Attention and Consciousness Flashcards

1
Q

means by which we actively process a limited amount of information from the enormous amount of information available through our senses, our stored memories, and our other cognitive processes

A

Attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

allows us to use our limited mental resources judiciously.

A

Attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

By dimming the lights on many stimuli from outside (sensations) and inside (thoughts and memories), we can highlight the stimuli that interest us. This heightened focus increases the likelihood that we can respond speedily and accurately to interesting stimuli.

A

Attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

the state of being aware of and able to think about one’s own existence, thoughts, and surroundings.

A

Consciousness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

It encompasses our overall awareness and subjective experience.

A

Consciousness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

First, it helps in monitoring our interactions with the environment

A

Conscious attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

it assists us in linking our past (memories) and our present (sensations) to give us a sense of continuity of experience.

A

Conscious attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

it helps us in controlling and planning for our future actions.

A

Conscious attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

We try to detect the appearance of a particular stimulus

A

Signal detection and vigilance

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

We try to find a signal amidst distracters

A

Search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

We choose to attend to some stimuli and ignore others, as when we are involved in a conversation at a party.

A

Selective attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

We prudently allocate our available attentional resources to coordinate our performance of more than one task at a time, as when we are cooking and engaged in a phone conversation at the same time.

A

Divided attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

In short notice they need to be attentive to see emergency going on in the beach that needs immediate care, and in longer period making sure that they are attentive throughout the working day see to it nothing tragic happens

A

Attending to Signals over the Short and Long Terms or Attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Finding Important Stimuli in a Crowd

A

Signal Detection

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

a framework to explain how people pick out the few important stimuli when they are embedded in a wealth of irrelevant, distracting stimuli

A

Signal-detection theory (SDT)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

often is used to measure sensitivity to a target’s presence.

A

SDT

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

When we try to detect a target stimulus (signal), there are four possible outcomes

A

hits
false alarms
misses
correct rejections

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

paying enough attention to perceive objects that are there

A

attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

perceiving faint signals that may or may not be beyond your perceptual range (such as a very high-pitched tone)

A

perception

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

indicating whether you have/have not been exposed to a stimulus before

A

memory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

refers to a person’s ability to attend to a field of stimulation over a prolonged period, during which the person seeks to detect the appearance of a particular target stimulus of interest.

A

Vigilance

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

the individual watchfully waits to detect a signal stimulus that may appear at an unknown time

A

being vigilant

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

appears to be an important brain structure in the regulation of vigilance

A

amygdala

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Two specific activation states play a role in vigilance

A

bursts and the tonic state

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

the result of relative hyperpolarization of the resting membrane potential (i.e., polarity of the membrane increases relative to its surrounding)

A

burst

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

results from relative depolarization

A

tonic state

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

During sleep, when people are less responsive to stimuli, the neurons are hyperpolarized and in burst mode higher levels of vigilance are associated with tonic discharges.

A

need iremember

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

refers to a scan of the environment for particular features—actively looking for something when you are not sure where it will appear.

A

search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

made more difficult by distracters, nontarget stimuli that divert our attention away from the target stimulus.

A

search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

cause more trouble under some conditions than under others.

A

Distracters

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

Suppose we look for an item with a distinct feature like color or shape. We conduct a ___?

A

feature search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

simply scan the environment for that feature

A

feature search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

we look for a particular combination (conjunction joining together) of features

A

conjunction search

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
34
Q

explains the relative ease of conducting feature searches and the relative difficulty of conducting conjunction searches.

A

Feature-Integration Theory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
35
Q

Feature-Integration Theory proposed by?

A

Anne Treisman and Garry Gelade in 1980

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
36
Q

focuses on how we integrate various visual features to perceive and identify objects in our environment.

A

Feature-Integration Theory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
37
Q

Imagine you’re searching for a red apple in a bowl of mixed fruits.

A

Pre-Attentive Stage: Your visual system detects various features of all fruits—colors (red, green, yellow), shapes (round, oval), and sizes (small, large) in parallel.

Focused Attention Stage: To identify the red apple, you focus your attention on combining the color red with the shape of an apple. This integration process involves scanning and attending to different fruits to find one that matches both features.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
38
Q

the data are a result of the fact that as the similarity between target and distracter stimuli increases, so does the difficulty in detecting the target stimuli. Thus, targets that are highly similar to distracters are relatively hard to detect. Targets that are highly disparate from distracters are relatively easy to detect

A

Similarity Theory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
39
Q

suggests that all searches, whether feature searches or conjunction searches, involve two consecutive stages

A

The guided-search model

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
40
Q

the individual simultaneously activates a mental representation of all the potential targets

A

parallel stage

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
41
Q

the individual sequentially evaluates each of the activated elements, according to the degree of activation

A

Serial stage

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
42
Q

Imagine you are looking for a specific type of book on a crowded bookshelf:

A

Feature Maps: You first detect features like color and size. Each book is processed for these individual features.

Salience Map: The visual system combines these features to create a salience map highlighting books that stand out based on the detected features.

Top-Down Guidance: If you know the book you’re looking for is a hardcover and red, this prior knowledge directs your attention toward books that are red and likely to be hardcover, making the search more efficient.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
43
Q

cocktail party problem, the process of tracking one conversation in the face of the distraction of other conversations.

A

Selective Attention

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
44
Q

Shadowing listen to two different messages

A

Selective Attention

45
Q

one of the earliest theories of attention, we filter information right after we notice it at the sensory level

A

Broadbent’s Model

46
Q

All incoming information is processed equally and that we use filters to manage the flow of information.

A

Broadbent’s Model

47
Q

the selective filter blocks out most information at the sensory level.

A

Selective Filter Model

48
Q

Unlike Broadbent’s model, which proposed that unattended information is completely blocked Attenuation Model suggests that unattended information is not entirely filtered out but rather attenuated or weakened. This means that some unattended information can still be processed to a degree.

A

Attenuation Model

49
Q

suggesting that the selection of information for further processing happens later in the cognitive process

A

Late-Filter Model

50
Q

According to the Late-Filter Model, all incoming sensory information is processed for meaning (semantic processing) before any selection or filtering occurs. This means that the initial stages of processing involve understanding and interpreting all sensory inputs.

A

Post-Perceptual Processing

51
Q

The selection of which information to respond to or focus on happens after all inputs have been analyzed for meaning. Essentially, the selection occurs at a later stage, based on the relevance or importance of the information.

A

Selective Attention

52
Q

Study Setup: Participants watched a videotape where a basketball game was superimposed on a hand-slapping game. They were tasked with monitoring both activities simultaneously.

Findings: Participants struggled to monitor both activities at once, even when each game was presented to a different eye

Hypotheses: Neisser and Becklen proposed that improvements in performance with practice were likely, but they believed this was due to skill development rather than special cognitive mechanisms

A

Early Research

53
Q

Study Setup: Participants performed a dual-task involving reading short stories and writing dictated words. Performance was measured in terms of response time (latency) and accuracy.

Findings: Initially, performance was poor for both tasks when done simultaneously. However, with extensive practice (85 sessions over several weeks), participants improved significantly. They became faster at reading and more accurate in comprehension and memory tasks.

Observations: Participants eventually noticed relationships among dictated words and could perform both tasks effectively without a loss in performance.

Conclusions: Spelke and colleagues concluded that with enough practice, controlled tasks can be automatized to require fewer attentional resources. Despite this, tasks remain intentional and involve significant cognitive processing.

A

Dual-Task Paradigm

54
Q

Study Setup: Research focused on simple, speeded tasks requiring quick responses.

Findings: Performance on one or both tasks typically slowed when tasks overlapped. This phenomenon, known as the psychological refractory period (PRP) effect or attentional blink, indicated that while perceptual processing of sensory stimuli can be managed, cognitive tasks (e.g., response selection, memory retrieval) suffer when performed simultaneously.

Implication: People can handle multiple perceptual tasks but struggle with cognitive operations requiring response choices or memory retrieval when tasks overlap.

A

Speeded Tasks and PRP Effect

55
Q

Study Setup: Participants solved mathematical problems while simultaneously listening for a tone and pressing a button upon hearing it.

Findings: More intelligent individuals performed both tasks more effectively. Intelligence was linked to better division of attention and multitasking ability.

A

Role of Intelligence

56
Q

Practice can improve performance on multiple tasks, allowing tasks to be performed with fewer resources, though they do not become fully automatic.

A

Practice and Automatization

57
Q

Simultaneous performance of cognitive tasks often results in slower responses due to the PRP effect.

A

Cognitive Load

58
Q

Intelligence affects the ability to multitask effectively, with more intelligent individuals typically performing better on divided attention tasks.

A

Individual Differences

59
Q

These models explain how we manage to do more than one attention-demanding task at the same time.

A

capacity models

60
Q

suggests that our attention is a limited resource. Just like we have a limited amount of physical energy to spend on tasks, we have a limited amount of mental attention that we can allocate. When we do multiple tasks, we have to split this limited resource among them.

A

Attentional-resources theory

61
Q

Concept: This model proposes that we have one single pool of attentional resources. We can divide this pool between different tasks as needed.

A

Single Pool Model

62
Q

Example: If you’re trying to do two tasks that both require verbal attention (like listening to a podcast while writing), the tasks might interfere with each other because they both draw from the same pool of verbal attention

A

Single Pool Model

63
Q

Concept: This model suggests that we have several separate pools of attention, each dedicated to different types of tasks or sensory modalities (e.g., visual, auditory).

A

Multiple Pools Model

64
Q

Example: Listening to music (auditory) while reading (visual) is easier because each task draws from a different pool of attention. This separation helps reduce interference between tasks

A

Multiple Pools Model

65
Q

Some argue that attentional-resources theory is too broad and doesn’t fully capture the complexity of attention. For example, it might not explain all aspects of how we manage attention, particularly in specific situations.

A

Criticisms

66
Q

Attentional-resources theory works well with other theories, such as filter theories. Filter theories focus on how we select and prioritize information when attention is limited, while resource theories provide a broader view of how attention is distributed.

A

Complementary Theories

67
Q

is crucial in our daily lives. For example, when driving, you need to be aware of potential dangers around you. If you don’t pay enough attention, like missing a car running a red light, you could cause or become part of a serious accident. Many car accidents are due to poor divided attention.

A

Divided attention

68
Q

When participants did this task alone without any other distractor, they were faster and more accurate however when listening to the radio or talking on a cell phone has been added the accuracy of pressing the break or ignore it based on the color has decrease. Talking on a cell phone while driving was found to be much riskier than listening to the radio. Research shows that cell phone use can be as dangerous as drunk driving, and drivers talking on the phone are more likely to exhibit anger and aggression, which can increase the likelihood of accidents. This explains why cell phone use while driving contributes significantly to accidents.

A

Divided attention

69
Q

Getting ready to pay attention and staying alert

A

Alerting

70
Q

Brain Areas: Right frontal and parietal cortexes, locus coeruleus

A

alerting

71
Q

Neurotransmitter: Norepinephrine (is essential for regulating alertness, managing stress responses, affecting mood, and supporting cognitive functions. Its proper functioning is crucial for overall mental and physical well-being.)

A

alerting

72
Q

Dysfunction: Problems with alerting can lead to ADHD or issues with aging

A

alerting

73
Q

refers to the process of directing attention to specific stimuli or locations in our environment.

A

Orienting

74
Q

It involves several cognitive and neural mechanisms that allow us to focus on what is important and ignore irrelevant information

A

Orienting

75
Q

Brain Areas: Superior parietal lobe, temporal-parietal junction, frontal eye fields, superior colliculus.

A

orienting

76
Q

These brain areas work together to manage how we focus our attention on specific stimuli or locations. They help in directing visual attention, responding to new or significant events, and making sure that our gaze aligns with what we need to focus on. By integrating sensory information and controlling eye movements, these regions enable efficient and effective orienting of attention.

A

orienting

77
Q

Neurotransmitter: Acetylcholine

A

orienting

78
Q

crucial for cognitive processes like attention, learning, and memory formation

A

Acetylcholine

79
Q

Dysfunction: Issues with orienting are linked to autism

A

orienting

80
Q

Managing and resolving conflicts among thoughts, feelings, and responses

A

Executive Attention

81
Q

Brain Areas: Anterior cingulate, lateral ventral, prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia.

A

Executive Attention

82
Q

Manages cognitive control, task switching, and goal setting, enabling complex decision-making and planning.

A

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

83
Q

Monitors conflicts and errors, integrates emotional information, and adjusts attention based on performance

A

Anterior Cingulate Cortex

84
Q

Controls response inhibition and integrates emotional information with cognitive processes

A

Ventral Prefrontal Cortex

85
Q

Selects and initiates actions, influences motor control, learning, and habit formation, and helps in prioritizing responses.

A

Basal Ganglia

86
Q

Neurotransmitter: Dopamine

A

Executive Attention

87
Q

Dysfunction: Problems here are associated with Alzheimer’s disease, borderline personality disorder, and schizophrenia

A

Executive Attention

88
Q

suggests that intelligence involves three key processing units, each linked to different brain areas

A

PASS model of intelligence

89
Q

Arousal and Attention: Associated with the brainstem and other regions, crucial for selective and divided attention.

A

Intelligence and Attention

90
Q

Simultaneous and Successive Processing: Involves understanding and processing information either all at once or in a sequence.

A

Intelligence and Attention

91
Q

Planning: Involves decision-making and goal setting

A

Intelligence and Attention

92
Q

The time needed to focus on and decide about items.

A

Inspection Time

93
Q

Measurement: Typically involves showing a target stimulus for a brief period and requiring a decision on which option is correct

A

Inspection Time

94
Q

Findings: Shorter inspection times are linked to higher intelligence scores, indicating that quicker decision-making can reflect higher cognitive abilities.

A

Inspection Time

95
Q

Concept: Intelligence is sometimes linked to how quickly the brain processes informationq

A

Reaction Time

96
Q

Experiment Choice Reaction Time: The time taken to choose the correct answer from multiple options

A

Reaction Time

97
Q

Findings: People with higher IQs generally have faster reaction times. This suggests quicker neural processing might be associated with higher intelligence.

A

Reaction Time

98
Q

Research often involves people with typical attention, but studying those with attention issues, such as brain damage or reduced blood flow in critical areas, helps us understand attentional processes better.

A

Normal Participants vs. Those with Deficits

99
Q

Problems with attention are associated with damage in these areas.

A

Frontal Lobe and Basal Ganglia

100
Q

Issues with visual attention are linked to these regions, as well as parts of the midbrain that control eye movements.

A

Posterior Parietal Cortex and Thalamus

101
Q

A condition characterized by persistent issues with attention and hyperactivity.

A

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

102
Q

Examples of how failing to pay attention can lead to missing changes or important details in our environment.

A

Change and Inattentional Blindness

103
Q

happens when people fail to notice changes in a scene or object while they are focused on something else.

A

Change blindness

104
Q

Traffic Safety: A driver might not notice a car that has suddenly appeared in their lane if they are distracted, increasing the risk of accidents.

A

Change blindness

105
Q

Medical Screening: A radiologist might miss a critical change in a scan (like a growing tumor) if they don’t notice the subtle differences from previous scans.

A

Change blindness

106
Q

occurs when people fail to see objects that are in their visual field because they are focused on something else.
Relevance:

A

Inattentional blindness

107
Q

A doctor might overlook a visible symptom or a piece of crucial information in a patient’s medical history if they are focused on other aspects of the examination.

A

Inattentional blindness

108
Q

Driving: A driver might not see a pedestrian crossing the road if they are concentrating on their phone or GPS, leading to potential accidents.

A

Inattentional blindness