Speech Flashcards

1
Q

```

~~~

Distinguish between aphasia and dysphasia

A
  • American textbooks often use these as synonyms.
  • In Australia and the UK, aphasia means complete loss of a particular language skill, while dysphasia means a partial loss.
  • ANUMS students are expected to use the Australian/UK terminology.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Breakdown dysphasia types

A
  • Receptive Dysphasia: Problem with understanding language input
  • Expressive Dysphasia: Problem with producing language output
  • Mixed or Global Dysphasia: Problem with both input and output
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Describe the modular nature of language processing

A
  • Different elements of language processing and speech production are performed by different anatomical components of the nervous system.
  • The important ones are Broca’s, Wernicke’s, and the arcuate fasciculus.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Describe language input/output

A
  • For most of our evolutionary history (before the invention of writing), all language input was auditory.
  • Not surprisingly, the language input center (Wernicke’s Area) is located right next to the primary auditory cortex, but…
  • One hemisphere plays the major role in language processing, while the other plays a minor supporting role (Wernicke’s Area is unilateral).
  • In >95% of right-handed subjects and 60-70% of left-handed subjects, the dominant hemisphere for language is the left hemisphere.

Language Components - Output
- For most of our evolutionary history, all language output was spoken.
- Not surprisingly, the language output center (Broca’s Area) is located right next to the mouth and tongue part of the primary motor cortex, but…
- As with language input, one hemisphere plays the major role in language processing, so Broca’s Area is unilateral.
- The dominant language output area is on the same side as the dominant input area. (Left in >95% of right-handed subjects and 60-70% of left-handed subjects).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Describe the connection between input and output

A
  • The major input and output areas for language are separated by the Sylvian fissure.
  • The shortest available route between the two centers is thus a curved (arcuate) path running around the posterior end of the Sylvian fissure.
  • The major direct connection between Wernicke’s and Broca’s Areas uses this obvious route, and is known as the Arcuate Fasciculus.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Briefly describe how compression expression and repetition may be tested

A

Three Main Language Skills
- Comprehension
- Expression
- Repetition

Testing the Three Basic Components
- Comprehension
- Can the patient follow one-step, two-step or three-step commands?

  • Expression
    • tell me how you make a cup of coffee
    • Note the patient’s spontaneous use of speech. Is it hesitant?
    • If necessary, ask them to explain or describe something.
    • Listen for grammatical usage, “telegraphic speech” (loss of short grammatical words, with meaning largely conveyed by the main verbs and nouns), semantic paraphasias (use of a word with a neighboring meaning – “arm” for “leg”) and literal paraphasia (use of a similar sounding word – “short” for “stroke”).
  • Repetition
    • eg no ifs ands or buts
    • The simplest language task, repeating a spoken phrase, requires all three basic components (Wernicke’s Area, the Arcuate Fasciculus, and Broca’s Area) but no other language components or connections.
    • Note that the repetition task also requires:
      1. Working ears and intact auditory pathways
      2. Working mouth and intact motor pathways
      3. Working vocal cords
    • … but these modules are not part of language processing.
    • Also note that this task only just qualifies as language; an infant, a home computer, or even a parrot may pass the repetition test, and we could pass the test in language we didn’t know.
    • In two relatively rare dysphasias, the repetition task may be the only part of the language system that fails (Conduction Aphasia) or the only part that still works (Transcortical Aphasia).
    • More commonly, the brunt of the injury falls on the input center (Wernicke’s Aphasia), the output center (Broca’s Aphasia), or both (Global Aphasia), causing a failure of repetition along with other deficits.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Describe other languge areas

A
  • The main difference between mere “parroting” and human language use is that our language has meaning.
  • Although the nature of that meaning is not completely sorted out, it is clear that anatomical connections and functional translations are required between the two major language regions (Wernicke’s and Broca’s Areas) and the rest of the cognitive machinery.
  • Anatomically, these connections appear to be distributed diffusely around the border of the language centers (in the neighboring cortex and in the white matter deep to the language centers).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Describe language and grammar

A
  • Vocabulary and Grammar, the two divisions of language that are most obvious when learning a second language, are needed for both comprehension and expression: Wernicke’s Area needs to “know” both grammar and vocabulary, and so does Broca’s Area.
  • Wernicke’s and Broca’s have each specialized in one of these skills.
  • The “lexicon”, or mapping between word-sounds and word-meanings, is closely associated with Wernicke’s Area. (Vocabulary)
  • The “syntactical engine”, responsible for correct application of grammar rules to sentences, is closely associated with Broca’s Area. (Grammar)
  • This specialization makes some sense. When learning a foreign language, for instance, vocabulary is more likely to be stretched during comprehension tasks, while grammar is rarely critical for correct interpretation (“man car drive”); conversely, our grammar is more likely to be stretched when we try to express ourselves.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Describe consequences of insults to language apparatus

A
  1. Wernicke’s aphasia
    • Loss of comprehension plus word-meanings
  2. Broca’s aphasia
    • Loss of expression plus grammar
  3. Conductive aphasia (Arcuate fasciculus)
    • Loss of repetition
  4. Transcortical sensory aphasia
    • Loss of comprehension but preserved repetition
  5. Transcortical motor aphasia
    • Loss of expression but preserved repetition
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Descrieb Broca’s apahsia

A
  • A common dysphasia, predominantly affecting expression.
    • The hallmark is a delay or complete failure at the expression phase of language processing, which affects both expression of the patient’s own ideas as well as repetition.
    • Impairment of the syntactical engine leads to preferential loss of syntactical words (“if”, “then”, “but”) and relative preservation of content words (“stroke”, “ambulance”, “cigarettes”). This is evident in spontaneous speech and in repetition tasks (“No ifs ands or buts”).
    • Speech is slow, hesitant (non-fluent) and syntactically simple (“telegraphic”, as if written in an old-fashioned telegram where each word cost money). For instance: “Blood pressure… doctor give tablet”.
    • The patient is usually frustrated.
    • For these reasons, it is often called “Expressive Dysphasia”.

Broca’s Aphasia

  • In a classic case of Broca’s Aphasia, comprehension is largely intact, but it is never completely intact.
  • There are two main reasons why comprehension may be mildly impaired in what otherwise looks like a predominantly expressive Broca’s Aphasia:
    • There may be some mild damage to Wernicke’s Area.
    • The “syntactical engine” is likely to be damaged, so sentences requiring complex or non-standard parsing maybe misinterpreted. (“The lion was killed by the tiger. Which animal died?” Patients with Broca’s aphasia may simply perceive “Lion…killed…tiger”).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

DeSCRIBE w’S

A
  • A reasonably common aphasia, but rarely a pure syndrome. (More often a mixed aphasia with dominant involvement of Wernicke’s Area.)
    • The hallmark is impaired comprehension. Questions are not answered appropriately and complex commands are not carried out. In milder versions, with only partial damage to Wernicke’s, this may be the only obvious feature.
    • Repetition is impossible in a complete case.
    • For both of these reasons, Wernicke’s Aphasia is often called “Receptive Aphasia”.
    • Because of the complex interdependence of Wernicke’s and Broca’s Areas, there are actually major impairments in expression, as well.

Wernicke’s Aphasia

  • In true Wernicke’s Aphasia, there is lexical impairment, and a poor mapping of sounds to meanings, causing word-substitutions (paraphasias).
  • There can be near-misses at the sound end of the mapping (literal or phonemic paraphasia - “pish” instead of “fish”) or at the meaning end of the mapping (verbal or semantic paraphasia - “clock” instead of “watch”).
  • Broca’s area is working well, but its output is based on poor lexical information (garbage in, garbage out) and the patient cannot self-monitor speech, or comprehend the first half of a sentence while trying to express the second half.
  • Patients often have anosognosia (unawareness of the deficit).
  • Speech is thus fluent and grammatically normal, but is often empty, meaningless or littered with phonemic and semantic paraphasias. (“The dird went in the doctor, my tablet was in the page with the words, and he gop a headache”).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Describe global or mixed

A
  • In complete global aphasia, comprehension and speech are both absent.
    • In cases that are almost complete, the patient is reduced to single words (“yes” and “no”, or common nouns) and may only understand simple questions or single-word prompts (“drink?” “yes”).
    • More commonly, there is a dysphasia with incomplete impairment in comprehension and expression. Speech is non-fluent and littered with errors. The problems with comprehension are only noted when specifically tested with complex commands.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Describe transcortical and conduction

A
  • Lesion is not intrinsic to language areas
  • Does affect their connections with the rest of the cortex
  • Hallmark is intact repetition either with a comprehension issue, expression issue or both
  • Both or mixed: one’s own language is foreign. Can repeat words verbatim but with impaired understanding

Conduction Dysphasia

  • This is a rare but interesting dysphasia. It provides good evidence that the “language of thought” is not our native language (English, etc), even though our thoughts are often presented to us in a linguistic form and some forms of higher thinking may be impossible without language.
  • The hallmark is impaired repetition, in the presence of normal comprehension. Ideas can be understood, and then re-expressed, but the original words are lost, as though repeating a story a day later.
  • For instance, when asked to repeat: “The fuel truck crashed and spilled petrol on the road,” a patient might say “A truck carrying petrol crashed, and there was petrol all over the road.”
  • Because Broca’s Area is separated from the lexicon, however, paraphasic errors are common in spontaneous speech, so it may resemble mild Wernicke’s Dysphasia.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Describe anomia

A

Anomia
- Anomia = the inability to name objects
- Common in all dysphasias (Wernicke’s/lexicon needed to retrieve the word, Broca’s to prepare the word for motor output).
- Has poor localizing value.
- Many dysphasias recover to leave a mild dysnomia as the only residual sign.
- Ease of recall depends on frequency of use, so mild dysnomias will only affect low-frequency words (“telescope”), leaving high-frequency words intact (“pen”).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Discuss delirium and psychosis

A
  • Both dysphasia and confusion may be described by laypeople as “speaking gibberish”.
    • Delirium should be considered whenever there is an apparent language disorder. Non-verbal behavior should be assessed, to determine whether the problem extends beyond language.
    • An episode of dysphasia (especially Wernicke’s) may be misdiagnosed as an episode of confusion.
    • Severe schizophrenia may produce such disordered thought processes that the language output may consist of unrelated words (“word salad”). This may superficially resemble Wernicke’s Aphasia.
    • Note that a patient may fail at repetition tasks because they don’t understand what is required (confusion) or have no interest in cooperating (drowsiness, apathy, severe schizophrenia).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Discuss associated neuro deficits

A
  • The language areas are usually affected in conjunction with nearby structures, so that dysphasia coexists with other deficits.
  • In Broca’s Aphasia, weakness of the right face and right arm are common, as is dysarthria, as expected from the arrangement of the motor homunculus.
  • In Wernicke’s Aphasia, weakness and dysarthria are usually mild or absent and a contralateral visual field defect is common, especially affecting the right upper quadrant.
  • In conductive or transcortical aphasias, associated deficits are generally milder.
17
Q

Discuss other language structures

A
  • Dysphasia may occur with subcortical lesions of the thalamus, striatum, and internal capsule. The reasons are complex and poorly understood. Just be aware that it is possible.
  • The non-dominant hemisphere plays a role in interpreting and producing the musical aspects of language: intonation, emotional flavoring, and so on (prosody). It communicates with the primary language areas via the corpus callosum. Some lesions may cause loss of prosody.
18
Q

Describe dysarthria

A
  • Dysarthria is a motor impairment of the mouth, tongue, or pharynx, leading to slurred or poorly articulated speech.
  • In a pure dysarthria, there is no problem with language processing or word selection. All the correct words are present in the patient’s speech but they are just less clearly pronounced.
  • As with any motor impairment, the potential causes are:
    • Diffuse CNS impairment (e.g., drunkenness)
    • An upper motor neuron lesion (e.g., stroke)
    • A cerebellar lesion (e.g., stroke)
    • A lower motor neuron lesion (e.g., cranial nerve lesion, particularly VII and XII)
    • A lesion of the neuromuscular junction (e.g., myasthenia gravis)
    • A muscle lesion (very rare as a cause of dysarthria)
    • A non-neuromuscular mechanical lesion (e.g., loose dentures, cleft palate)

Spastic Dysarthria vs Nonspastic Dysarthria
- The hallmark of any upper motor neuron is increased tone, increased effort in overcoming opposing muscles, and a mildly disturbed temporal pattern.
- The same features are found in a UMN lesion of the muscles of speech production.
- Spastic speech is slow, strained, with a longer time spent on some syllables than is usual; it looks effortful.
- The hallmark of a lower motor neuron lesion is wasting and weakness with little or no disturbance of temporal patterning.
- In an LMN dysarthria, speech proceeds with a normal rhythm but consonants are pronounced indistinctly; it resembles severe mumbling.

19
Q

Describe scanning

A
  • The hallmark of cerebellar motor impairment is that the motor components are put together awkwardly, with impaired temporal patterning, difficulty with rapid sequences (dysdiadochokinesis), and a failure to scale the effort of the movements accurately (dysmetria).
  • The same features are found in cerebellar lesions affecting speech.
  • Cerebellar speech has an irregular rhythm with some syllables slower and some faster than usual, and some too loud or too soft, and unexpected breaks within words or words joined. It may be difficult to distinguish from spastic dysarthria.
20
Q

Distinguish between total abarthria and aphasia; dysphinia

A
  • Both of these are associated with complete lack of speech, so how do you tell them apart?
  • Distinguishing these can be very important for correct stroke management.
  • Anarthria: difficulty moving the tongue or palate, difficulty swallowing, intact comprehension.
  • Aphasia: deficits in comprehension.
  • Associated deficits may point to the relevant brain region (anterior circulation vs posterior circulation).
  • Milder deficits during the onset or recovery from anarthria/aphasia may be easier to distinguish than the deficits during the worst of the event.

Dysphonia
- Dysphonia is an impairment of voice production.
- In pure dysphonia, language is normal and whispering speech is normal, but voiced speech is too quiet, or is hoarse.
- The most familiar example is laryngitis.
- Possible causes are lesions of the laryngeal nerves, the larynx, respiratory weakness, or sometimes higher motor centres (Parkinson’s disease produces hypophonia).
- Weakness of one vocal cord usually causes hoarseness, while weakness of both causes the patient to whisper and, if severe, may cause an inspiratory obstruction (stridor).