EBP Flashcards

1
Q

Prevention

A

Action that prevent disease occurrence / slow disease progression / minimise impact of disease

Examples of clinical preventive interventions:

  1. Immunisation
  2. Behavioural counselling e.g. smoking cessation
  3. Screening
  4. Chemoprevention e.g. statins for CVS disease prevention
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Levels of prevention

A

Primary:

  • aim to ↓ incidence
  • targets ***risk factors / causes / disease in populations / individuals
  • e.g. immunisation

Secondary:

  • aim to ↓ prevalence
  • targets individuals with ***early disease
  • e.g. screening

Tertiary:

  • aim to ↓ complications
  • targets individuals with ***established disease
  • e.g. stroke rehabilitation

Primordial:

  • targets underlying ***social, economic, environmental determinants of health
  • accomplished through **public policies and **intersectoral action
  • e.g. control of air pollution

Quaternary:

  • identifies individuals and groups at risk of ***over-diagnosis / over-treatment
  • accomplished through actions that prevent iatrogenesis
  • e.g. evidence-based medicine
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Individual vs Population approach

A

Individual (high-risk) approach (only target individuals at high risk):

  • Intervention appropriate to individual —> high motivation for physician and subject
  • Screening has inherent diffulcties and costs —> only palliative, not radical
  • Limited potential for populations —> large number of people at small risk may outweigh small number of people at high risk

Population approach (lower risk for whole population):

  • Large potential for populations
    —> aims to shift whole distribution of exposure in a favourable direction (i.e. lower prevalence of disease of whole population: shifting the normal distribution curve)
    —> behaviourally appropriate; changes norms
  • Limited by ***prevention paradox
    —> individuals perceive little benefit from intervention even though measure may benefit population
    —> lower physician, subject motivation
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Screening

A

Presumptive identification of an unrecognised disease/defect by application of tests/examinations/other procedures which can be applied rapidly

  • Sort out apparently well (asymptomatic) people who probably have a disease from those who probably do not
  • NOT intended to be diagnostic
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Goals of screening

A

Early detection —> Early intervention —> Reduction in morbidity / mortality

  1. Reduction in morbidity / mortality from the newly identified disease among screened individuals due to ***early treatment
  2. Reduce disease burden for the population
  3. Provide cost-effective screening to target population
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Wilson and Jungner screening criteria

A

Knowledge of diseases:

  1. Condition should be ***important health problem
  2. There should be a ***recognisable latent / early symptomatic stage
  3. ***Natural history of condition, including development from latent to declared disease, should be adequately understood

Knowledge of test:

  1. ***Suitable tests / examinations
  2. Test ***acceptable to the population
  3. Case-finding should be ***continuous (NOT “once and for all”)

Treatment of disease:

  1. ***Accepted treatment for patients with recognised disease
  2. ***Facilities for diagnosis and treatment should be available
  3. ***Agreed policy on whom to treat as patients

Cost considerations:
1. Cost of case-finding (including diagnosis and treatment of patients diagnosed) should be ***economically balanced in relation to possible expenditure on medical care as a whole

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Assessing a screening test: Reliability and Validity

A

Reliability: degree to which results can be replicated
- depends on
—> Variability between people
—> Measurement error

Validity: degree to which results obtained by a test are true

  • Sensitivity
  • Specificity
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

***Sensitivity and Specificity

A

Used to decide whether a test should be used

Sensitivity (probability of true positive):
With disease + Positive test / Number of people with disease

Specificity (probability of true negative):
Without disease + Negative test / Number of people without disease

There will be trade-off between Sensitivity and Specificity
—> ↑ Sensitivity —> ↓ Specificity

Receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curve

  • used to help decide best cut-off point
  • Sensitivity plot against Specificity
  • maximising **True positive (Sensitivity), minimise **False positive (1-Specificity)
  • cut-off point usually at “shoulder” of ROC curve
  • ***Area under ROC curve: used to compare overall accuracy of tests
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

***Predictive values

A

Predictive values:

  • used to determine how likely a patient has / not has disease
  • ***Post-test probability
PPV = True positive / All positive —> ***↑ with prevalence of disease (given constant SN, SP) (∵ ↑ true positive)
NPV = True negative / All negative —> ***↓ with prevalence of disease (given constant SN, SP) (∵ ↓ true negative)

Sensitivity = True positive / All disease
—> ability to pick up all diseased people
—> high sensitivity
—> high NPV (very few false negative) (無就一定無, 有病就一定可以pick up到)
—> low type II error

Specificity = True negative / All healthy
—> ability to exclude normal people (無就一定無)
—> high specificity
—> high PPV (very few false positive) (有就一定有, 所有無病一定exclude)
—> low type I error

Use of sensitive test: correctly identify people WITH disease, rule out dangerous conditions (negative result tends to rule out diagnosis)
Use of specific test: correctly identify people WITHOUT disease, false positive results are highly undesirable (positive result tends to rule in diagnosis)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

***Bayes’ theorem

A

Prevalence of disease:
- reflects Pre-test probability (probability of disease before diagnostic test result is known)

Predictive values:
- reflects Post-test probability (probability of disease after diagnostic test)
—> ***influenced by Prevalence (Pre-test probability)

SN, SP:

  • ***Inherent characteristics of test
  • ***Independent of Pre-test portability / prevalence

Implications of Predictive values dependent on Prevalence:
1. Better estimate prevalence by using best available information sources
—> high quality surveillance data
—> epidemiological studies with minimal bias

  1. Increase Pre-test probability may increase PPV
    —> Apply diagnostic tests to more susceptible patients (use sparingly in primary care)
    —> Interpret results of studies with care (tertiary care have higher disease prevalence, cannot be applied to primary / secondary settings)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Risks of screening

A
  1. Discomfort and inconvenience
  2. Psychosocial consequences
    - Anxiety
    - Family discord on further procedures
  3. Adverse health outcomes
    - False-positives: possibly invasive diagnostic procedures
    - False-negatives: undetected disease
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Screening for colorectal cancer

A

Asymptomatic Hong Kong residents 50-75

- Faecal occult blood test every 1 to 2 years
OR
- Flexible sigmoidoscopy every 5 years
OR
- Colonoscopy every 10 years
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

PICO question

A

Patient/Population in question
Intervention being considered
Comparison group
Outcome

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

***Biases associated with screening

A
  1. Lead-time bias
    - Lead-time: Time between detection by screening and usual diagnosis
    - Apparently improved survival time with no change in time from disease onset to death
    - To avoid: Use ***mortality rather than survival rates
  2. Length-time bias
    - Slow-growing lesions are more likely to be detected by screening than fast-growing lesions
    - Screening tends to pick up slower growing lesions which are associated with better prognoses
    - To avoid: Compare outcomes in ***RCT
  3. Compliance bias
    - Individuals who are adherent to interventions usually have better prognosis than non-compliant individuals
    - tend to be more health conscious and higher socioeconomic position
    - To avoid: Compare outcomes in ***RCT
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Why studies showed that screening cannot save lives?

A
  1. Studies may be underpowered to detect small overall mortality benefit
  2. Any reductions in mortality may be offset by deaths due to ***downstream effects of screening
    - e.g. PSA screening yields numerous false-positive —> numerous prostate biopsies —> unnecessary harms and deaths

Misuse and Overuse of unnecessary tests/interventions:
Misuse: Interventions more harmful than beneficial
Overuse: Interventions that yield little useful information

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Appraising a study

A

Relevance:

  1. Is population similar to the patient?
  2. Intervention feasible?
  3. Comparison group reasonable?
  4. Outcome patient-oriented?

Validity (3 factors affecting):

  1. Random error
  2. Systematic error / bias
    - Selection bias
    - Information bias
  3. Confounding

Variations in responses:

  1. Participant variability
  2. Result of errors

Validity:

  1. Study sample
    - Selection bias (representative of target population?)
    —> inclusion / exclusion criteria reasonable?
    —> where were patients recruited?
    —> enrolled consecutively?
    —> response rates reasonable?
  2. Study design
    - Systematic reviews > individual studies
    - RCT for effectiveness of intervention
    - Prospective cohort studies for prognosis / etiology

For assessing prognosis:

  • is there an inception cohort? (group of patients assembled at onset of disease)
  • at the same point / stage in their illness?
  1. Study conduct
    - Selection bias (allocation to groups concealed? follow-up reasonably complete? drop-out rate differ between groups?)
    —> loss to follow-up —> reduce statistical power
    —> need >=70-80% patients follow up
    - Information bias (observer bias, recall bias)
    —> measurement of outcomes differ by exposure status?
    - Confounding (adjust for important potential confounders)
  2. Study results
    - Statistically significant (confidence intervals, P values, adequate power: 0.8 (sample size) for null associations)
    —> bigger effect size —> smaller sample size needed
    - Clinically significant (effects in clinical practice, relative risk reduction, absolute risk reduction, NNT)
    - How to apply results to patient
    - Adjusted for confounders
  3. Other factors that could affect interpretation of results
    - STEPS (Safety, Tolerability, Effectiveness, Price, Simplicity)
    - Conflicts of interest
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Bias

A
  1. Information bias
    —> Observer bias (by investigators)
    —> Recall bias (by subjects, particular problem if public is aware of exposure-disease association)
    - reduce bias:
    —> Blind assessment of outcome status (investigator blinded)
    —> Prospective assesssment of outcome (no need to recall)
    —> Use objective measures
  2. Selection bias:
    - systematic difference between subjects and target population
    - subjects not representative of target population —> affects EXTERNAL validity
    - subjects groups are not comparable —> affects INTERNAL validity
    - reduce bias:
    —> random sample
    —> maximise response rate
    —> minimise attrition rate
    —> select case and control from same underlying population
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

***Relative risk (RR)

A

Risk of benefit / harm of intervention compared to another
- depend only on relative difference and does not account for risk of no treatment (absolute risk at baseline)

EER (experimental event rate) = treated with effect / total treated
CER (control event rate) = control with effect / total control

Relative risk reduction (RRR) = (1-RR) x 100% OR ***CER-EER / CER

Absolute risk reduction (ARR) = Incidence in control group - Incidence in intervention group (i.e. CER-EER)

Number needed to treat (NNT) = 1 / ARR

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

***Number needed to treat (NNT)

A

NNT = 1 / ARR

  • Number of patients need to be treated for one patient to receive benefit/prevent disease
  • Ideal NNT = 1
  • take into account that not everyone benefit from an intervention
  • may be more useful than RR
    —> combines relative benefit with background risk of patients
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Prognosis

A

Likely course and outcome of disease / condition over time

Clinical course = prognosis of a condition that has received intervention
Natural history = prognosis of a condition that has NOT received intervention (determines when to intervene, disease staging etc.)

Types:

  1. Average / Overall prognosis: “most likely course of a condition”
  2. Prognostic factors: factors associated with outcome
  3. Risk prediction models: risk groups who are likely to have worse outcomes

Finding out:
1. Clinical experience (prone to bias)

(2. Systematic review of cohort studies)

  1. Cohort studies (exposure睇outcome)
    - **better evidence on prognosis
    - recruit based on **
    exposure characteristic:
    —> prognostic factor: exposed (e.g. smoking) vs not exposed (e.g. no smoking)
    - prospective —> look for ***outcome (morbidity / mortality)
  2. (Control arm of) RCT
    - treatment arm —> clinical course
    - control arm —> natural history
    - problem: participants may not be representative of population of interest
    - may give information on prognosis but ***participants may not be representative of population of interest
  3. Case-control studies (outcome睇exposure)
    - recruit based on **outcome:
    —> Cases
    —> Control
    - retrospective —> look for prognostic factors / **
    exposure
    - problem: does not provide information on outcome rates (∵ you select cases)
  4. Cross-sectional studies
    - cannot know temporal sequence
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Causation

A

Necessary cause: outcome would not occur without exposure
Sufficient cause: exposure guarantees cause will occur (very few in medicine)

E.g.

  • HIV is a necessary cause of AIDS (must have HIV to have AIDS)
  • but not a sufficient cause for AIDS (not everyone has HIV develop AIDS)

Finding cause:

  • Experimental studies e.g. RCT
  • Observational studies only tells ***associations

Bradford Hill’s Criteria

  1. Consistency (on replication)
  2. Strength (of association)
  3. Specificity
  4. Dose response relationship
  5. Temporality
  6. Biological plausibility
  7. Coherence
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

OA risk factors:

A

Non-modifiable:

  • age
  • sex
  • genetics
  • bone deformities

Modifiable:

  • obesity
  • joint injuries
  • occupational exposure
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Dealing with confounding

A
  1. Multivariable analysis
    - adjust / control for confounders
    - Regression analysis

Regression analysis:
- mathematical model to describe relationship between Dependent variable and ***>=1 Independent variables
- allow estimation of a range of exposures, each one adjusted for potential confounding effect of others
- describe relationship between exposure and outcome adjusted for effects of potential confounders
—> “all else being equal, what is effect of X on Y?”

Types of regression analysis:

  1. Logistic regression —> Binary outcome
  2. Linear regression —> Quantitive outcome
  3. Cox regression —> Time to occurrence of an event (e.g. survival)
    - ***effects of several variables on time to occurrence of an event
    - Hazard: instantaneous rate of the event at time t
    - Cox proportional hazards model assume that Hazard Ratios comparing different exposure groups remain constant over time
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Hazard ratio

A

Ratio of hazards between 2 groups
- interpreted in the same ways as risk ratio

HR>1: high hazard than comparison group
HR=1: no difference in hazard from comparison group
HR<1: lower hazard than comparison group

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Survival curve vs Survival rate

A

Survival curve:

  • Probability of survival starts at 100%, decreases over time
  • Shows proportion of study population that is still alive at successive time points
  • Patients are censored when lost from study

Survival rate:

  • alternative to survival curve
  • survival rates at different time points (1 year, 5 years), median survival rate, disease-free survival rate etc.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

Mortality

A

All-cause mortality

  • includes all causes of mortality, even those seemingly unrelated to study exposure
  • ***less prone to misclassification
Disease-specific mortality
- heavily relies on accuracy and completeness of death certification
—> immediate cause
—> intervening cause
—> underlying cause
- ***more prone to misclassification

—> intervention may just reduce one of the above (All-cause / Disease-specific mortality)

Doctors when issuing death certificates, causes of death must be consistent with those in ICD-10 (classification system by WHO)

Standardised mortality ratio (SMR):

  • Mortality of study group to Mortality of standard population
  • SMR =1: same mortality rate as standard population
  • SMR >1: higher mortality rate in study group
  • SMR <1: lower mortality rate in study group

(Sometimes 100 is used rather than 1)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

Diagnosis

A

Diagnostic tests:

  • Laboratory tests
  • Imaging
  • Endoscopy
  • History + Physical examination
  • combination of above

Diagnosis is a ***Probability based on:

  1. Frequency of disease:
    - Prevalence (more commonly available)
    - Incidence
  2. Index of suspicion:
    - Risk factors / risk predictors: age, smoking status etc.
    —> Effect measures: Odds ratios, Risk ratios
    - Careful history and clinical judgement
  3. Accuracy and validity of diagnostic tests
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

Screening vs Diagnosis

A

Screening:
- for Asymptomatic patients with early disease
—> secondary prevention
—> lower prevalence of disease
- Aims to identify people with high risk of disease where interventions may reduce morbidity / mortality
- NOT intended to diagnose

Diagnosis:
- for Symptomatic patients
—> higher prevalence of disease
- Aim: make a definitive diagnosis / narrow down DDx
- NOT intended to prevent disease
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

Issues with determining validity

A
  1. An appropriate “gold standard” (reference standard)
  • best available test to determine presence / absence of disease
    —> new test should not be compared to imperfect test
  • sometimes too invasive / costly
    —> simpler tests should only be used as proxies when risk of misclassification is low
  • disease may lack objective standards for diagnosis e.g. mental disorders
  1. Lack of information on negative tests
    - clinicians reluctant to further test when initial test is negative
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

***Likelihood Ratio (LR)

A

Describes performance of a test:
LR = probability (test results if disease is present) / probability (test result if disease is absent)

LR+ = P(+ve test in diseased) / P(+ve test in normal people)
—> Sensitivity / 1-Specificity
—> ***high LR+ —> better the positive test can rule in disease

LR- = P(-ve test in diseased) / P(-ve test in normal people)
—> 1-Sensitivity / Specificity
—> ***lower LR- —> better the negative test can rule out disease

LR = 1
—> no change in likelihood of disease —> not a useful test

Use:
1. Allow application to individuals with different **pre-test probabilities:
- ∵ Predictive values are not transferable with different prevalence, but LR still applicable even though prevalence is low
- **
Pre-test odds x LR = Post-test odds
(need to convert probability to odds)

  1. Summarise test performance for a range of possible ***test values
    - rather than SN and SP defined by a single cut-off point
    - disease should be more likely for extreme than borderline test results
31
Q

Multiple tests

A

Parallel testing (囊括越多人) —> ↓ False negative:

  • ↑ SN and NPV
  • but may ↑ false positive
  • clinical prediction rules may be used to combine multiple test results and pre-test probabilities

Serial testing (逐漸縮窄範圍) —> ↓ False positive:

  • ↑ SP and PPV
  • but may ↑ false negative
  • serial LR may be calculated
32
Q

Potential harms of diagnostic tests

A
  1. Diagnosis does not necessarily lead to improvement in outcomes
  2. Hazards of inappropriate diagnosis
    - false positive results
    - false negative results
    - overdiagnosis
  3. Overuse / Misuse of diagnostic test is waste of resources
33
Q

Therapy / treatment

A

Any intervention intended to improve course of established disease

May include:

  • medications
  • surgery
  • changes in organisation / financing of health care
34
Q

Observational vs Interventional studies

A

Observational studies:

  • Examine distributions and determinants of outcomes (observe exposed vs not exposed)
  • Without any attempt to influence them
  • Easier to conduct, less expensive
  • ***Residual confounding problem —> should equally allocate confounders on 2 side
    1. Descriptive (cross-sectional, cohort)
    2. Analytical (cross-sectional, cohort, case-control)

Interventional / Experimental studies (e.g. clinical trials):

  • Modify an exposure within a population (observe intervention group vs control group)
  • Examine its effect on outcome
  • More time-consuming, costly
  • ***Randomisation minimise confounding
  • ***Selection bias, Information bias problem

RCT:

  • gold standard study design to provide evidence for effectiveness due to randomisation
  • participants randomly allocated to different intervention group to minimise confounding
  • followed up prospectively for assessment of outcomes
35
Q

Level of evidence

A

Highest —> Lowest:

  • Evidence summaries
  • Meta-analysis / Systematic review
  • RCT
  • Cohort studies
  • Case-control / Cross-sectional / Ecological studies
  • Expert opinion
36
Q

Elements of clinical trials

A
  1. Ethics
  2. Intervention
  3. Control / Comparison
  4. Outcomes / End-points
37
Q
  1. Ethical principles
A
  1. Equipoise:
    - randomisation is ethical when either of the allocated interventions is better than the other
  2. Respect for rights of individual participants:
    - life, health, dignity, integrity, self-determination, privacy, confidentiality
    - informed consent
    - voluntary participation
    - specific consideration for vulnerable groups
  3. Minimising risks:
    - measures to assess, monitor, minimise risk to human subjects
  4. Research ethics committee:
    - research protocol should be submitted for consideration and approval
    - right to monitor ongoing studies, esp. for serious adverse events
  5. Reporting and dissemination of results:
    - registration in publicly available database
    - full reporting + dissemination of results
38
Q
  1. Intervention considerations
A
  1. Feasibility:
    - some interventions may not be available in all settings
  2. Complexity:
    - treatment plan usually involve multiple interventions (will an intervention work with others?)
  3. Effect size:
    - relative effects may exaggerate absolute effects (does the intervention has a clinically significant effect)
39
Q
  1. Control group
A
  1. No treatment
    - Hawthorne effect: participants may change their behaviours even without interventions (modify an aspect of their behavior in response to their awareness of being observed)
  2. Placebo
    - intervention intended to be indistinguishable from active treatment
    - intended to have no active effect
    - Placebo effect: report improvement even with placebo
  3. Usual care / another intervention
    - ***meaningful if this is the most effective treatment available (i.e. “gold standard”)
40
Q
  1. Outcomes / End-points
A
  1. Primary
    - outcome investigators are most interested in
    - basis for ***hypothesis testing + sample size calculation
  2. Secondary
    - additional outcomes of interest
    - associations are only ***hypothesis-generating as may have occurred by chance
    —> study may not have enough power to detect these associations
    —> interpret with caution
    - e.g. SE, differences by subgroups
  3. Hard
    - defined according to **objective criteria, involves **no subjective assessment
    - usually preferred over soft outcomes
    - e.g. All-cause mortality
  4. Soft
    - require subjective assessment by investigator / participants
    - potential ***information bias —> blinding is important
    - e.g. Cause-specific mortality
  5. Patient-oriented
    - focus on helping patients live longer and better
    - ***preferred over disease-oriented outcomes
    - e.g. mortality, morbidity, QOL
  6. Disease-oriented
    - focus on pathophysiological mechanisms / markers of disease
    - not preferred but ***easier to measure
41
Q

Types of clinical trials

A

Function:
- Prevention / Diagnosis / Treatment

Design:

  • Cluster vs Individual
  • Randomised vs Non-randomised
  • Parallel-group vs Cross-over
  • Superiority vs Non-inferiorly / Equivalence

Objective:

  • Explanatory (efficacy) vs Pragmatic (effectiveness)
  • Phases 1 to 4
42
Q

Superiority vs Non-inferiority trials

A

Superiority trials

  • aim to establish that a new treatment is BETTER than another
  • e.g. a new drug more effective than usual care

Non-inferiority trials
- aim to establish that a new treatment is NOT WORSE (not clinically inferior) than current / control treatment by more than a non-inferiority margin
—> smallest difference in effect considered to be clinically important
—> control treatment must be more effective than placebo
- one-sided
- Equivalence trials: 2-sided (new treatment is not better not worse than current treatment)
- e.g. a new drug with similar effectiveness is safer, cheaper, easier to administer
- conducted when new treatment is ***not expected to be better than control in terms of primary health outcomes, but may offer other benefits

***Non-inferiority is only shown if 95% CI for different between test and reference treatments are > -Δ (過左-Δ —> 代表同之前一樣; 過曬-Δ —> 差過之前)

Non-inferiority: CI well below non-inferiority threshold
Superiority: CI well above non-inferiority threshold

43
Q

Explanatory (efficacy) vs Pragmatic (effectiveness) trials

A

Explanatory (efficacy) trials:
- Can treatment help under **ideal circumstances (e.g. RCT)?
- include highly selective and homogeneous patients who are adherent
—> higher internal validity
—> may have lower external validity / generalisability
—> limitations: **
selection bias

Pragmatic (effectiveness) trials:
- Can treatment help under ***ordinary circumstances?
- done after efficacy is established
- done in routine health care settings
- include more diverse patients
—> participants not followed as closely to ensure adherence
—> ***higher external validity
—> may have lower internal validity
44
Q

Clinical trial phases

A

Phase 1:

  • safety + dosage
  • 20-100 participants over several months
  • “is it safe in health people?”

Phase 2:

  • efficacy + SE (i.e. explanatory trials)
  • several hundred participants over months - 2 years
  • “does it work?”

Phase 3:

  • efficacy / effectiveness + adverse reactions (i.e. pragmatic trials)
  • hundred to thousands of participants over 1-4 years
  • “is it any better than current treatment?”

Phase 4:

  • post-marketing surveillance of long-term benefits, risks
  • several thousand participants
  • “will it have any long term results?”
45
Q

***Limitations of clinical trials

A
  1. Confounding
    - ***Randomisation —> randomly distribute confounders in intervention / control groups
  2. Selection bias
    - Healthy volunteer bias
    —> participants may be healthier than average patients —> need to note how ***sampling was done
  • Cherry-picking participants
    —> assign healthier participants to desired treatment group —> need to ensure ***allocation concealment
  • Attrition
    —> participants who drop out may not be representative of all enrolled participants —> need to note follow-up rates and whether ***intention to treat analysis was done
  1. Information bias
    - ***Blinding
  2. Practical limitations
    - time-consuming
    - cost
    - not feasible / ethical interventions
46
Q

Randomisation

A
  • Randomly distribute confounders in intervention / control groups
  • Bad examples:
    —> assignment based on day of admission (not random)
    —> assignment based on day of birth (not concealed)
  • Non-randomised studies: overestimate / underestimate effect of intervention
  • ***Check if baseline characteristics of participants are similar across groups
    —> if sample size is small —> baseline differences are even exaggerated
    —> significance tests of baseline differences (giving P values) may not be very useful

Types of randomisation

  1. Unrestricted (simple) randomisation
    - no constraints in generation of random allocation sequence (e.g. a table of random numbers)
  2. Restricted randomisation
    - ensure particular allocation ratios to treatment groups (e.g. 1:1)
47
Q

Sampling

A

Meet strict criteria for participation

Inclusion criteria:

  • diagnostic criteria to ensure patients have the condition under study
  • patients with unusual, mild / equivocal manifestations of disease may be excluded

Exclusion criteria:
- usually involve patients who have
—> comorbidities that may affect outcomes
—> limited life expectancy that may affect assessment of outcomes
—> contraindications to treatment
—> do not participant
—> do not cooperate in early stages of trial

However, problem of highly selective:
- Limit ***external validity / generalisability

Example of sampling:
Sampled population —> Invited to participate —> Able to contact and agreed to participate —> Eventually randomised

48
Q

Allocation concealment

A

Ensure investigators do not know / have no influence over which group participants are allocated to
—> minimise selection bias

Low risk of bias:

  • opaque sealed envelopes containing group assignment
  • central randomisation by a third party

High risk of bias:

  • allocation based on day of admission
  • allocation sequence posted on staff room wall

Inadequate allocation concealment yield ***larger effect estimates when differences in results did occur

49
Q

Incomplete outcome data

A

Missing outcome data may:

  1. Bias the observed effects
    - esp. when % missing differ across groups
    - patients who drop out / missing data may have worse prognosis
  2. Reduce statistical power
    - esp. when % missing is high

Attrition / Drop out because:

  • withdrawal (due to SE)
  • do not attend follow up (busy)
  • do not provide relevant data
  • cannot be contacted (migration / death)
  • later found to be ineligible
  • investigators ceased to follow up (inadequate funding)
50
Q

Non-adherence

A

Extent to which patients fail to follow treatment recommendations / take medications as prescribed

Patient factors: illness, perceived benefits and risks
Treatment factors: cost, complexity, duration, SE
Other factors: poor doctor-patient communication

Measuring adherence in clinical trials:
- cheaper but less accurate
—> self-report, diaries, pill counts, prescription refills, third party observations
- more accurate but more costly
—> measuring drug levels in plasma, directly observed therapy

Dealing with non-adherence:

  1. Exclude non-adherent patients during run-in period
    - patients given placebo and observed for adherence —> non-adherence patient then excluded before randomisation
  2. Intention-to-treat analysis
    - analyse study outcomes in all participants according ***original assigned treatment group

Advantages:
- perserves randomisation (reduces confounding)
—> provide more conservative estimate of effect
—> selection bias may still exist if outcome data incomplete

  • more accurately reflects what happens in reality
  • **different from per protocol analysis: include participants only if they received the intended intervention in accordance with the study protocol
    —> may **
    overestimate treatment effect due to confounding (lose randomisation)
    —> may be done alongside ITT analysis to investigate influence of missing data
51
Q

Blinding

A

Keeping people unaware of which intervention is administered to which participants
—> prevent ***information bias

Done on various levels

  1. Participants
  2. Care providers
  3. Assessors of outcomes (most important, always feasible)
    - non-blinded outcome assessors tend to ***overestimate effect of intervention

Types of blinding
1. Single-blind, double-blind, triple-blind
2. Open-label
—> usually assumes no attempt at blinding
3. Placebo-controlled
—> usually assumes that participants are blinded

Lack of blinding may have more influence on subjective outcomes than objective outcomes

52
Q

Allocation concealment vs Blinding

A

Occur at different timeline

Allocation concealment: Prevent **selection bias
Blinding: Prevent **
information bias

53
Q

CONSORT statement

A

Consolidation of Standards of Reporting Trials
- specifies content to be covered when reporting RCT e.g. Full trial protocol, to be repeated
—> help us assess quality of reporting
- consist of:
1. Checklist
2. Flow diagram (showing passage of subjects through RCT)
- enrolment —> allocation —> follow-up —> analysis

54
Q

Harm to patient safety in medical practices

A
  • Only 1/3 medical practices = Beneficial / Likely to be beneficial
  • ~20% expenditure wasted on unnecessary diagnostic tests / treatments

Overuse: interventions that yield little useful information
Misuse: interventions that may be more harmful than beneficial

Hospital:

  1. Unsafe use of medication (Leading cause)
  2. Healthcare-associated infections
  3. Surgical complications

Primary care:

  1. Administrative errors
  2. Diagnostic errors

Combatting Overuse / Misuse:

  1. Choose wisely
  2. Prudent healthcare
  3. NICE
55
Q

Measures of patient safety

A

Hospital-acquired infection:

  • Ventilator pneumonia
  • Wound infection
  • Decubitus ulcer (i.e. Bedsore)

Operative / Post-op complications:

  • Complication of anaesthesia
  • Post-op sepsis
  • Post-op DVT

Sentinel events:

  • Transfusion reaction
  • Wrong surgical site
  • Foreign body left

Obstetrics:
- Birth trauma

Other care-related adverse effects:

  • Falls
  • Fractures
56
Q

Unsafe care

A
Structures
- Resources and organisational arrangements in place that influence care
—> Safety culture
—> Inadequate Human Resources
—> Stress and fatigue
Processes
- Activities of physicians and other providers of healthcare
—> Misdiagnosis
—> Poor test follow-up
—> Inadequate measures of patient safety

Outcomes
- Results / Consequcnes of clinical activities
—> Adverse events due to medications
—> Surgical complications

Example:
Adverse events to drug treatment
1. Health system level:
- lack necessary information for safe use of drug
- error-prone conditions: look-alike / sound-alike medication

  1. Provider level:
    - lack of knowledge about drug
    - lack of follow-up
    - fail to recognise drug SE
    - fail to prescribe drug
57
Q

Types of medical errors

A
  1. Preventive
    - fail to provide prophylactic treatment
    - inadequate monitoring to follow-up
  2. Diagnostic
    - delay in diagnosis
    - fail to use indicated tests
    - use of inappropriate tests
    - fail to act on test results
  3. Treatment
    - error in administering drug
    - error in procedure / operation
    - delay in treatment
    - inappropriate treatment
58
Q

Progress in patient safety

A
  • Little evidence that patient outcomes improved despite research
  • Investment in safety research is much lower than magnitude of problem
  • Medical errors are caused by faculty system, processes / conditions that lead people to make mistakes rather than result from individual recklessness
59
Q

Ethical issues in clinical research

A
  1. Respect for rights of individuals participants
    - informed consent
    - confidentiality
    - specific consideration for vulnerable groups
  2. Minimising risk
    - measures in place to minimise risk
  3. Research ethics committee
    - approve research protocol
    - monitor ongoing studies esp. adverse effects
  4. Reporting and dissemination fo results
    - registration in publicly available database
    - full reporting and dissemination of results

Ethical issues concerning patient safety research:

  1. Subject may be system / healthcare provider rather than a patient
  2. Tensions may arise between a researcher’s duty of care and aims of research
  3. Liability of health care providers when health care failures are documented
  4. Patient confidentiality
  5. Dissemination of findings
60
Q

Study designs

A

Observational (e.g. Cohort studies):
- Examine outcome without attempt to influence them —> too see Prognosis

Interventional (e.g. RCT):
- Modify an exposure and examine outcome —> measure effectiveness

61
Q

Hierarchy of evidence

A
Evidence summaries
—> Systematic reviews / Meta-analysis
—> RCT
—> Cohort studies
—> Case-control studies
—> Expert option / Consensus
—> Pathophysiologic reasoning
62
Q

Systematic reviews (i.e. Synthesis review)

A
  • Review of all available evidence on a specific topic
  • Apply **Systematic, **Explicit strategies that limit bias
    —> Assembly, Critical appraisal and Synthesis of evidence of all relevant studies
    —> provide more reliable evidence than individual studies
    —> identify gaps in existing evidence
63
Q

Process of systematic reviews

A

Literature search, Selection of studies, Data extraction
—> done by **>=2 **independent reviews to limit ***selection bias
—> should declare conflicts of interests (financial / intellectual conflicts of interest)

Steps:
1. Ask an answerable clinical question
2. Define inclusion + exclusion criteria
3. Develop search strategy + Identify relevant information sources
- Search strategy:
—> Determined by Eligibility criteria of studies for a specific research question —> PICO (Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcome)
—> Information sources: Databases (PubMed), Grey literature (Reports by government agencies), Bibliography
—> Search terms: plus different combination
—> Search filters: time period, language, document format

  1. Select relevant studies
  2. Extract relevant data
  3. Explicitly and systematically assess quality (i.e. risk of bias) of individual studies:
  4. Synthesise results from individual studies
    - Meta-analysis
64
Q

Systematic reviews vs Narrative reviews

A

Systematic review:

  • Systematic + Explicit methods to identify, appraise and synthesise evidence
  • 2 independent reviewers —> less selection bias
  • May involve Meta-analysis

Narrative review (Summary review):

  • NO systematic / explicit methods to identify, appraise and synthesise evidence —> ***Uncertain validity
  • **Content experts who pick and choose relevant studies —> **prone to selection bias
  • NOT involve Meta-analysis
65
Q
  1. Explicitly and systematically assess quality (i.e. risk of bias) of individual studies
A
1. Use Explicit criteria
should include:
- Confounding
- Selection bias
- Information bias

Checklists:
- Jadad score
—> evaluate **quality of reporting of RCT (評核三方面)
—> Randomisation
—> Blinding
—> Account of all withdrawals and dropouts
- **
NOT include allocation concealment

  • Newcastle-Ottawa Quality assessment scale:
    —> for Case-control studies, Cohort studies
    —> Selection, Comparability, Exposure
  1. Sensitivity analysis
    - to assess potential influence of studies at high risk of bias —> repeating analysis by excluding them
    - many arbitrary assumptions are involved in a meta-analyses:
    —> Case definitions
    —> Study designs
    —> Study quality
    —> Type of analysis etc.
    - involve testing of different assumptions —> ***robustness of findings to different assumptions suggests that results from the meta-analysis are valid
66
Q

EQUATOR network

A

Reporting guidelines for research studies according to study design

  • recommend ***required reporting items for systematic reviews
  • can be used as a framework to evaluate ***quality of reporting
  1. PRISMA: systematic reviews
    - Identification
    - Screening
    - Eligibility
    - Included
  2. CONSORT: RCT
  3. STROBE: observational studies
  4. STARD: diagnostic / prognostic studies
  5. AGREE: clinical practice guidelines
67
Q

Reporting biases

A

When dissemination of research findings is influenced by nature of direction of results

  1. Publication bias —> positive results more likely to be published
  2. Time lag bias —> positive results published sooner
  3. Language bias —> English studies more likely found
  4. Citation bias —> positive results more likely to be cited
  5. Selective outcome reporting bias —> outcomes with favourable associations more likely to be reported

解決方法: Funnel plot

68
Q

Funnel plot

A

Scatter plot of **effect estimates (OR, RR) from individuals studies against **measure of study size

  • Dotted vertical line: Combined effect
  • Solid vertical line: Null effect
  • Diagonal lines: Triangular region with which 95% of studies are expected to lie without bias / heterogeneity

Precision of effect estimate ***↑ with larger sample sizes

  • smaller studies scatter more widely around bottom of funnel
  • “small-study effect”: tendency for smaller studies to show larger effects

Interpretation:
Plot should be symmetrical in the absence of bias / heterogeneity between studies
—> 越高(越大sample size) —> 結果(effect estimate)應該越中庸之道 —> 三角形
—> scatter of studies only due to ***sampling variation

  • Asymmetry could be due to
    1. **
    Reporting bias
    2. **
    Heterogeneity
    3. Chance —> statistical tests (e.g. Egger test) help to determine likelihood

Funnel plot ONLY suggest potential biases but do NOT address them

69
Q
  1. Synthesise results from individual studies: Meta-analysis
A

Statistical method to **quantitatively **combine results from individual studies
- Usually a ***weighted average of estimates from individual studies

Increase overall ***statistical power of individual studies
—> increases chance of detecting small but potentially important effects

Should only be done when studies are reasonably ***homogeneous (i.e. results from individual studies are reasonably similar / consistent)

70
Q

Forest plot

A
  • Graphical summary of results from individual studies and meta-analysis
  • Aid in ***identification of heterogeneity
  • Solid vertical line: Null line
  • Horizontal line: 95% confidence interval
  • Square:
    —> Centre: **Mean effect
    —> Size: Proportional to **
    % weight of study
  • Diamond (Only一粒): **Combined result
    —> Centre + Dotted vertical line: **
    Pooled estimate
    —> Horizontal tips: ***95% confidence interval

How to identify heterogeneity:
—> Use statistical tests —> when there is poor overlap of confidence intervals
1. I^2 statistic
- estimated proportion of variability that is explained by heterogeneity rather than sampling variation
- ***I^2 >30-40% usually suggests significant heterogeneity

  1. Cochran’s Q (Chi^2) statistic
    - weighted sum of squared differences between individual study effects and pooled effect
    - has low power; ***P value <0.1 suggests significant heterogeneity
  2. ***Identify potential sources of heterogeneity
71
Q

Heterogeneity

A
  1. Clinical diversity
    - variability in participants, interventions, outcomes studied
  2. Methodological diversity
    - variability in study design / risk of bias
72
Q

Dealing with heterogeneity

A
  1. Meta-analysis may not be appropriate when studies too heterogeneous
    —> combined estimate not a meaningful representation of studies
  2. ***Random effects model in meta-analysis
    —> assumes that there are different underlying effects between studies other than sampling variation
    —> Fixed effect model: assume that one true effect underlie all studies and that all differences in observed effects are due to sampling error (does not account for heterogeneity between studies)
73
Q

Strengths and Limitations of Systematic reviews

A

Strengths:

  1. Increase statistical power of individual studies to detect small but important effects
  2. Explicit and more objective summary of evidence compared to narrative reviews

Limitations:

  1. Limited by quality of individual studies
  2. Reporting biases
  3. Meta-analysis may not be appropriate when studies are too heterogenous
74
Q

Summary

A
  1. Relevance:
    - PICO
  2. Validity:
    - study design —> Observation / RCT
    - study conduct —> Search strategy, Selection bias, Validity / Risk of bias (Confounding, Selection bias, Information bias), Reporting bias (Asymmetry), Meta-analysis (Heterogeneity)
    - study results —> Statistically significant, Clinically significant, STEPS, Conflict of interest

STEPS:

  1. Safety
  2. Tolerability
  3. Effectiveness
  4. Price
  5. Simplicity