (1997)
Difference:
This study focused on investigating children’s understanding of desires and how it relates to emotional expressions. The primary emphasis was on the influence of emotional cues on attributing desires to others
Findings
They found that by the age of 18 months, children could use emotional expressions as cues to infer other’s desires. Specifically, they observed that children attributed different desires to characters based on the characters emotional reactions to a particular objects or event
Kushnir et al (2010)
Difference:
Kushnir et al. extended the exploration of children’s understanding of desires by investigating the role of probabilistic information. The study delved into how children use statistical sampling information to infer the preferences of others
Findings:
They discovered that children as young as 14 months could use statistical information to make inferences about other’s desires. The study presented scenarios where an individual consistently chose one object over another, and children were able to pick up on this statistical regularity to infer the person’s preference or desire
Repacholi & Gopnik (1997) focused on emotional cues and their impact on desire attribution, finding that by 18 months, children could use emotional expressions to infer others’ desires
Kushnir et al. (2010) extended this exploration by introducing probabilistic information, revealing that children as young as 14 months could utilize statistical sampling to infer the preferences or desires of others. Both studies contribute to our understanding of the early development of social cognition and the ability to attribute desires to others