sleep disorders Flashcards
N-REM sleep
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
conventionally subdivided into 4 stages defined along the EEG measurement axis
The 4 NREM stages roughly parallel a depth-of-sleep continuum - arousal thresholds lowest in stage 1 and highest in stage 4
EEG pattern in NREM sleep is commonly described as synchronous, with characteristic waveforms like sleep spindles, K-complexes, and high-voltage slow waves
NREM is a relatively inactive yet actively regulating brain in a movable body
REM sleep
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
defined by EEG activation, muscle atonia, and episodic bursts of rapid eye movements
Typically not divided into stages, although tonic and phasic can be distinguished
- Distinction is based on short lived events such as eye movements that tend to occur in clusters separated by episodes of relative inactivity
The most common marker of REM sleep phasic activity are bursts of rapid eye movements - muscle twitches often accompany REM bursts
Mental activity is associated with dreaming
REM is an activated brain in a paralyzed body
measures of sleep
electromyogram (EMG)
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
measures muscle response or electrical activity in response to a nerve’s stimulation of the muscle
sleep onset:
may show a gradual diminution of muscle tonus as sleep approaches, but rarely does a discrete EMG change pinpoint sleep onset
measures of sleep
electrooculogram (EOG)
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
measures eye movements
sleep onset:
shows slow, possibly asynchronous eye movements that usually disappear within several minutes - occasionally coincides with sleep onset
measures of sleep
electroencephalogram (EEG)
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
measures electrical activity in the brain
sleep onset:
the EEG changes from a pattern of clear rhythmic alpha activity, to a relatively low-voltage, mixed-frequency pattern (stage 1 sleep)
- Usually occurs seconds to minutes after the start of slow eye movements
- However, a number of investigators require the presence of specific EEG patterns - the K-complex or sleep spindle (i.e., stage 2 sleep) - to acknowledge sleep onset
= it is difficult to accept a single variable as marking sleep onset
The consensus is that EEG change to stage 1, usually accompanied by slow eye movements, identifies the transition to sleep, provided that another EEG sleep pattern doesn’t intervene
behavioral concomitants of sleep onset
simple behavioral task
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
The experimental paradigm is that a participant performs a simple task, that becomes automatic quickly
What is commonly observed is that the simple behavior continues after the onset of slow eye movements and may persist for several seconds after the EEG changes to a stage 1 sleep pattern
The behavior then ceases, usually to recur only after the EEG reverts to a waking pattern
i.e., simple, ‘automatic’ behavior can persist past sleep onset and as one passes in and out of sleep
behavioral concomitants of sleep onset
visual response
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Used to show perceptual disengagement from the environment that accompanies sleep onset
A bright light is placed in front of the subject’s eyes, and the subject is asked to respond when a light flash is seen pressing a sensitive microswitch
when the EEG pattern is stage 1/2, the response is absent more than 85% of the time
behavioral concomitants of sleep onset
auditory response
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Response to sleep onset is examined with a series of tones played over earphones to a subject who is instructed to respond each time a tone is heard
Reaction times to auditory stimuli become longer in proximity to the onset of stage 1 sleep
behavioral concomitants of sleep onset
hypnic myoclonia
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Hypnic myoclonia = a fairly common sleep-onset experience, perceived as a general/localized muscle contraction very often associated with rather vivid visual imagery
Not pathological, however it tends to occur more commonly in association with stress or with unusual sleep schedules
Explanation: a dissociation of REM sleep components → a breakthrough of the imagery component of REM sleep (hypnagogic hallucination) occurs in the absence of the REM motor inhibitory component
memory near sleep onset
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
The transition from wake to sleep tends to produce a memory impairment
One view is that sleep closes the gate between short- and long-term memory stores
Sleep inactivates the transfer of storage from short- to long-term memory
Encoding of the material before sleep onset is of insufficient strength to allow recall
normal pattern/cycle of sleep
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Enters sleep through NREM sleep, REM doesn’t occur until 80 minutes or longer thereafter
NREM and REM sleep alternate throughout the night
- REM episodes usually become longer across the night
- Stage 3 and 4 occupy less time in the 2nd cycle and might disappear altogether from later cycles, as stage 2 expands to occupy the NREM portion of the cycle
- The average period of the NREM-REM cycle is +/- 90-110 minutes
first sleep cycle
NREM stages
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Stage 1: usually lasts for a few minutes and can be easily discontinued (1-7 min) = low arousal threshold
- Occurs as a transitional stage throughout the night
- An increase in the amount/percentage of stage 1 sleep points to a severely disturbed sleep
Stage 2: signaled by sleep spindles or K-complexes in the EEG
- 10-25 min
- A more intense stimuli is required to produce arousal
- account for 45% of your time alseep (whole night of sleep)
Stage 3: high-voltage slow wave activity comprising more than 20%, but less than 50% of the EEG record
- Lasts for only a few minutes
- Transition to stage 4
Stage 4: identified when the high-voltage slow-wave activity comprises more than 50% of the record
- Investigators sometimes combine stage 3 and 4 and call it delta sleep, or slow wave sleep (SWS)
first sleep cycle
REM stage
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
In the first cycle it is usually short-lived (1-5min)
Arousal threshold is variable throughout the night
- Possibly at times, the person’s selective attention to internal stimuli precludes a response
- Arousal stimulus is incorporated into the ongoing dream story rather than producing an awakening
first sleep cycle
distribution of sleep stages across the night
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
SWS dominates the NREM portion of sleep towards the beginning of the night (i.e., first 1/3 of sleep)
- The preferential distribution of SWS shows a marked response to the length of prior wakefulness
REM episodes are longest on the last one third of the night
- This preferential distribution of REM sleep is thought to be linked to a circadian oscillator, which can be gauged by the oscillation of body temperature
Brief episodes of wakefulness intrude later in the night, usually near REM transitions and are not long enough to be remembered in the morning
first sleep cycle
length of sleep
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Varies widely, thus it’s difficult to characterize a ‘normal’ pattern
Volitional control (i.e., staying up late, waking by alarm, etc.,) is among the most significant predictors of the length of nocturnal sleep
- Genetic determinants: the volitional control is superimposed on the genetic determinants of sleep length
As sleep is extended, the amount of REM sleep increases, bc REM sleep depends on the persistence of sleep into the peak circadian time in order to occur
factors modifying sleep stage distribution
age
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
Strongest and most consistent moderator
- SWS is maximal in young children and decreases markedly with age
- By age 60, SWS might no longer be present, particularly in men
REM sleep as a percentage of total sleep is well maintained into old age
Arousals during sleep increase markedly with age
More heterogeneity in sleep among older populations making it harder to create normative assumptions
factors modifying sleep stage distribution
prior sleep history
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
A person who has experienced sleep loss on one/more nights shows a sleep pattern that favors SWS during recovery
- REM sleep rebounds on the subsequent recovery nights after an episode of sleep loss
- = SWS tends to be preferentially recovered compared with REM sleep, which tends to recover only after the restoration of SWS
When one is differentially deprived of REM or SWS (through medication or operationally), there’s a preferential rebound of that stage of sleep when natural sleep is resumed
Chronic restriction of nocturnal sleep can result in premature REM sleep
- Associated with hallucinations, sleep paralysis or increased muscle tweaking
factors modifying sleep stage distribution
circadian rhythms
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
the circadian phase at which sleep occurs affects the distribution of sleep stages
REM sleep, in particular, occurs with a circadian distribution that peaks in the morning hours coincident with the cycle of the core body temperature rhythm
- Thus, if sleep onset is delayed until the peak REM phase of the circadian rhythm - that is, the early morning - REM sleep tends to predominate and can even occur at the onset of sleep
factors modifying sleep stage distribution
drug ingestion
Carskadon & Dement (2005)
Normal Human Sleep: An Overview
antidepressants tend to suppress REM sleep
Benzodiazepines tend to suppress SWS and have no consistent effect on REM sleep
Alcohol intake - acute presleep intake can produce an increase in SWS and REM sleep suppression early, which can be followed by REM sleep rebound in the latter portion of the night as the alcohol is metabolized
Marijuana [THC] - minimal sleep disruption, characterized by a slight reduction of REM sleep
results
Brautsch et al (2023)
Digital media use and sleep in late adolescence and young adulthood: A systematic review
delayed bedtime: 4/5 studies found an association between digital media use and delayed bedtimes
- E.g., receiving a message @ night was associated w/ later bedtimes among 15-17 yr-olds
sleep onset latency and problem falling asleep: 5 studies - inconsistent results
sleep disturbances: 2/3 studies found an association
short sleep duration: 23 studies - most found an association BUT depended on type of media
- Evidence for mobiles, computers (gaming), internet, social media, general use of screens
- Inconsistent for tablets, game console, and television
early wakening: inconsistent
daytime tiredness and poor daytime function: association found for 1 but inconsistent for 2
poor sleep quality: studies indicated that screen time and use of mobile phone/ smartphone, computer, internet, and social media worsened sleep quality
results
summary of results
Brautsch et al (2023)
Digital media use and sleep in late adolescence and young adulthood: A systematic review
General support that the timing of digital media use is important for young people’s sleep
Using digital media at bedtime/nighttime is associated with delayed bedtime, short sleep duration, sleep quality and daytime tiredness
Particularly, the use of smartphone before going to bed and notifications from the smartphone during the night were identified as related to delayed bedtime, short sleep duration, sleep quality, and daytime tiredness
Engagement w/digital media content - gaming, internet use and social media use are associated w/shorter sleep duration, and poor sleep quality
Especially addiction to and problematic use of social media/smartphone are related to poor sleep quality
discussion
2 hypotheses about the timing of digital media use
Brautsch et al (2023)
Digital media use and sleep in late adolescence and young adulthood: A systematic review
- Engagement with digital media may induce mental arousal/hyperarousal, leading to difficulties falling asleep and poor sleep quality
More ‘communicative’, engaging media (e.g., smartphone social media) has a greater impact than more ‘passive’ media (e.g, television) - Exposure to blue light emitted from screens can disrupt the 24-h circadian rhythm and delay the secretion of melatonin
Could be both → as in that the more blue light and the more arousing the media content, the higher the negative impact on sleep quality/duration
discussion
gender differences
Brautsch et al (2023)
Digital media use and sleep in late adolescence and young adulthood: A systematic review
Some consistency across studies that call and text interactions, nighttime notifications or availability demands are associated w/immediate disturbed sleep among women
In contrast, in males, having more facebook friends and call/text interactions were associated w/ better sleep
Thus, smartphone use may have a positive impact on sleep among men - although authors argued that this could reflect a person’s sociability
obstructive sleep apnea
Carter (2014)
Common Sleep Disorders in Children
Characterized by upper airway obstruction, despite respiratory effort, that disrupts normal sleep patterns and ventilation
Can be associated with obesity, and biological pathology, however, in children, the obstruction is primarily due to enlarged tonsils and adenoids
Onset between 2 and 8 yrs, coinciding with peak tonsil growth, but can manifest at any age
Prevalence: 1-5%
Gender distribution: equal
Untreated OSA is associated with neurobehavioral problems, decreased attention, disturbed emotional regulation, decreased academic performance, impaired growth