Lecture 22 - Reconstructive Memory in the Real World Flashcards
real world memory
real world context outside the lab with more importance on what your remembering and the accuracy
Eyewitness testimony is a type of
episodic memory
misinformation effect
how episodic memory can be affected when you’re integrating incorrect info
flashbulb
things you feel are very accurate and “burned into your memory”
Which of the following best describes the relationship
between schemas and scripts:
Schemas are less structured than scripts and provide general information about a type of event.
contain many slots for values to be entered for a particular type of event
A schema is a….
….a general knowledge structure used for
understanding complex situations.
A schema is general for a ____ of situation, and helps give
______ to an specific event.
type
organizational structure
Schemas are more than a bunch of facts –
– they are structured to include the relationships between facts
scripts
much more detail
specific order of events within a schema
gives you causal (order) links: tells you how to move onto the next step in the script
Last time: Problems with schemas/scripts
Just as we use schemas to understand (encode) events, we
also use them later during recall.
- Source monitoring
- Sometimes we use stereotypes (a schema for a person) to reconstruct a memory.
• This can be very useful – as with all schemas you don’t need to
recall every detail, but can fill-in with defaults.
• However, this can (and does) lead to many errors.
source monitoring
when you try to ID where did that memory come from: is that what happened last time or the time before that? what is the source of the specific memory that has popped into my head?
is the process of determining the origin of some memory or belief.
stereotypes
systematic errors based on previous episodic info
source monitoring error.
When we attribute a remembered item to the wrong source
memory that popped into your head isn’t from last week it’s from 2 weeks ago
Marsh, Cook, & Hicks (2006) examined the
role of gender stereotypes on source
attributions.
• Subjects read statements (e.g. “I like
baseball.” or “I designed a center piece.”), that
were attributed to one of two people: Chris or
Pat.
• Sentences were masculine, feminine, or neutral.
• Subjects were given a puzzle (delay) and later
told the gender of the people (Chris is a
heterosexual male, Pat is a heterosexual
female).
• They then re-read the statements, and asked the source (who had said them).
• People were more accurate when the statements matched the stereotypes for masculine or feminine (schemas). => 83% of masculine statements were correctly attributed to the male name, but only 65% were correctly attributed to the female name.
Reconstructive memory
When recalling an event, we sometimes…
…. add elements that were not originally present.