Core study five Moray-cognitive area Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

Background

A

Attention is a limited resource. When our attention is focused on certain things, a ‘barrier’ is put up that stops us from focusing on other things.

Cherry (1953) was interested in how people put up an inattentional barrier at a party with multiple conversations going on.
This is where you only listen to the conversation you’re participating in and not the conversations around you.
The cocktail party effect is when this barrier can be broken only by the sound of your name.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Cocktail party effect

A

The concept originally suggested by Cherry in which we would hear it when our own name is said within a crowded room

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Dichotic listening

A

When headphones are worn by a participant and a different message is played to each ear

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Shadowing

A

When a participant is told to focus on a passage of text and repeat it out
loud as they hear it

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Affective instruction

A

When a person is asked to do something,
preceded by their name being said

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Non-affective instructions

A

When a person is asked to do something, but their name is not used

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Overall Aims (experiment one aim)

A

To test Cherry’s findings on the intentional barrier more thoroughly.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Experiment one sample

A

Undergraduate students
Both genders
From Oxford University

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Experiment one procedure

A

Participants had to shadow a piece of prose that they could hear in one ear. This is the attended message because participants were focusing on it.
In the other ear (the message that they were NOT paying attention to) a list of simple words was repeated 35 times. This is the rejected message.
At the end of the task, participants completed a recognition task. Participants had to indicate what they recognised from a list of 21 words (7 from the shadowed passage, 7 from the rejected passage and 7 similar words that were in neither passage).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Results of experiment one

A

Shadowed passage: Participants recognized on average 4.9/7 words from the shadowed passage
Rejected passage: Participants recognised on average 1.9/7 words from the rejected passage
Similar words: Participants recognised on average 2.6/7 words that were in neither passage

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Conclusions of experiment one

A

Participants are much more able to recognise words from the shadowed passage. Almost none of the words from the rejected message are able to break the ‘inattentional barrier’.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Aim of experiment two

A

This experiment wanted to find out if an affective cue (their name) would break the inattentional barrier.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Sample of experiment two

A

12 Undergraduate students
Both genders
From Oxford University

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

IV of experiment two

A

Affective instructions- (When a person is asked to do something, preceded by their name being said) or a non-affective- (instruction When a person is asked to do something, but their name is not used.)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

DV of experiment two

A

Whether participants were more likely to hear an instruction in a message they’re not paying attention to if it is preceded by their name. (either by reporting so or following the instruction).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Procedure of experiment two

A
  1. Passages- Participants heard 10 passages of light fiction including both affective and non-affective instructions (repeated measures)
  2. Instructions- Participants were told to either change ear or to stop. They were told to make as few errors as possible.
  3. Order- The instructions were at the start and/or the end of the passage
  4. Controls- Passages were ready at a steady monotone with a pace or 130 words per minute by a single male voice
17
Q

Results of experiment two

A

Affective instructions- Participants heard/followed the instructions preceded by their name 20/39 times

Non-affective instructions- Participants heard/followed the instructions not preceded by their name 4/36 times

18
Q

Conclusions of experiment two

A

Affective messages (such as names) are able to break the ‘inattentional barrier’. This backs up the previous work by Cherry.

19
Q

Reflections from experiment two

A

Warning-Participants were given a warning in passages 8 and 10.
Effect- There was a slight increase in how many times the rejected message instruction was heard.
Interested- Moray was interested whether pre-warning could help break the inattentional barrier

20
Q

Aim of experiment three

A

This experiment wanted to find out if pre-warning would help neutral material break the inattentional barrier.

21
Q

Sample of experiment three

A

28 Undergraduate students
Both genders
From Oxford University
Split into 2 groups of 14

22
Q

Independent variable of experiment three

A

Warning (Participants were told they should memorise as many digits they heard as possible.) or no warning (Participants were told they’d be asked questions at the end about the shadowed passage.)

23
Q

Dependent Variable of experiment three

A

How many digits the participants were able to recall from the rejected message.

24
Q

Experiment three procedure

A

Participants were asked to shadow one message
The messages sometimes contained digits towards the end
The digits were sometimes only in the shadowed passage, sometimes only in the rejected passage, sometimes in both and sometimes there were no digits (control)

25
Q

Results of experiment three

A

There was no significant difference between the groups in how many digits they were able to recall from the rejected passage.

26
Q

Conclusions of experiment three

A

Warnings do not help neutral information break the inattentional barrier. The information must be meaningful in order to do this.

27
Q

Overall conclusions

A

Almost none of the verbal content from a rejected message penetrates a block when attending to another message.

‘Important’ messages like names can penetrate the barrier.

A short list of simple words cannot be remembered even when repeated several times.

It is difficult to make ‘neutral’ material (e.g. digits) important enough to break the inattentional barrier.

28
Q

Validity

A

Internal validity or construct validity: Controlled (e.g passages used, pace and voice of speaker, recognition task in experiment 1) BUT results may have been down to understanding of the passages, hearing ability etc and not attention

Population validity: All students from a single area and occupation BUT both males and females

Ecological validity: Realistic to hear multiple conversations at once BUT not isolated, wearing headphones and tested afterwards

29
Q

Reliability

A

Internal reliability: The internal reliability is high as a standardised produce was used because the same monotone voice (male) at the same pace. They shadowed the same passage this means that the produce was standardised as consistent across all participants.

External reliability: The study had low external reliability because there was a small number of participant’s in experiment one and two there was twelve participants and in experiment three it was fourteen people per condition. The sample was too small to establish consistent effects could be a fluke.

30
Q

Ethics

A

Informed consent: Students had the tasks clearly explained prior to participation, they knew the aim was to break the intentional barrier.

No psychological harm: No real stress to the tasks themselves, all they had to do was read a passage which does not cause high level of stress

The participants were kept confidential: the names were not reaved in experiments two all we know is that they were oxford undergratudes.

Deception was broken in experiment three as the control group as they were not told about the questions on the digits as they were informed the questions would only be on the shadowed passage.

31
Q

Ethnocentrism

A

It could be seen as ethnocentric because it was only done in the UK which a western culture and attention could be different in other cultures which we cannot assume.

It could not be seen ethnocentric because of the ability to process bit of information is universal due to attention being a limited rescore.

32
Q

What area does Moray link to

A

Moray’s study falls within the cognitive area because it is investigating the cognitive process of attention. Specifically it aimed to investigate selective attention by trying to find out whether (and what types of) ‘unattended’ material could break through the attentional barrier that is set up when a person focuses their listening on a spefic task.