2.4: Retrieval failure theory (cue-dependent forgetting) Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks)

A

Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory’

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue

A

A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.

A

Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
Example

A

For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).

A

With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.

A

There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.

A

The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

First AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
Example

A

For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
Who is this further supported by?

A

This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
Why is this a strength of the retrieval failure explanation for forgetting?

A

This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
Why is this especially true?

A

This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact,

A

In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

Second AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.

A

Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
Example

A

For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater

17
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.

A

In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough

18
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
Why is this a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting?

A

This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting

19
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

Third AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested

20
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
Why is this?

A

This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions

21
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of

A

This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way

22
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way.

Fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph

A

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that Although context-related cues appear not to have a very strong effect on forgetting, Baddeley still suggests that they are worth paying attention to and focusing on

23
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that Although context-related cues appear not to have a very strong effect on forgetting, Baddeley still suggests that they are worth paying attention to and focusing on.
Example

A

For example, many people have experienced the tip of the tongue phenomenon, whereby you think to do something such as collecting an item from a room, but when you go into that room, you forget why you came into that room

24
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that Although context-related cues appear not to have a very strong effect on forgetting, Baddeley still suggests that they are worth paying attention to and focusing on.
For example, many people have experienced the tip of the tongue phenomenon, whereby you think to do something such as collecting an item from a room, but when you go into that room, you forget why you came into that room.
What does this show?

A

This shows that when we are struggling to remember something, it is worth making the effort to try and recall the environment in which you learned it first

25
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that Although context-related cues appear not to have a very strong effect on forgetting, Baddeley still suggests that they are worth paying attention to and focusing on.
For example, many people have experienced the tip of the tongue phenomenon, whereby you think to do something such as collecting an item from a room, but when you go into that room, you forget why you came into that room.
This shows that when we are struggling to remember something, it is worth making the effort to try and recall the environment in which you learned it first.

A

This is in fact a basic principle of the cognitive interview, a method of getting eyewitnesses to crimes to recall more information

26
Q

Describe and evaluate retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting (16 marks).
Retrieval failure is when material is stored in the LTM, but cannot be consciously recalled due to a lack of retrieval cues to ‘jog the memory.’
A cue is a ‘trigger’ of information that allows us to access a memory.
Such cues may be meaningful or may be indirectly linked by being encoded at the time of learning.
For example, cues may be external (the environmental context) or internal (your mood or degree of drunkenness).
With retrieval failure, there is a lack of external contextual cues, where the environment for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different room.
There is also a lack of internal contextual cues, where the physical state for learning and recall is different, for example being in a different mood.
The types of cues that have been studied by psychologists include context, state and organisation.

The first AO3 PEEL paragraph is that an impressive range of research supports retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting.
For example, Godden and Baddeley carried out a study of deep sea divers working underwater. Divers learned a list of words underwater or on land and then were asked to recall the words either underwater or on land (creating four conditions).
In two of these conditions, the environmental contexts of learning and recall matched, whereas in the other two conditions, they did not.
Accurate recall was 40% lower in the non-matching conditions.
This is further supported by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave antihistamines to their participants which had a mild sedative effect, making them slightly drowsy.
This created an internal physiological state different from the ‘normal’ state of being awake and alert.
The participants had to learn a list of words and passages of prose and then recall the information, creating four conditions again.
In the conditions where there was a mismatch between the internal state at learning and at recall, performance on the memory test was significantly worse, so when the cues are absent (you are drowsy when recalling information, but were alert while learning it for example), there is more forgetting.
This is a strength of retrieval failure explanation for forgetting, because it suggests that it is a well-founded explanation for forgetting.
This is especially true when the evidence shows that retrieval failure occurs in real-life situations, as well as in the highly controlled conditions of the laboratory.
In fact, one prominent memory researcher, Michael Eysenck (2010), goes as far to argue that retrieval failure is perhaps the main reason for forgetting from LTM, highlighting its validity.

The second AO3 PEEL paragraph is that However, Baddeley (1997) argues that context effects are actually not very strong, especially in real life.
Different contexts have to be very different before an effect is seen.
For example, it would be difficult to find an environment as different from land as underwater.
In contrast, learning something in one room and recalling it in another is unlikely to result in much forgetting, because these environments are generally not different enough.
This is a limitation of retrieval failure as an explanation for forgetting, because it means that the real-life applications of retrieval failure due to contextual cues don’t actually explain much forgetting.

The third AO3 PEEL paragraph is that The context effect may be related to the kind of memory of tested.
This is because Godden and Baddeley (1980) replicated their underwater experiment, but used a recognition test instead of recall, meaning that participants had to say whether they recognised a word read to them from the list, instead of retrieving it for themselves.
When recognition was tested, there was no context-dependent effect; performance was the same in all four conditions.
This is a further limitation of context effects and thus retrieval failure, because it means that the presence or absence of cues only affects memory when you test it in a certain way.

The fourth AO3 PEEL paragraph is that Although context-related cues appear not to have a very strong effect on forgetting, Baddeley still suggests that they are worth paying attention to and focusing on.
For example, many people have experienced the tip of the tongue phenomenon, whereby you think to do something such as collecting an item from a room, but when you go into that room, you forget why you came into that room.
This shows that when we are struggling to remember something, it is worth making the effort to try and recall the environment in which you learned it first.
This is in fact a basic principle of the cognitive interview, a method of getting eyewitnesses to crimes to recall more information.
What does this do?

A

This highlights how retrieval failure has useful real world application