Morphology Flashcards

1
Q

Morphemes

A

Smallest units of language that carry information about meaning or function

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Inflection

A

Modifies a word’s form to indicate grammatical subclass

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Derivation

A

Building a new word by adding an affix
- changes the word class and/or basic meaning of the word;

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Grammatical morphemes

A

Inflectional affixes & function words.
- Serve the same kind of role, but inflection is bound to its host, and function words are freestanding

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Components of inflectional morphology

A
  • number (singular vs. plural)
  • tense (past, future)
  • agreement (first, second, third person)
  • gender (masculine, feminine).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

To build a grammar of the structure of words, children must:

A

-Segment words into morphemes and assign meaning or function to each morpheme
- Learn the category types of morphemes (root vs. affix, prefix vs. suffix, derivation vs. inflection, lexical vs. functional, etc.);
- Learn the structure of morphologically complex words
- Assign a unique underlying representation to each morpheme;
* Recognize that morphemes may vary in their shape, depending on the context in which they appear (e.g. [s] in cats vs [z] in dogs)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Findings of Brown’s research on Adam, Eve and Sarah

A

Relatively uniform order of acquisition due to :
1. frequency (plural -s > possessive -s)
2. saliency (plural -s > past tense -ed) 3. semantic weight (plural -s > third person -s)
-ing and prepositions are acquired firrst

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Methodological issues for interpreting data

A
  1. Obligatory context : a sentence can be grammatical but incorrect given the context
    2.Cut-off : determining when a child has acquired a construct (Brown’s criterion: 90% use in obligatory contexts)
  2. Variation across sessions : Brown required that the rate of use remain above 90% for three consecutive sessions
  3. Variation across learners : children do not learn structures at same age; solution : MLU
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

True or false : there is a uniform order of grammatical morphemes acquistion across languages

A

False, morphology is more robust in certain languages

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

True or false : order of grammatical morphemes acquistion is relatively stable within a given language

A

True

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Productivity challenge to Brown’s experiment

A

Do children memorize the right context for inflected forms use or do they acquire productive rules and representations that they apply to new words ?
90% inflection can be attained with memorization
- You can incorrectly store keys as irregular (‘keys’ instead of ‘key’ + ‘s’ like ‘men’) , but produce it correctly

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Wugs test

A

Tests generalization of rules to novel forms
* If children can pluralize nouns they have never heard before, they must have a rule of plural formation (suffix -s to a noun to form its plural):

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Allomorphy challenge to Brown’s findings

A

In determination of “acquired”, how do we factor in different trajectories for different allomorphs?
* 4-year-old children can correctly pluralize forms that take the allomorph [s] (pifs) or [z] (wugs);
* But forms that take [əz] (tasses) are usually produced as uninflected (tass).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Phonological complexity challenge to Brown’s findings

A

Could phonological constraints lead children to delete inflectional morphology even when they have acquired target-like rules (and corresponding representations) for inflectionally complex words?
E.g. Children correctly produced plural for: vowel-final words 68%> stop-final words 51% (p = .02)
* Experiment 2: Children correctly produced plural for: son C-final 59% words > stop-final words 44% (p = .02)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Chunking

A

Employing memorized fixed forms

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Children usually acquire the inflectional system before __ years old

A

4

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

How can children acquire most of the inflectional system of their language (morphemes and rules) by age 4 ?

A

Universal Grammar perspective : children have a disposition for rule learning, knowledge of the kinds of semantic categories typically encoded by inflection, and the kinds of morphological processes that languages typically exhibit
Input perspective:
* Children can get everything they need for the acquisition of inflection solely from the input; because the task of segmenting inflection from the base to which it attaches and assigning an interpretation to an inflectional affix is relatively straightforward.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Attributes of inflection

A

No category change: Inflectional affixes never change the grammatical category of the base to which they attach;
Semantic compositionality: The grammatical distinctions encoded by inflection are relatively easy to assign a meaning to: the meaning of an inflectionally-complex word is the sum of the meaning of its parts;
Productivity: Inflectional affixes attach to virtually all instances of the particular category (e.g., plural to virtually all nouns).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

2 most common inflectional morphemes

A

Prefixes and suffixes

20
Q

Children find ______prefixes/suffixes easier

21
Q

Operating principle of Slobin

A

Idea that a rule tells children to be attentive to the end of words

22
Q

Children tend to omit obligatory ______prefixes/suffixes

23
Q

Why are infixes easy to acquire in Tagalog ?

A

The infix is sometimes realized as a prefix to optimize the syllable structure (phonology places constraints). The infix being realized as a prefix makes it easier for the child to identify the inflectional morpheme.
Consonant-initial roots: Agent-focus <um> is infix
Vowel-initial roots: Agent-focus <um> is prefix</um></um>

24
Q

Children acquiring more morphologically rich languages acquire inflection _____earlier/later than more empoverished languages like English

25
In morphologically rich languages, morphology does some of the job of ______
Syntax
26
Languages with richer morphology have relatively free _____ _______
Word order
27
Case marking
Indicates the role that a noun plays in a sentence (subject or object).
28
Case marking suffixes in Hungarian correspond to ... in English
Prepositions
29
Why do English children acquire locative prepositions later than Hungarian children acquire locative case marking ?
In Hungarian : 1. case markers appear to the right of the root as the final suffix in word: haːz-ban, haːz-at-ok-ban 2. Vowels in case markers harmonize with root-final vowel, which makes them cohere as a unit In English : 1. Prepositions are function words: a lot of different types of material can intervene between preposition and noun 2. No vowel harmony binds a preposition to a noun or other following word
30
Productivity in morphology
The morphological component of the language is not a catalogue of all the words that children have heard. * Instead, children acquire rules that can be productively applied to build new words.
31
Evidence for productivity
- Generalization of rules to novel forms (e.g., using Wug Test); - Types of errors that children make (i.e., morphological overregularization).
32
More segmental and syllable complexity _____reduces/increases plural production
Reduces
33
Morphological rule of past tense formation
V → V + /d/ past - Irregulars like ‘bring’ must be marked in the lexicon as not undergoing the rule of past tense formation; regulars like ‘play’ are not marked in any special way.
34
Errors of omission
Leaving out a morpheme
35
What do errors of omission indicate ?
2 options : 1. Lack of morphological knowledge 2. Uncertainty as to the context for use of the inflection
36
Errors of commission
Using the wrong form (e.g. "I walks")
37
What do errors of commission indicate ?
Lack of knowledge
38
Errors of ______ are more common
Omission
39
Errors of overregularization
Similar to errors of commission, but not due to lack of knowledge (e.g. mans for men)
40
Children make errors when a morphological rule is applied to ______
Exceptions
41
U-shaped development for irregular forms
Stage 1: No rule * Productions in past contexts: [pleɪd] ‘played’, [brɑt] ‘brought’. * Rule for past tense formation not yet acquired * Regular and irregular forms stored in lexicon as morphologically unanalysed chunks: /pleɪ/ (Vpresent) /pleɪd/ (Vpast) Stage 2: Rule acquired → overregularization: * Productions in past contexts: [pleɪd] ‘played’, [brɪŋd] ‘bringed’. * Since children never hear forms like [brɪŋd] in the input, they have acquired the rule for past tense formation : V → V + /d/past * Regular and irregular forms stored in same manner in lexicon: /pleɪ/ (V) /brɪŋ/ (V) Stage 3: Rule acquired and exceptions marked in lexicon: * Productions in past contexts: [pleɪd] ‘played’, [brɑt] ‘brought’. * Children recognize that there are exceptions to the rule of past tense formation; the behaviour of irregular verbs must be learned one at a time. * Rule for past tense formation acquired (as per Stage 2): V → V + /d/past Regular and irregular forms stored differently in lexicon: /pleɪ/ (V) /brɪŋ/ (V*) /brɑt/ (Vpast)
42
Analogy
type of associative learning that takes certain salient properties of one word and applies those properties to other words like it Example : "rat" for "wrote" or "brang" for "bringed" based on rhymes with "sat" and "sang"
43
Blocking approach
Once the child hears the irregular form in the input, the overregularized form is blocked and removed from the child's grammar
44
Competition approach
Children initially postulate the overregularized form and when they hear the irregular form for the first time, they entertain both options for a time. As the child hears the irregular and never the regular, the overregularization is eventually removed. - More input is needed Why common irregulars are acquired earlier
45
Syntactic fast mapping
One meaningful is enough for children to acquire some aspects of morphosyntax (even for rare morphemes)