Chapter 03 - The Social Self Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

According to research on the accuracy of self-knowledge, for what qualities are we the best judges of ourselves? For what qualities are others superior judges of us? How does motivation contribute to this asymmetry?

A

Although people predict that they are the best judges of themselves, when it comes to predicting behavior, others know us about as well as we know ourselves. However, we are superior judges of ourselves when it comes to private, inner qualities that are not easily observable (like our inner thoughts and feelings), whereas others are superior judges of us when it comes to qualities that are readily displayed in social settings (like our level of talkativeness and enthusiasm). Motivation contributes to the self/other knowledge asymmetry regarding qualities that have a positive or negative connotation. We are strongly motivated to see ourselves favorably, which may give us blind spots about our socially undesirable shortcomings. In these cases, others tend to know us more accurately than we know ourselves. (And thank goodness; you wouldn’t want to be fully aware of your every flaw!)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

actual self

A

The self that people believe they are.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

better-than-average effect

A

The finding that most people think they are above average on various personality trait and ability dimensions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

contingencies of self-worth

A

A perspective maintaining that people’s self-esteem is contingent on the successes and failures in domains on which they have based their self-worth.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Do people from Eastern cultures generally feel worse about themselves than people from Western cultures? How do researchers interpret self-reported self-esteem differences between cultures?

A

Members of Eastern cultures tend to report lower feelings of self-esteem than members of Western cultures; however, rather than reflecting an overall more negative view of the self, this difference may reflect differing cultural value systems. In East Asian and other non-Western cultures, views of the self are more interwoven with the social context. Accordingly, these cultures prioritize improving the self (perhaps to better fulfill one’s obligations and duties in social relationships and systems) and meeting the goals of the group, rather than meeting the goal of being a self-confident and powerful individual (which Western cultures tend to prioritize). Thus, it may not be accurate to conclude that members of Eastern cultures feel worse about themselves than Westerners; they simply feel different about the self as a whole.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

face

A

The public image of ourself that we want others to believe.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

How do people’s daily experiences in their contingent versus noncontingent domains affect their state self-esteem? Over time, how might these experiences translate to trait self-esteem?

A

Performing well in a contingent domain boosts state self-esteem, whereas performing poorly in a contingent domain diminishes state self-esteem. Performance in noncontingent domains has less influence on state self-esteem. Over time, accumulating experiences of good versus poor performance may influence trait self-esteem. Repeatedly performing poorly in contingent domains (threatening state self-esteem frequently) may ultimately reduce overall trait self-esteem. Similarly, regularly performing well in contingent domains (boosting state self-esteem repeatedly) may ultimately increase overall trait self-esteem. Performance over time in noncontingent domains is unlikely to affect trait self-esteem.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

How might a female undergraduate’s working self-concept regarding her gender shift during a day on campus as she attends her advanced math class (in which she is the only female), has a low-key lunch with a friend, and attends her gender studies class? Will her frequently shifting self-concept undermine her sense of having a coherent self?

A

The working self-concept shifts with situational cues, such that in a given situation, especially relevant and/or distinctive self-aspects become part of the working self-concept. In her advanced math class, this young woman’s gender identity is highly distinctive (as she is the only female), and in her gender studies class, her gender identity is highly relevant to the situation (discussing gender theory and struggles). Thus, both class contexts should highlight her gender identity in her working self-concept. During lunch, however, it’s unlikely that gender will be part of her working self-concept, being neither particularly relevant nor distinctive in that context. Despite these shifts in her working self-concept, she is probably not confused about her identity; she probably has a core set of self-aspects that define who she is regardless of her current working self-concept. Moreover, she is probably used to these fluctuations during her days on campus, since they form a stable pattern of activation and deactivation of her gender in her working self-concept (making her feel especially female in her math and gender studies classes and less so at other times).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

ideal self

A

The self that embodies people’s wishes and aspirations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

If you’re fairly sure you are scatterbrained, but a friend tells you that you’re organized and focused, what will your cognitive reaction likely be? What will your emotional reaction likely be? Which motive—self-enhancement or self-verification—drives which set of reactions?

A

Self-verification, the need to be seen accurately by others for important self-defining traits, should drive your cognitive reactions to this feedback, making you dubious about the quality of the feedback your friend has provided. In contrast, your emotional system, ruled more by self-enhancement, should register this as positive feedback and lead you to feel good about this feedback, even as your cognitive mind tells you it is inaccurate. Thus, you may think your friend is off-base in her compliment to you (perhaps making you question her competence as a judge of you), but you will still feel emotionally better about it than if she had confirmed your negative self-view of being scatterbrained.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

implementation intention

A

An “if-then” plan to engage in a goal-directed behavior (“then”) whenever a particular cue (“if”) is encountered.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Josie is a 13-year-old girl who thinks she’s a funny person, and her friends and family generally think Josie is funny too. How would Cooley’s notion of the “looking-glass self” explain how Josie’s sense of herself as funny developed? Bearing in mind that Josie is an adolescent, what does research suggest is likely occurring in her brain when she thinks about her self-views?

A

Cooley (1902) would argue that Josie’s sense of self developed from the way Josie thinks others perceive her, using others’ views as a mirror, or “looking glass,” to perceive the self. Research suggests that among young adolescents, brain areas associated with perspective taking are especially active when contemplating one’s self-views, so it seems likely that when Josie thinks about her self-views, regions of her brain that help her take the perspective of others will be activated. Thus, as an adolescent, Josie may be especially prone to incorporate what she thinks other people think of her into her self-concept.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

ought self

A

The self that is concerned with the duties, obligations, and external demands people feel they are compelled to honor.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

prevention focus

A

Self-regulation of behavior with respect to ought self standards; a focus on avoiding negative outcomes through avoidance-related behaviors.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

promotion focus

A

Self-regulation of behavior with respect to ideal self standards; a focus on attaining positive outcomes through approach-related behaviors.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

reflected self-appraisal

A

A belief about what others think of one’s self.

17
Q

self-affirmation theory

A

The idea that people can maintain an overall sense of self-worth following psychologically threatening information by affirming a valued aspect of themselves unrelated to the threat.

18
Q

self-discrepancy theory

A

A theory that behavior is motivated by standards reflecting ideal and ought selves. Falling short of these standards produces specific emotions: dejection-related emotions in the case of actual-ideal discrepancies and agitation-related emotions in the case of actual-ought discrepancies.

19
Q

self-enhancement

A

The desire to maintain, increase, or protect one’s positive self-views.

20
Q

self-esteem

A

The overall positive or negative evaluation people have of themselves.

21
Q

self-handicapping

A

The tendency to engage in self-defeating behavior in order to have an excuse ready should one perform poorly or fail.

22
Q

self-monitoring

A

The tendency to monitor one’s behavior to fit the current situation.

23
Q

self-presentation

A

Presenting the person we would like others to believe we are.

24
Q

self-regulation

A

Processes by which people initiate, alter, and control their behavior in the pursuit of goals, including the ability to resist short-term rewards that thwart the attainment of long-term goals.

25
Q

self-schema

A

A cognitive structure, derived from past experience, that represents a person’s beliefs and feelings about the self, both in general and in specific situations.

26
Q

self-verification theory

A

The theory that people sometimes strive for stable, subjectively accurate beliefs about themselves because such self-views give them a sense of coherence and predictability.

27
Q

Should people be more likely to display the better-than-average effect for their own intelligence before or after learning how intelligence is measured in scientific research? How do construals contribute to this process?

A

According to research on self-serving construals, the better-than-average effect is more prevalent for qualities with ambiguous, or fuzzy, definitions, but dissipates in strength when objective standards become apparent. Thus, before obtaining a clear definition of intelligence, people may be more prone to self-enhance with respect to their intelligence in order to bolster their self-worth. Under these conditions of ambiguity, people may construe different qualities as being more or less important to intelligence, construing their own strengths (perhaps artistic or interpersonal skill) as key to intelligence and construing their weaknesses as less relevant to intelligence. However, once people learn the scientific standards for intelligence, their tendency to display the better-than-average effect should decrease by reducing their freedom to construe their own best qualities as core to intelligence.

28
Q

situationism

A

The notion that our social self changes across different contexts.

29
Q

social comparison theory

A

The idea that people compare themselves to other people to obtain an accurate assessment of their own opinions, abilities, and internal states.

30
Q

Social psychology is common sense.

TRUE of FALSE

A

FALSE

31
Q

sociometer hypothesis

A

The idea that self-esteem is an internal, subjective index or marker of the extent to which a person is included or looked on favorably by others.

32
Q

Suppose two friends both have an actual self that is relatively happy and a potential self that is extremely happy (happier than their actual self). If this discrepancy in happiness leads one friend to experience agitation and the other friend to experience dejection, what does this tell you? What theory would this evidence support?

A

Actual selves that are discrepant with ought selves produce agitation, whereas actual selves discrepant with ideal selves produce dejection. This suggests that for the agitated friend, the extremely happy potential self is an ought self, whereas for the dejected friend, the extremely happy potential self is an ideal self. In other words, the first friend feels that she really should be happier, whereas the second friend feels she would like to ideally be happier (but it’s not a matter of “should”). This pattern of emotional responses would support self-discrepancy theory.

33
Q

What are Social Psychology’s Big Ideas?

A
  • *1. We construct our social reality**
  • *2. Social intuitions are driving forces…for better or for worse**
  • *3. Social influences shape behavior**
  • *4. We all have attitudes and dispositions**
  • *5. Biology influences social behavior**
34
Q

What is personal identity?

A

The aspect of self that is composed of psychological traits and dispositions that gives an individual personal uniqueness.

35
Q

What is social identity?

A

Aspects of an individual’s self-identity that derive from his or her knowledge of being a member of categories and groups of people, along with thevalue and emotional significanceattached to those memberships.

36
Q

What Is Social Psychology?

A

It is the scientific study of how people think about, influence, and relate to one another:

  • Social thinking: perception, beliefs, judgments, attitudes.
  • Social influence: culture, conformity, persuasion, groups.
  • Social relations: prejudice, aggression, attraction, intimacy
37
Q

working self-concept

A

A subset of self-knowledge that is brought to mind in a particular context.