EMOTION Flashcards
• Basic Emotions • Measuring & Manipulating Emotion in the Lab • Emotion & Attention/Memory • Emotion in the Brain
Emotion
– Subjective, conscious experience.
– Brief response to internal or external event.
– Facial expressions, biological reactions, mental states.
– Linked to arousal of nervous system.
Feelings
subjective representation of emotion
Moods
diffuse state; longer & less intense than emotions
Affect
broad term for emotion/feelings/moods
“affective neuroscience”
Basic Emotions: Darwin
Darwin (1859) proposed limited number of basic, universal human emotions.
Basic Emotions: Ekman
Ekman (1971): evidence for 6 basic emotional expressions • Anger • Disgust • Fear • Happiness • Sadness • Surprise
Anger + disgust = contempt
• These emotions are Universal;
how we convey these emotions through facial expression does not vary from culture to culture.
Dimensions of Emotion
Multidimensional scaling approach to classifying emotions
– Dimension 1 = valence (how negative or positive)
– Dimension 2 = arousal (intensity of reaction)
valence
how negative or positive
arousal
intensity of reaction
Measuring
Emotion
– Directly
• self-report
• direct assessment:
participants explicitly report their emotional
reaction, mood, or attitude.
• Drawbacks: Personal misinterpretation, lying
–Indirectly
• Indirect assessment: “Which do you like
more?” (happy face vs. sad face) – assumes emotion influences choice
• Inhibition or facilitation of a behavior (e.g., RT or eye movements)
• Psychophysiology: relationship between mental states and physiological responses
…..E.g., skin conductance response (autonomic nervous system arousal)
Directly Measuring
Emotion
• self-report
• direct assessment:
participants explicitly report their emotional
reaction, mood, or attitude.
• Drawbacks: Personal misinterpretation, lying
Indirectly Measuring
Emotion
• Indirect assessment: “Which do you like
more?” (happy face vs. sad face) – assumes emotion influences choice
• Inhibition or facilitation of a behavior (e.g., RT or eye movements)
• Psychophysiology: relationship between mental states and physiological responses
…..E.g., skin conductance response (autonomic nervous system arousal)
Manipulating
Emotion
• Mood induction: changing the baseline state reported by the participants via some procedure
– Show the participant affective film clips
– Play music
– Ask the participant to focus on affective situations, real or imagined
- Successful if the participant reports a shift of mood in the predicted direction.
- Use surveys to assess mood
- Emotionally evocative stimuli
Posner
cuing
The Posner Cueing Task (a.k.a, the Posner paradigm) is a neuropsychological test used to assess attention. Formulated by Michael Posner, the task assesses an individual’s ability to perform an attentional shift. It has been used and modified to assess disorders, focal brain injury, and the effects of both on spatial attention.
- Spatial Cue can be valid or invalid (location of target)
- Cue is face instead of plain location cue
- When invalid & emotional, Reaction Time is slower than invalid & neutral
- Emotion holds attention (takes longer to disengage)
Method
Posner’s spatial cueing task has been used to measure manual and eye-movement reaction times to target stimuli in order to investigate the effects of covert orienting of attention in response to different cue conditions.
In the general paradigm, observers are seated in front of a computer screen situated at eye level. They are instructed to fixate at a central point on the screen, marked by a dot or cross. To the left and the right of the point are 2 boxes. For a brief period, a cue is presented on the screen. Following a brief interval after the cue is removed, a target stimulus, usually a shape, appears in either the left or right box. The observer must respond to the target immediately after detecting it. To measure reaction time (RT), a response mechanism is placed in front of the observer, usually a computer keyboard which is pressed upon detection of a target. Following a set inter-trial interval, lasting usually between 2500 and 5000 ms, the entire paradigm is repeated for a set number of trials predetermined by the experimenter. This experimental paradigm appears to be very effective in recasting attentional allocation.
Endogenous and exogenous cues in the Posner Paradigm.
Cues
2 major cue types are used to analyze attention based on the type of visual input. An endogenous cue is presented in the center of the screen, usually at the same location as the center of focus. It is an arrow or other directional cue pointing to the left or right box on the screen. This cue relies on input from the central visual field. An exogenous cue is presented outside of the center of focus, usually highlighting the left or right box presented on the screen. An exogenous cue can also be an object or image in the periphery, a number of degrees away from the center, but still within the visual angle. This cue relies on visual input from the peripheral visual field.
Valid and Invalid Trials
Posner devised a scheme of using valid and invalid cues across trials. In valid trials, the stimulus is presented in the area as indicated by the cue. For example, if the cue was an arrow pointing to the right, the subsequent stimulus indeed did appear in the box on the right. Conversely, in invalid trials, the stimulus is presented on the side opposite to that indicated by the cue. In this case, the arrow pointed to the right (directing attention to the right), but the stimulus in fact appeared in the box on the left. Posner used a ratio of 80% valid trials and 20% invalid trials in his original studies. The observer learns that usually the cue is valid, reinforcing the tendency to direct attention to the cued side. Some trials do not present cues prior to presenting the target. These are considered neutral trials. Some tasks use neutral trials that do present cues. These neutral cues give the participant an idea as to when the target will appear, but do not give any indication of which side it is likely to appear on. For example, a neutral cue could be a double-sided arrow. The comparison of performance on neutral, invalid, and valid trials allows for the analysis of whether cues direct attention to a particular area and benefit or hinder attentional performance. Since the participant is not allowed to move their eyes in response to the cue, but remain fixated on the center of the screen, differences in reaction time between target stimuli preceded by these 3 cue conditions indicates that covert orienting of attention has been employed.
EmoNonal
stroop
task
– Name color of words (like regular stroop)
– Takes longer for emotional words (need to disengage)
– {DEMO}
– Does it correlate with emotional states/traits?
The emotional Stroop task is used as an information-processing approach to assessing emotions. Related to the standard Stroop effect, the emotional Stroop test works by examining the response time of the participant to name colors of negative emotional words. For example, depressed participants will be slower to say the color of depressing words rather than non-depressing words. Non-clinical subjects have also been shown to name the color of an emotional word (e.g., “war”, “cancer”, “kill”) slower than naming the color of a neutral word (e.g., “clock”, “lift”, “windy”).
While the emotional Stroop test and the classic Stroop effect elicit similar behavioral outcomes (a slowing in response times to colored words), these tests engage different mechanisms of interference. The classic Stroop test creates a conflict between an incongruent color and word (the word “RED” in the font color blue) but the emotional Stroop involves only emotional and neutral words—color does not affect slowing because it does not conflict with word meaning. In other words, studies show the same effects of slowing for emotional words relative to neutral even if all the words are black. Thus, the emotional Stroop does not involve an effect of conflict between a word meaning and a color of text, but rather appears to capture attention and slow response time due to the emotional relevance of the word for the individual. The emotional Stroop test has been used broadly in clinical studies using emotional words related to a particular individual’s area of concern, such as alcohol-related words for someone who is alcoholic, or words involving a particular phobia for someone with anxiety or phobic disorders. Both the classic and the emotional Stroop tests, however, involve the need to suppress responses to distracting word information, while selectively maintaining attention on the color of the word to complete the task.