Week 3 Neuroscience of Movement Coordination Flashcards
So we’ve seen this picture before, the decerebrate cat. So the cat has had the cerebral cortex separated from its central nervous system. We put the cat on the treadmill and the cat is able to produce rhythmic ambulatory motion. For what it is, it’s a fairly complex rhythmic motion because it’s not just two limbs – it has the forelimb and the hind limb. The hind limb that has to be controlled in order for ambulation to occur. So we get to this idea of where do these patterns sit? And we’re not very creative with our name. And we call this a _____ pattern generator. Ben Lindemman started to talk a little bit about synergistic patterns that are produced with a cortical injury. The synergy patterns are ingredients that go into this central pattern generator. At some point, we learn and put together patterns and we store them away somewhere. And they are basic, but they let us function In somewhat of a primitive way, but it still allows us to function. So in week 3 or week 4, we discussed the idea of something coming at your head. What do you do? Most of us are going to put out our hands and try to protect ourselves, right? We talked about all the muscles that are involved. When we think about all the muscles that were involved, they all sat along a very similar C7 distribution. That’s an example of something that we call ______ and we’re going to be talking more about synergistic patterns and representations because that’s how we start to put together movement patterns and skilled movement.
central; synergy
But at the end of the day, we know that there’s a central pattern generator somewhere within our brains. Somewhere between the (midbrain/hindbrain) and the (basal ganglia/brainstem), maybe with some cortical input that when the cortex is under duress or we’re just starting to learn motion. So if you’re an infant, infants tend to present in synergistic patterns. Or if you’re an adult and we have a cortical injury, we go back to these base root synergies. But everything that we do, a lot of what we do from a movement perspective is synergistic and we talked about this, but we’ll bring it back into view in this particular picture. That decerebration line where we separated the cortex from the spinal cord was right (below/above) the midbrain. If it goes lower and we get the midbrain out of the picture and we cut the pattern right off of the spinal cord, the central pattern generator (is/is not) able to impact movement and decerebrate cat (is/is not) able to continue walking on a treadmill. So there’s definitely something about the mid-brain that is important.
midbrain; brainstem; above; is not; is not;
When we take a look at human beings, depending on the level of injury of the spinal cord, whether it’s a complete spinal cord injury or an incomplete spinal cord injury. There are different levels of body weight supported gait that the individual is able to produce. But also when we look at individuals with critical injuries, when we take a look at strokes in brain injuries, just simply putting the human being onto a treadmill and allowing for movement, we don’t get the same type of reciprocal gait pattern that we do from a decerebrate cat, which brings into the idea that humans have a little bit (less/more) cortical input into the central pattern generator than quadrupedal animals. So there’s a little bit (less/more) thinking and there’s a little bit (less/more) skill involved, perhaps in terms of how humans have to move. And we don’t know why that is because if you look at some animals and look at how they produce movement, just the agility and nimbleness, It’s hard to think that we require more cortical inputs for what we do. But somehow the wiring seems to come out that way.
more; more; more;
When we take a look at central pattern generators a little bit more and look at that reflexive walking, what’s interesting is, if we cut in the middle - So if we follow that blue line and cut the cord and the connections from the brain right in the middle of the midbrain, the animal (is/is not) able to produce reciprocal walking, but it (can/can’t) hold itself upright. So you have to put the cat on a truss to be able to control posture and then allow for the stimulation in the sensory input that’s coming from the treadmill touching the foot and impacting gait to happen. If you go just a few millimeters up and you cut where the midbrain meets the cerebral cortex, the animal (is/is not) able to walk in this reflexive pattern with postural control. So there’s something that happens within humans (we’re talking maybe 3-4 millimeters, in smaller animals, you’re talking about a millimeter) within that millimeter of processing that is the difference between being able to hold postural control and impact your environment and not have postural control as you impact the environment. But there is a singular problem with that - It doesn’t result in the ability to adapt to the environment, you’re just responding. So how do we start to put together motion that integrates environmental impact on what it is that we do? That comes down to the control of movement.
is; can’t; is
The control of movement reintroduces anatomical regions, which is the primary motor cortex - (M1/M2). It reintroduces the premotor cortex and it reintroduces the supplemental motor cortex into the picture. We went over gross functions of these, but want to start putting things together a little bit more in terms of what it is they do and how they do it. I’m going to add a new word into your vocabulary, which is the pre-motor area, which is confusing because we have a premotor cortex. So in some motor development and motor learning models, they clump together M2, which is the ______ motor cortex and the PMC, which is the ______ cortex and they refer to those two areas as the premotor area. I just always remember that P with premotor cortex and what it does, which is (postural/sequencing) adjustments and then supplemental area, I think of the S in supplemental and start thinking about the (sequencing/postural) of movement.
M1; supplemental; premotor; postural; sequencing
M1 is proportional to the (body mass/complexity) of the individual. So what is happening at M1? M1 primary motor cortex is the region that is connecting with our spinal cord, with our (upper/lower) motor neurons. And it’s the one that’s responsible for executing (motion/sensation). So the larger you are, the (more/less) motor units you have. So M1 is not necessarily dictated by how skilled you are in movement, but how big you are. So we want to consider that. The premotor area grows with the level of (body mass/skill) that’s responsible for movement. What’s interesting is that the premotor area is anywhere from three to six times (smaller/larger) in humans than other primates. The only real difference that we have between us and other primates is an opposable thumb, as well as the ability to produce (vision/speech). So we are able to take our mouths and our tongues and orient them and organize them in ways to produce words that make sense, whereas other primates don’t. And we have fine motor control of opposable thumbs and finger control that other primates have. So the thought is, the larger representation is really just based on the complexity of producing speech and the function of producing (gross/fine) motor coordination.
body mass; lower; motion; more; skill; larger; speech; fine
We now have technology that is relating to transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation. With these techniques we’re able to study what’s happening by either exciting or in other cases, changing the excitability of the motor pathways and the sensory pathways to get a sense of if we stimulate this area, what happens? So now we’re able to take individuals that are neurologically intact and get a sense of what they’re doing and what their brain is responsible for. So what happens when we look at motor region stimulation? When we go and excite just the primary motor cortex, we only see movement at a (single/multi) joint. So you have one muscle contracting, maybe two muscles contracting, but it is not a coordinated control movement, it is just movement through a particular joint. We used to think the primary motor cortex is a big one, it must do everything. That’s not necessarily the case. It goes back to reinforce that M1, or the primary motor cortex, is really just responsible for producing the force of movement at a (single/multiple) joint. When we stimulate the premotor area (M2 – supplemental motor cortex and premotor cortex) we start to get these (small/large) complex movement patterns involving (single/multi joints), which reinforces the idea that it’s those regions in the brain that are the most important to us from a movement production perspective. If we go back and think, where do these regions sit in the brain? They sit in the (frontal/occipital) lobe. What else happens at the frontal lobe? Judgment, decision-making, cognitive processing. So we think that there is a relationship in terms of the complexity of movement sitting at the frontal lobe because everything that we do is planned and we have to have judgment in terms of what we’re doing.
single; single; large; multi; frontal;
Motor Cortex Muscle Mapping
Individual neurons from motor cortex (do/do not) innervate individual muscles. They innervate (individual muscle/small GROUPS) of muscles.
(Primary Motor Cortex/Pre motor area) codes the FORCE of movement of muscle groups - Limb Force and limb velocity is modulated by the (anterolateral/rubrospinal) tract.
Individual neurons from the motor cortex (do/do not) innervate individual muscles. So we have upper motor neuron A - that doesn’t go to the biceps lower motor neuron A, it goes to limb flexors. So at that level it’s not controlling individual muscles, but groups of similar muscles that need to work together. Where is that information turfed out? That information probably is turfed out through the spinal cord levels. But again, if you go back to having to protect your face from a moving object, it’s not an individual muscle that M1 is controlling. Its controlling the group of neurons. Especially if it’s something that’s simple, or if you’re about to fall off the edge of a cliff and you have to step back. You don’t want 15 different pieces having to come together, you want one to control a wide network. So when we take a look at cortical mapping, it’s not a one-to-one relationship of one motor neuron from the cortex goes down to one motor neuron down at the spinal cord level, down to one motor unit.
do not; small GROUPS; Primary Motor Cortex; rubrospinal; do not;
Motor Cortex Muscle Mapping
The (primary motor cortex/premotor area) codes the force of movement of muscle groups. It looks like limb force and limb velocity is modulated once that directive is created by the motor cortex, the directive, in terms of modulating it to meet the environmental circumstances, is modulated by the (lateral corticospinal/rubrospinal) tract. Besides the primitive responses that we saw in the very beginning of the term last year, now that we know a little bit more, what else is the red nucleus hooking onto? The (basal ganglia/cerebellum)! There’s a big component of the cerebellum into the red nucleus and out of the red nucleus. So as we start to look at what’s happening, that modulation by the red nucleus is highly likely cerebellar inputs that are being considered in terms of choosing the appropriate response. In everything that we consider from a coordination and control of movement is the appropriateness of the response. You’re able to produce an appropriate response where somebody that has cortical injury, cortical damage or injury might not be able to produce the appropriate response. And then as Ben starts to go into cerebellum and basal ganglia disorders in neuro E&I II, you’ll start to see that most of the diagnostic features are in relation to inappropriate movements - So moving faster than you really want to, producing ballistic motions when they’re not necessary, etc.
primary motor cortex; rubrospinal; cerebellum;
So M1, as the primary motor cortex it’s bottom of the barrel in terms of all the processes that actually have to happen, it just (plans/executes) the movement, so that’s all M1 does, that’s all the primary motor cortex does.
executes
The supplemental motor area/cortex is involved in the planning of movement and the idea of initiating movements versus externally triggered movements. What’s an externally triggered movement? It’s a reflex. If we think about the flexor withdrawal, you step on something, it hurts, that external stimulus results in you stepping away with the involved limb and thrusting down with the uninvolved limb to give you limb support. So those we consider externally triggered, whereas throwing a ball (is/is not) the supplemental motor cortex. You want to actually take the ball, throw it at a target. You want that ball to get there at a very specific time. All of those things that you are initiating is being carried through to the supplemental motor area. There’s also the difference of catching a ball. While the act of catching a ball is not necessarily reflex because there’s visual input that has to be processed. There’s the speed of the ball that has to be processed. There’s the where do I put my arm? That appears (to be/to not be) controlled by supplemental motor area because it’s an externally triggered response. The thing that we want to consider about the supplemental motor areas are that it does things that we want to do and have to do. So it’s not as simple as one particular cortical region. And is probably more a relay. A feedforward and feedback. The supplemental motor area is also responsible for action selection. So how do we produce the motion that we want? And in general, we see supplemental motor cortex activating at about _ milliseconds (prior/after) to the initiation of movement.
is; to not be; 60; prior
The premotor cortex is the (sequencing/preparation) to move – the postural control. We’ll see that at about _ milliseconds (prior/after) to the movement. So the premotor cortex starts to activate (before/after) any other planning has to happen because we have to control for posture first. We have to be able to control our center of mass before we start to expand upon limb use. So the postural control is critical to be able to produce controlled movement at the limbs.
preparation; 100; prior; before
Got it
Gotit
Medial Pathway
Phylogenetically, (youngest/oldest) part of movement system
The main components of the medial pathway consist of:
(Cerebrocerebellar/Vestibulospinal) - Where am I in space? Am i upside down? Am i heading straight? Where is my head in space?
(Spinalcerebellar/Reticulospinal) - Alertness. There has to be some level of alertness to what you want to do.
(Tegmental/Tectospinal) tracts - The tectum is comprised of the superior and inferior colliculus. So those are response to auditory stimuli and visual stimuli.
(Advanced/Basic) Postural control - all of those tracks if you go back, sit (lateral/medial). And their response is at the trunk musculature. So the medial pathway really responsible for the most basic of postural control.
(Doesn’t have/Has) cortical input: may have (LESS/MORE) pre motor cortex input
oldest; Vestibulospinal; Reticulospinal; Tectospinal; Basic; medial; Has; more;
So when we take a look at postural control, it is run by a very specific pathway called the (lateral/medial) pathway. If we take the central nervous system and we break it down into two components, we have a cerebral cortical and then we have a brainstem and spinal cord structure. And so really what we’re dealing with in terms of the medial pathway.. think about what sits in the brainstem – rubraspinal, reticulospinal, tectospinal, spinal cerebellar. It doesn’t need a whole lot of cortical input just yet. But those are the things that are sitting along the brainstem pathways that we’ve gone over at this point. Those pathways along with the spinal cord are what’s working together with input from the (premotor/M2) cortex. So it takes information from the premotor cortex, likely (less/more) information from the premotor cortex than other regions of the cerebrum and it uses that to influence what’s happening along this medial pathway. The medial pathway is also where we start taking subcortical structures like the basal ganglia, the subthalamic nucleus, the ventral tegmental area, and the substantia nigra - all of those structures that sit deep within because if you look at it from a lateral to medial perspective, all those structures are medial with respects to the cortical structures. So those are the inputs that are responsible for postural control. So it’s not necessarily one particular part of the brain, but a connection that puts together the idea of feedforward and feedback controllers.
medial; premotor; more;