Week 3 - Models of Health Behaviour Flashcards

1
Q

How did the COVID pandemic change our view of health behaviour?

A

We were able to look at behaviours such as hand washing, mask ventilation, and vaccination not just individually but in a group (nationally and globally).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

What are distal influences of health behaviour?

A

Not direct predictors of the behaviour, but these are contextual, more distant factors that shape and influence your experience, expectations, understanding, beliefs, and motivations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What are some examples of distal influences of health behaviour?

A

Age, gender, sociocultural norms (culture/society, family, ethnicity, religion, subculture/peer group, media), socioeconomic status (living arrangements, employment status, income), personality

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

In adolescence, whose attitudes, beliefs, values, and behaviours do we give the most credence to, according to Mercken et al. (2011)?

A

One’s peers and siblings over the advice and attitudes of parents or teachers (Mercken et al. 2011). This shows that people’s expectations of health and responses to social pressure differ with age. The initiation of risk behaviour decreases with age.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What did Renier et al. (2016) find regarding male health behaviour?

A

Males perceived behaviour as less risky, took more risks, were less sensitive to negative outcomes (present or future), and were less socially anxious than female participants.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

What did Visser and Smith (2007), Calisanti et al. (2013), and Marcell et al. (2007) find regarding male health behaviour?

A

Males may drink alcohol more excessively then females and avoid seeking health care (older men and adolescents alike) due to projections of masculinity and a desire to be seen as ‘strong.’ E.g. testicular cancer is often found at stage 3.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

In Birch et al. (2019), what did a study of a representative UK adult sample find regarding the role of age and gender in health behaviours?

A

Males aged 18-24 years consumed the most ready meals and fast food. Older men (65 years+) were more likely to consume harmful levels of alcohol. Older women were more likely to be sedentary. The influences on all of these may vary/are complex (distal vs. proximal, micro vs. macro).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What are the big 5 factors of personality according to McCrae and Costa (1990)?

A

*Neuroticism, *Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness (top 3: agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness = *psychoticism)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What personality factors are associated with greater risk-taking and negative health behaviour?

A

Openness to new experiences, extraversion typically associated with risk behaviour (e.g. drinking), agreeableness may mean you are more amenable to social pressures, neuroticism tends to increase SOME negative health behaviours (e.g. emotional over/undereating) and is associated with anxiety which may increase risk behaviours (e.g. drinking or smoking)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What personality factors are associated with less risk-taking/more health-protective behaviour?

A

Openness can be associated with healthy diet, conscientiousness and agreeableness typically associated with positive health behaviour and outcomes, neuroticism associated with high use of healthcare which may mitigate against negative health outcomes (overrepresented in heartcare statistics)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

What is a multidimensional health locus of control (MHLOC)?

A

Internal or external accountability over your life/health. May differ depending on culture.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What is internal HLOC?

A

We determine our own health. It is a matter of our habits

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What is external HLOC?

A

Health is a matter of luck/fate/genes/weather. Powerful others, such as doctors/surgeons/professionals/God(s), may determine our health.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

_____ HLOC is associated with reduced risk behaviour and health-protective behaviour

A

Internal

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

In Steptoe and Wardle’s (2001) analysis of 7000 students across 18 countries, what did they find?

A

The odds of healthy behaviour was increased by 40% amongst those high in internal HLOC compared to those low in internal HLOC. It was reduced by 20% in those high in chance/external HLOC compared to low in chance HLOC.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

True or False: people must value their health in order to engage in health protective behaviour

A

False. People will engage in health protective behaviour even if health isn’t in their top life values.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

What are some examples of proximal influences on health behaviour?

A

Attitudes (orientations to objects/people/events… e.g. may like the taste of alcohol but not the effects), beliefs, expectations and goals, motives

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

What are the three components in an attitude-object relationship?

A

Thoughts/cognitive: beliefs about attitude-object (e.g. smoking is weak/dangerous).
Feelings/emotional: feelings toward object (e.g. smoking is disgusting/scary).
Behavioural: intended action (e.g. will not smoke)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

What is unrealistic optimism?

A

Our tendency to have unrealistically optimistic appraisals of our personal risk. We tend to have biased risk perception as we tend to compare ourselves with the groups that make us come out better.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

What is self-preservation bias?

A

Our tendency to protect ourselves by comparing ourselves to people who look okay, but have worse/similar health behaviours. E.g. “I know someone who’s smoked and drank all their life - never had a day’s illness.”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

What are the four factors involved in unrealistic optimism (Weinstein, 1987)?

A

Lack of personal experience with behaviour/problem/health outcome, belief that action can prevent problem (behavioural control), belief that if problem has not occurred then it won’t in the future (bias of youth), belief that problem is uncommon.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

What is one limitation of unrealistic optimism studies?

A

They have yet to measure ‘perceived risk’ in relation to ‘actual risk’

23
Q

What is behavioural self-regulation theory?

A

We have motivations and do things for a reason. Outcome expectancies help us set goals. These goals are what focus our mind and direct (most of) our behaviour (self-regulation) towards achieving these goals

24
Q

What is a challenge of behavioural self-regulation theory?

A

Habits challenge the idea that we are doing things for a reason because they are motivated by unconscious goals.

25
Q

What is Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) according to Bandura (1977, 1986)?

A

Behaviour is determined by three types of individual expectancies: situation-outcome expectancies whereby a person connects a situation to an outcome (e.g. smoking to a heart attack); outcome expectancies (e.g. believing that stopping smoking would reduce the risk of heart attack); self-efficacy beliefs - these are critical (e.g. the extent to which the person believes they can stop smoking).

26
Q

What is self-efficacy according to Bandura (1986)?

A

“A belief in ones capabilities to organise and execute the sources of action required to manage prospective situations.” In other words, it is the belief and confidence that a future action (e.g. weight loss) is within your capabilities.

27
Q

What cognitive and emotional activities is self-efficacy likely to generate?

A

Goal-directed behaviour (i.e. the setting of high personal goals), positive outcome expectancies, and reduced anxiety about future (this is one of the main reasons people don’t try). These cognitions and emotions in turn affect actions (e.g. dietary change, exercise, and promoted perseverence).

28
Q

What are some psychosocial factors that appear in various models of health behaviour?

A

Demographic factors, personality, social norms, attitudes, risk perceptions and unrealistic optimism, goals and motivation, self-efficacy

29
Q

What is the ‘upward spiral theory of lifestyle change’ that was proposed by Van Capellen et al. (2018)?

A

This considers the role of positive affect and implicit, nonconscious motives in relation to health behaviour change, perhaps particularly relevant to the maintenance of behaviour change.

30
Q

What is the difference between models and theories? What are the similarities?

A

Models describe associations, theories explain them. They generate research, predict behaviour, help explain data, and solve problems.

31
Q

How/why do we test models?

A

Take an underpinning theory that can be assessed quantitatively, and test it empirically against another concept/theory that can be tested quantitatively, then look at correlations, associations, and predictions.

32
Q

What are the caveats to modeling health behaviour?

A

Different health behaviours are controlled by different external factors (e.g. smoking is socially sanctioned against nowadays, while exercise is being socially promoted). Attitudes towards health behaviours vary within (as well as between) individuals (e.g. drinking vs. smoking cigs). Motivating factors may differ for different health behaviours (e.g. exercise for fitness vs. aesthetic reasons). Motivating factors may change over time (e.g. motivation to drink as an adolescent vs. adult). Individual differences in attitudes and motivations are in part explained by life stage and the associated perceived norms (e.g. a teen may diet for aesthetic reasons while a middle-aged person may diet to avoid heart disease). The social context can trigger or alternatively limit behaviour (e.g. you might binge drink with friends but not family).

33
Q

What is the health belief model (Becker, 1974)?

A

The likelihood that somebody will engage in a particular health behaviour depends on the demographic factors (the distal things) as well as perceived threat (severity and susceptibility) of the endpoint and weighing the perceived barriers and benefits of carrying out the behaviour. Combined, these will trigger the likelihood of the behaviour change taking place. Added cues to action (1975) - internal or external - to directly or indirectly predict health behaviour and health motivation (1977) to directly predict health behaviour.

34
Q

What are the assumptions of the Health Belief Model (Becker, 1974)?

A

Situation outcome beliefs: a situation is connected to an outcome (e.g. smoking is related to heart attack). Outcome expectancies: by taking a recommended positive action, an individual can avoid a negative health condition (e.g. if I stop smoking I can reduce my risk of a heart attack). Self-efficacy beliefs: individual believes that they can smoothly and successfully take action to prevent a negative health consequence (e.g. I can manage to sop smoking). Assumes the greater the threat, the more the action… the opposite can be true.

35
Q

What is subjective expected utility theory?

A

Individuals are active and generally rational decision-makers influenced by the perceived utility (usefulness to them) of certain actions/behaviours. In other words, how we make decisions rationally based on pros and cons of action.

36
Q

What are some limitations to the HBM?

A

Several versions over the years. May better predict preventative behaviour uptake than risk behaviour reduction. Components studied independently but may interact and vary in salience. Over-estimates the role of threat (overuse of fear arousal can be counterproductive… increasing stress and the behaviour). Static model - excludes change in beliefs/perceptions. Does not explicitly include emotions. Removing barriers is not always sufficient to increase positive behaviour. It is user-friendly, easy-to-assess, and quite intuitive, but it hasn’t been highly predictive.

37
Q

What is the Theory of Planned Behaviour (Azjen, 1985, 1991)?

A

Comes from social psychology and Bandura’s 1986 Social Cognition Theory that presumes any behaviour is explained by intention, which is explained by your attitudes, outcome expectancies, beliefs in control, and normative beliefs. Addresses the psychological processes linking attitude and behaviour by incorporating wider social influences and perceived norms (motivation to comply), beliefs in personal behavioural control (similar to self-efficacy), and the necessity of intention formation.

38
Q

Theory of Planned Behaviour can explain ____ of the variance in people’s intentions from their attitudes, perceived behavioural controls, and subjective norms.

A

~45%

39
Q

How does perceived behavioural control relate to intention?

A

Internal control = skills/ abilities/ information. External control = obstacles/ opportunities. If a person has less control than they perceive, behavioural change is less likely even if a positive intention had been formed.

40
Q

What were the findings of Sutton and White’s (2016) systematic review and meta-analysis of 38 studies of predictors of sun-protection intentions and behaviour?

A

PBC and subjective norms proved significant but attitudes were the strongest predictors of intentions and intentions were stronger than PBC in terms of predicting behaviour.

41
Q

What are the limitations of the Theory of Planned Behaviour?

A

Does not acknowledge likely bi-directional relationships between independent and dependent variables. Research over-relied on cross-sectional data instead of longitudinal studies. Assumption that the same factors that lead to initiation of health behaviours also maintain them - not much evidence on maintenance. Intention theorised as the critical precursor of behaviour change, but intervening to increase intention (e.g. by targeting normative of PBC beliefs) has not had a huge effect on behaviour.

42
Q

What did Rhodes and Dickau’s (2012) meta-analysis of TPB find?

A

Medium-sized changes in intention only resulted in trivial-sized changes in behaviour.

43
Q

If goals are valueed and self-efficacy is high, what can be done to further change behaviour according to Gollwitzer (1993, 1999)?

A

Write an implementation intention action plan. This increases commitment. These plans work well whether the goal is more proximal or distal (longer-term).

44
Q

According to Weinstein et al. (1988), a stage theory has 4 properties:

A

Classification system to define stages, ordering of stages (e.g. early, mid, and late stage), common barriers to change facing people within same stage, different barriers to change facing people in different stages.

45
Q

What two models are stage models?

A

Transtheoretical model and Health Action Process Approach (HAPA)

46
Q

What is the transtheoretical model (Prochaska & Diclemente, 1986)?

A

A stage model that progresses from precontemplation, to contemplation, to preparation, and finally to action. Sometimes people fall off, relapse, and exit the model. Some come back and maintain for a bit then relapse. This may result in termination of the behaviour change. This change doesn’t necessarily have to last forever.

47
Q

What is the evidence of the efficacy of the transtheoretical model?

A

This model has been shown to be useful clinically. When it comes to intervention, you can intervene at stages and help people move more towards behaviour and different interventions to deal with relapse.

48
Q

What is the Health Action Process Approach or HAPA (Schwarzer, 1992)?

A

A hybrid model, having both ‘static’ and staged, temporal qualities. It distinguishes motivational and volitional phases. Self-efficacy is not static, it’s a process that is crucial in each phase. It feeds in and influences all phases.

49
Q

What are the motivational phases of HAPA?

A

Self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, and risk perceptions.

50
Q

What are the volitional phases of HAPA?

A

Following the motivational phase, individuals decide to act and then make plans to begin/maintain behaviour as well as deal with failure. This further subdivides into planning phase, action phase, maintenance phase.

51
Q

What did the meta-analysis of 95 studies using the HAPA (Zhang, Zhag, Schwarzer, & Hagger, 2019) confirm?

A

It confirmed the predictive role of both action and maintenance (motivational and volitional) self-efficacy and of outcome expectancy. The effects of outcome expectancies and action self-efficacy were mediated by intentions, action, and coping planning.

52
Q

What did a meta-analysis of 81 studies by Brewer et al. (2016) reveal about how anticipatory regret predict intention and behaviour?

A

Anticipatory inaction regret (i.e. regret from not doing something) is a stronger predictor of intention and behaviour than anticipatory action regret (i.e. regret from doing something).

53
Q

*There are four factors involved in unrealistically optimistic appraisals of personal risk (Weinstein, 1987). What is not a factor?

A

Belief that no action can prevent the problem.

54
Q

*According to Bandura (1977, 1986), behaviour is determined by individual expectancies. What is one of the three types of individual expectancies outlined by Bandura?

A

Outcome expectancies