Morays Flashcards

1
Q

Background

A

Colin Cherry created a dichotic listening task (2 messages play at once - one in each ear) and asked participants to shadow a message. He found that participants failed to notice details of the unattended message (even if language changed or message reversed)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Overall Aim

A

To replicate Cherry’s findings and provide evidence for the cocktail party effect in a more rigorous scientific way.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Apparatus used

A

A modified Brennell Mark IV tape recorder that allowed for a pair of headphones to play different outputs to each ear.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Experiment 1 sample

A

Undergraduate students and research workers of both genders at Oxford University

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Experiment 1 procedure

A

Participants had to shadow a passage of prose that they could hear in one ear (attended message)
In the other ear (unattended message) was a list of simple words that was repeated 35 times (rejected message)
At the end of the task participants were given a recognition task of 21 words (7 from shadowed, 7 from rejected, 7 from neither)
Participants had to identify words from the rejected passage.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Experiment 1 Results

A

Word list (1) Mean number of recognised words (2)
(1) 7 words from shadowed passage, 4.9 recognised
(1) 7 words from rejected passage, 1.9 recognised
(1) 7 similar words in neither passage, 2.6 recognised

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Experiment 1 Conclusions

A

Participants were much more able to recognise words from shadowed passage. Almost none of the words from the rejected message were able to break the innattentioanl barrier.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Experiment 2 Aim

A

To see if affective instructions (e.g. using names) will be strong enough to break the attentional barrier

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Experiment 2 Sample

A

12 research workers/undergrads from Oxford uni

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Experiment 2 IV

A

Whether an instruction in the rejected message was preceded by the participants name or not

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Experiment 2 DV

A

Whether participants were more likely to hear an instruction in a message they’re not paying attention to if it is preceded by their name. This was operationalised by whether they reported hearing the instruction or whether they actually followed the instruction.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Experiment 2 Procedure

A

Participants were given a dichotic listening task. Two passages of light fiction were heard, one in each ear
Both passages that the participants heard contained and instruction at the start and then another instruction within them.
All passages were read in a steady monotone pace of 130 words per minute by a single male voice.
Participants shadowed 10 passages of light fiction (all in the same order with a repeated measures design).
They were told to try to make as few mistakes as possible when shadowing the passages.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Which passages had the affective and non affective instructions in experiment 2?

A

Passages with affective instructions: 3,7 and 10
Passages with non-affective instructions: 1,5 and 8

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q
A
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Experiment 2 Results

A

No. times affective instructions presented - 39
No. times non-affective instructions presented - 36
No. times affective instructions were heard - 20
No. times non-affective instructions were heard - 4

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Experiment 2 Conclusions

A

People were far more likely to hear instructions that were affective (used name) than non-affective (didn’t use name)

17
Q

Experiment 3 Aim

A

To see if instructions given at the start (to listen to and remember digits) would make a difference and penetrate the block of the attentional message.

18
Q

Sample in experiment 3

A

28 participants 14 in each condition (independent measures design)

19
Q

Experiment 3 IV

A

If the group was asked to specifically remember the digits or just told they would be asked questions at the end of each message

20
Q

Experiment 3 DV

A

The number of digits correctly recalled

21
Q

Experiment 3 Procedure

A

2 groups of 14 participants given a dichotic listening task where they had to shadow 1 of the messages. Digits were stated at the end of the message (digits used as they’re non affective). 1 group of the participants were told they would be asked questions about the shadowed message (group 1) while the other group was told to listen out for digits to remember (group 2)

22
Q

Experiment 3 Results

A

No significant difference in number of digits recalled between group 1 (told they would be asked questions only end of passage) and group 2 (told to listen for digits to remember)

23
Q

Experiment 3 Conclusion

A

Non affective information like digits cannot be made important enough to break the inattention barrier